Skip to main content
Log in

Animated Bodies in Immunological Practices: Craftsmanship, Embodied Knowledge, Emotions and Attitudes Toward Animals

  • Emperical Study/Analysis
  • Published:
Human Studies Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Taking up the body turn in sociology, this paper discusses scientific practices as embodied action from the perspective of Husserl’s phenomenological theory of the “Body”. Based on ethnographic data on a biology laboratory it will discuss the importance of the scientist’s Body for the performance of scientific activities. Successful researchers have to be skilled workers using their embodied knowledge for the process of tinkering towards the material transformation of their objects for data production. The researcher’s body then is an instrument of measuring as well as a kind of archive of knowing. Their body becomes a disciplined instrument which has its own place and function inside the laboratory. Furthermore, the appresentational apperception of Bodies (Husserl) is being discussed as a basis for the emotional and ethical concerns towards laboratory-animals. Attitudes towards animals in the laboratory setting (as well as elsewhere) are highly emotional. Nevertheless, following the literature of the sociology of the body, those emotional reactions still follow certain cultural patterns which themselves can be understood as embodied ways of knowing “right” or “wrong”. Besides as an instrument, the scientist’s body can also be understood as a resource of emotional attachment towards animals. It is an instrument for performing transformation as well as one for caring.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The material presented in E-1 to E-9 are translated and slightly shortened excerpts from field-notes and interviews from an immunology-laboratory I was allowed to visit and observe between spring 2005 and 2008.

  2. For a critical discussion of Schutz theory of science from the perspective of STS see Lynch (1988b); comp. Bischur (2010).

  3. In using “body” for “Körper” and “Body” for “Leib” I follow the translation of Richard Rojcewicz and André Schuwer (Husserl 1989: XIV–XV): “German has two terms for what in English is designated by the single word, “body”. The distinction made in German is that between Körper, inanimate physical matter, and Leib, the animated flesh of an animal or human being. Both these terms would ordinarily be best rendered simply as “body” in English, the context determining the proper sense. But Husserl often plays on the distinction between these passages intelligible. We are proposing, then, to translate Leib as “Body” (with a capital) and Körper as “body,” and the same applies to the derivate words, “bodily,” “corporeal,” etc. Leibkörper thus becomes “Corporeal body”. This stratagem was chosen as the most simple and as having a precedent in the analogous (a limited analogy, of course) distinction between Objekt (“Object”) and Gegenstand (“object”). Just as, for Husserl, Gegenstand is the more general term, encompassing anything at all that can be intended in any way, a sensation, for example, and Objekte are only certain kinds of Gegenstände, intersubjective ones, so Körper is more general: i.e., every Objekt is an object, but not vice versa, and in the same manner every Body is a body”. Compare Dorion Cairns on “Leib” in his “Guide for Translating Husserl” (Cairns 1973: 79): “Leib (animate) organism (body). Not “living body”. So far as possible save “body” for “Körper””.

  4. For a discussion of Plessners theory of the living see List (2001) and Lindemann (2009a).

  5. One might suggest that some kind of “bracketing” the natural attitude towards animals might as well apply to other professions concerned with the killing of animals.

  6. Although this aspect of animal experimentation may rise interesting questions concerning the boarders of the social (Luckmann 1970; Lindemann 2009b) and the conception of a philosophical anthropology, this topic would go beyond the scope of this essay.

  7. For an overview on the general principles, problems, and regulations of the welfare of laboratory animals, see the contributions in Kaliste (2004).

