Event Abstract

Dynamic information about letter production influences visual letter perception:
Evidence from an acquired letter recognition deficit

  • 1 Johns Hopkins University, Cognitive Science, United States
  • 2 Macquarie University, Cognitive Science, Australia

Letter recognition is traditionally assumed to involve matching a perceptual representation of a stimulus letter with a stored representation of a static letter shape. However, people learn not only to recognize letters, but also to write them, and several researchers have suggested that knowledge concerning the dynamics of letter production (e.g., knowledge about the direction and sequencing of writing strokes) may play a role in letter recognition (James & Atwood, 2009; Longcamp, Lagarrigue, & Velay, 2010; Parkinson & Khurana, 2007). We report results from NGN, a 77-year-old man who suffered a large left ventro-medial lesion as a consequence of stroke. NGN is severely impaired in visual letter identification. His perception of letter shapes is intact, but his ability to map these shapes onto abstract letter representations is severely disrupted. We investigated whether dynamic presentation of letters might improve NGN’s letter identification. Upper-case letters were presented individually in dynamic or static conditions (see Figure 1). In the Dynamic-Forward condition a moving dot drew a path from the beginning to the end of the letter as it is typically written, until the entire letter was displayed. In the Static condition the whole letter was displayed for the full duration required to “write” the letter in the dynamic condition. NGN’s letter identification accuracy was significantly higher in the Dynamic-Forward condition (90%) than in the Static condition (73%), p < .001. Two additional dynamic conditions established that the advantage of the Dynamic-Forward condition was not due to the mere presence of motion in this condition (a possibility suggested by the findings of Rauschecker et al., 2011), or to the fact that the stimulus shape evolved over time. In the Dynamic-Backward condition a moving dot “wrote” the letter from end to beginning; and in the Dynamic-Random condition the dots making up the letter shape appeared in random order at the same rate as in the other dynamic conditions, until the entire letter was displayed. In both of these conditions, NGN’s performance was significantly worse than in the Dynamic-Forward condition (ps < .01), and no better than his performance in the Static condition: Accuracy was 80% in the Dynamic-Backward condition and 74% in the Dynamic-Random condition. These results demonstrate that mere motion, or mere change over time, are not sufficient to facilitate NGN’s letter identification; only the typical writing motion is helpful. Our findings are consistent with hypotheses holding that knowledge of writing motions contributes to letter identification, and also suggest that dynamic letter displays may be worth exploring for purposes of rehabilitation. Our results also raise an interesting point about rehabilitation strategies in which individuals with letter identification deficits are asked to trace the shapes of visually-presented letters on their hands, in the air, or on a surface. Facilitatory effects of the tracing strategies on letter identification performance have typically been attributed to the tactile or kinesthetic representations activated by tracing (e.g., Starrfelt, Olafsdóttir, & Arendt, 2013). Our results suggest that the dynamic visual information provided by the tracing motions may also play an important role.

Figure 1

References

James, K. H., & Atwood, T. P. (2009). The role of sensorimotor learning in the perception of letter-like forms: tracking the causes of neural specialization for letters. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 26(1), 91–110. http://doi.org/10.1080/02643290802425914

Longcamp, M., Lagarrigue, a., & Velay, J. L. (2010). Contribution de la motricité graphique à la reconnaissance visuelle des lettres. Psychologie Francaise, 55(2), 181–194. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.psfr.2010.03.001

Parkinson, J., & Khurana, B. (2007). Temporal order of strokes primes letter recognition. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 60(9), 1265–1274. http://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600937460

Rauschecker, A. M., Bowen, R. F., Perry, L. M., Kevan, A. M., Dougherty, R. F., & Wandell, B. a. (2011). Visual feature-tolerance in the reading network. Neuron, 71(5), 941–953. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.06.036

Starrfelt, R., Olafsdóttir, R. R., & Arendt, I.-M. (2013). Rehabilitation of pure alexia: a review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 23(5), 755–79. http://doi.org/10.1080/09602011.2013.809661

Keywords: Dyslexia, reading, letter identification, Rehabilitation, Writing

Conference: Academy of Aphasia 53rd Annual Meeting, Tucson, United States, 18 Oct - 20 Oct, 2015.

Presentation Type: Poster

Topic: Not student first author

Citation: Reilhac C, Schubert T and McCloskey M (2015). Dynamic information about letter production influences visual letter perception:
Evidence from an acquired letter recognition deficit. Front. Psychol. Conference Abstract: Academy of Aphasia 53rd Annual Meeting. doi: 10.3389/conf.fpsyg.2015.65.00009

Copyright: The abstracts in this collection have not been subject to any Frontiers peer review or checks, and are not endorsed by Frontiers. They are made available through the Frontiers publishing platform as a service to conference organizers and presenters.

The copyright in the individual abstracts is owned by the author of each abstract or his/her employer unless otherwise stated.

Each abstract, as well as the collection of abstracts, are published under a Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 (attribution) licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/) and may thus be reproduced, translated, adapted and be the subject of derivative works provided the authors and Frontiers are attributed.

For Frontiers’ terms and conditions please see https://www.frontiersin.org/legal/terms-and-conditions.

Received: 01 May 2015; Published Online: 24 Sep 2015.

* Correspondence: PhD. Caroline Reilhac, Johns Hopkins University, Cognitive Science, Baltimore, United States, caroline.reilhac@gmail.com