References

  • Abraham, A. (2006). Der Körper als Speicher von Erfahrung. Anmerkungen zu übersehenen Tiefendimensionen von Leiblichkeit und Identität. In R. Gugutzer (Ed.), Body turn: Perspektiven der Soziologie des Körpers und des Sports (pp. 119–139). Bielefeld: Transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acampora, R. R. (2006). Corporal compassion: Animal ethics and philosophy of body. Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amann, K. (1994). Menschen, Mäuse und Fliegen. Eine wissenschaftssoziologische Analyse der Transformation von Organismen in epistemische Objekte. Zeitschrift für Soziologie, 23(1), 22–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arluke, A. (1994). ‘We build a better beagle’: Fantastic creatures in lab animal ads. Qualitative Sociology, 17(2), 143–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behnke, E. A. (1997). Ghost gestures: Phenomenological investigations of bodily micromovements and their intercorporal implications. Human Studies, 20, 181–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birke, L. (2003). Who—or what—are the rats (and mice) in the laboratory. Society and Animals, 11(3), 207–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Birke, L., Arluke, A., & Micheal, M. (2007). The sacrifice: How scientific experiments transform animals and people. West Lafayette: Purdue University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bischur, D. (2003). Toleranz. Im Wechselspiel von Identität und Integration. Wien: Passagen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bischur, D. (2010). Wissenschaftliche Praxis und die Welt des Wirkens. In M. Staudigl (Ed.), Alfred Schütz und die Hermeneutik (pp. 253–281). Konstanz: UVK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruun, H., & Langlais, R. (2003). On the embodied nature of action. Acta Sociologica, 46(1), 31–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burian, R. M. (1993). How the choice of experimental organism matters: Epistemological reflections on an aspect of biological practice. Journal of the History of Biology, 26(2), 351–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cairns, D. (1973). Guide for translating Husserl. The Hague: Nijhoff.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, A. E., & Fujimura, J. H. (1992). What tools? Which jobs? Why right? In A. E. Clarke & J. H. Fujimura (Eds.), The right tools for the job: At work in twentieth-century life sciences (pp. 3–44). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clause, B. T. (1993). The Wistar rat as a right choice: Establishing mammalian standards and the ideal of a standardized mammal. Journal of the History of Biology, 16(2), 329–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, H. M. (1992). Changing order: Replication and induction in scientific practice. Chicago, London: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston, L. (2000). Introduction: The coming into being of scientific objects. In L. Daston (Ed.), Biographies of scientific objects (pp. 1–14). Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1997). Discours de la méthode pour bien conduire sa raison, et chercher la verité dans les scinces. Hamburg: Meiner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fleck, L. (1983). Schauen, sehen, wissen. In L. Fleck (Ed.), Erfahrung und Tatsache: Gesammelte Aufsätze (pp. 147–174). Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gugutzer, R. (2004). Soziologie des Körpers. Bielefeld: Transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gugutzer, R. (2006). Der body turn in der Soziologie: Eine programmatische Einführung. In R. Gugutzer (Ed.), Body turn: Perspektiven der Soziologie des Körpers und des Sports (pp. 9–53). Bielefeld: Transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gusterson, H. (1998). Nuclear rites: A weapons laboratory at the end of the cold war. Berkeley, Los Angeles, London: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, F. L. (1993). The old martyr of science: The frog in experimental physiology. Journal of the History of Biology, 26(2), 311–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1954). Die Krisis der europäischen Wissenschaften und die transzendentale Phänomenologie: Eine Einleitung in die phänomenologische Philosophie (Husserliana VI). The Hague: Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1982). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. First book: General introduction to a pure phenomenology (Collected Works—Vol. II). The Hague, Boston, London: Nijhoff.

  • Husserl, E. (1989). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. Second book: Studies in the phenomenology of constitution (Collected Works—Vol. III). Dordrecht, Boston, London: Kluwer.

  • Husserl, E. (1995). Cartesianische Meditationen: Eine Einleitung in die Phänomenologie (3rd ed.). Hamburg: Meiner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1999). Erfahrung und Urteil: Untersuchungen zur Genealogie der Logik (7th ed.). Hamburg: Meiner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaliste, E. (Ed.). (2004). The welfare of laboratory animals. Dordrecht, Boston, London: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, P., Cambrosio, A., & Mackenzie, M. (1992). The tools of the discipline: Standards, models, and measures in the affinity/avidity controversy in immunology. In A. E. Clarke & J. H. Fujimura (Eds.), The right tools for the job: At work in twentieth-century life sciences (pp. 312–354). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr Cetina, K. (1988). Das naturwissenschaftliche Labor als Ort der ‘Verdichtung’ von Gesellschaft. Zeitschrift für Soziologie, 17(2), 85–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knorr Cetina, K. (1997). Sociality with objects: Social relations in postsocial knowledge societies. Theory, Culture & Society, 14(4), 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, R. E. (1993). Drosophila: A life in the laboratory. Journal of the History of Biology, 26(2), 281–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, R. E. (1994). Lords of the fly: Drosophila genetics and the experimental life. Chicago, London: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kutschmann, W. (1986). Der Naturwissenschaftler und sein Körper: Die Rolle der ‘inneren Natur’ in der experimentellen Naturwissenschaft der frühen Neuzeit. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (2002). Die Hoffnung der Pandora. Untersuchungen zur Wirklichkeit der Wissenschaft. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B., & Woolgar, S. (1986). Laboratory life: The construction of scientific facts (2nd ed.). Princton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindemann, G. (2009a). From experimental interaction to the brain as the epistemic object of neurobiology. Human Studies, 32, 153–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindemann, G. (2009b). Das Soziale von seinen Grenzen her denken. Weilerswist: Velbrück.

    Google Scholar 

  • List, E. (2001). Grenzen der Verfügbarkeit: Die Technik, das Subjekt und das Lebendige. Wien: Passagen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, C. A. (2001). ‘[A]re Norway rats … things?’ Diversity versus generality in the use of albino rats in experiments on development and sexuality. Journal of the History of Biology, 34, 287–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luckmann, T. (1970). Über die Grenzen der Sozialwelt. In T. Luckmann (2007), Lebenswelt, Identität und Gesellschaft. Schriften zur Wissens-und Protosoziologie (pp. 62–90). Konstanz: UVK.

  • Lynch, M. (1985). Art and artefact in laboratory science: A study of shop work and shop talk in a research laboratory. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (1988a). Sacrifice and the transformation of the animal body into a scientific object: Laboratory culture and ritual practice in the neurosciences. Social Studies of Science, 18, 265–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (1988b). Alfred Schutz and the sociology of science. In L. Embree (Ed.), Wordly phenomenology: The continuing influence of Alfred Schutz on North American human science (pp. 71–100). Lanham/MD, London: University Press of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M., Livingston, E., & Garfinkel, H. (1983). Temporal order in laboratory work. In K. Knorr Cetina & M. Mulkay (Eds.), Science observed: Perspectives on the social study of science (pp. 205–238). London, Beverly Hills, New Delhi: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maehle, A., & Tröhler, U. (1987). Animal experimentation from antiquity to the end of the eighteenth century: Attitudes and arguments. In N. A. Rupke (Ed.), Vivisection in historical perspective (pp. 14–47). London, New York, Sydney: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London, New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulcahy, D. (1999). (actor-net) Working bodies and representations: Tales from a training field. Science, Technology and Human Values, 24(1), 80–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peck Gossel, P. (1992). A need for standard methods: The case of American bacteriology. In A. E. Clarke & J. H. Fujimura (Eds.), The right tool for the job: At work in twentieth-century life sciences (pp. 287–311). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, A. (1995). The mangle of practice: Time, agency and science. Chicago, London: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plessner, H. (1975). Die Stufen des Organischen und der Mensch (3rd ed.). Berlin, New York: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1985). Implizites Wissen. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rader, K. (1998). The mouse people: Murine genetics work at the Bussey institution, 1909–1936. Journal of the History of Biology, 31(2), 327–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rader, K. (2004). Making mice: Standardizing animals for American biomedical research, 1900–1955. Princeton, Oxford: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberger, H. J. (2000). Cytoplastmic particles: The trajectory of a scientific object. In L. Daston (Ed.), Biographies of scientific objects (pp. 270–294). Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rheinberger, H.-J. (2002). Experimentalsysteme und epistemische Dinge: Eine Geschichte der Proteinbiosynthese im Reagenzglas. Göttingen: Wallstein.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, R. (2006). ‘Geistige Arbeit’ als körperlicher Vollzug: Zur Perspektive einer vom Sport ausgehenden praxeologischen Sozialanalyse. In R. Gugutzer (Ed.), Body turn: Perspektiven der Soziologie des Körpers und des Sports (pp. 297–319). Bielefeld: Transcript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (2004). Der sinnhafte Aufbau der sozialen Welt: Eine Einleitung in die verstehende Soziologie (ASW II). Konstanz: UVK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (2010). Parsons’ Theorie des sozialen Handelns. In A. Schutz (Ed.), Zur Methodologie der Sozialwissenschaften (ASW IV) (pp. 241–312). Konstanz: UVK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shilling, C. (2005). The body in culture, technology and society. London, Thousand Oaks, New Delhi: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Star, S. L. (1992). Craft versus commodity, mess versus transcendence: How the right tool became the wrong one in the case of taxidermy and natural history. In A. E. Clarke & J. H. Fujimura (Eds.), The right tools fort he job: At work in twentieth-century life sciences (pp. 257–286). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldenfels, B. (2000). Das leibliche Selbst: Vorlesungen zur Phänomenologie des Leibes. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waldenfels, B. (2006). Schattenrisse der Moral. Frankfurt/M.: Suhrkamp.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wieder, D. L. (1980). Behavioristic operationalism and the life-world: Chimpanzees and chimpanzee researchers in face-to-face interaction. Sociological Inquiry, 50, 73–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This article is based on a paper held at the 4S-Annual Meeting 2007 in Montreal and is part of a larger research project “Social Frames of Ethics in the Scientific Practices of Life-Sciences” (APART 11084), financed by the Austrian Academy of Science (ÖAW) (2005-2008). My thanks go to the observed immunology research group.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Daniel Bischur.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bischur, D. Animated Bodies in Immunological Practices: Craftsmanship, Embodied Knowledge, Emotions and Attitudes Toward Animals. Hum Stud 34, 407–429 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10746-011-9205-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10746-011-9205-x

Keywords

Navigation