Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Language co-evolved with the rule of law

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Mind & Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many scholars assume a connection between the evolution of language and that of distinctively human group-level morality. Unfortunately, such thinkers frequently downplay a central implication of modern Darwinian theory, which precludes the possibility of innate psychological mechanisms evolving to benefit the group at the expense of the individual. Group level moral regulation is indeed central to public life in all known human communities. The production of speech acts would be impossible without this. The challenge, therefore, is to explain on a Darwinian basis how life could have become subject to the rule of law. Only then will we have an appropriate social framework in which to contextualize our models of how language may have evolved.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aiello LC, Dunbar RIM (1993) Neocortex size, group size, and the evolution of language. Curr Anthropol 34:184–193

    Google Scholar 

  • Aiello LC, Wheeler P (1995) The expensive tissue hypothesis: the brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol 36:199–221

    Google Scholar 

  • Alcorta CS, Sosis R (2005) Ritual, emotion, and sacred symbols: the evolution of religion as an adaptive complex. Hum Nat 16(4):323–359

    Google Scholar 

  • Atran S (2002) In gods we trust: the evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Austin JL (1978 [1955]) How to do things with words. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1986) An evolutionary approach to social norms. Am Polit Sci Rev 80:1095–1111

    Google Scholar 

  • Boehm C (2001) Hierarchy in the forest: the evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Botha R (2003) Unravelling the evolution of language. Elsevier, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu P (1991) Language and symbolic power. Polity Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowles S (2006) Group competition, reproductive levelling, and the evolution of human altruism. Science 314:1569–1572

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1992) Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizeable groups. Ethol Sociobiol 13:171–195

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyer P (2001) Religion explained. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Brody H (2000) The other side of Eden: hunters, farmers and the shaping of the world. North Point Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan SF, de Waal FBM (2003) Monkeys reject unequal pay. Nature (Ldn) 143:297–299

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown P, Levinson SC (1987 [1978]) Politeness: some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Burling R (2005) The talking ape: how language evolved. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Chase P (1999) Symbolism as reference and symbolism as culture. In: Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 34–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1965) Aspects of the theory of syntax. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1988) Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky N (1995) The minimalist programme. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowlishaw G, Dunbar RIM (1991) Dominance rank and mating success in male primates. Anim Behav 41:1045–1056

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins R (2006 [1976]) The selfish gene, 30th anniversary edn. Oxford University Press, Oxford

  • Dawkins R, Krebs JR (1978) Animal signals: information or manipulation? In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, pp 282–309

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal FBM (1996) Good natured: the origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Dessalles JL (1998) Altruism, status and the origin of relevance. In: Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 130–47

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1988) Primate social systems: studies in behavioural adaptation. Croom Helm, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1999) Culture, honesty and the freerider problem. In: Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture: an interdisciplinary view. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 194–213

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkheim E (1963 [1898]) La prohibition de l’inceste et ses origins. Année Sociol 1:1–70. (Reprinted as Incest: the nature and origin of the taboo, trans E Sagarin. Stuart, New York)

  • Durkheim E (1965 [1912]) The elementary forms of the religious life. Free Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis L (1995) Dominance and reproductive success among nonhuman animals: a cross-species comparison. Ethol Sociobiol 16:257–333

    Google Scholar 

  • Engels F (1972 [1884]) The origin of the family, private property and the state. Pathfinder Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Enquist M, Leimar O (1993) The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Anim Behav 45:747–757

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdal D, Whiten A (1994) On human egalitarianism: an evolutionary product of Machiavellian status escalation? Curr Anthropol 35(2):175–183

    Google Scholar 

  • Erdal D, Whiten A (1996) Egalitarianism and Machiavellian intelligence in human evolution. In: Mellars P, Gibson K (eds) Modelling the early human mind. McDonald Institute Monographs, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Flack J, Girvan M, de Waal FBM, Krakauer DC (2006) Policing stabilizes construction of social niches in primates. Nature (Ldn) 439:426–429

    Google Scholar 

  • Foley RA, Lee PC (1991) Ecology and energetics of encephalization in hominid evolution. Phil Trans R Soc Ldn 334:223–232

    Google Scholar 

  • Fouts RS (1975) Capacities for language in great apes. In: Tuttle RH (ed) Socioecology and psychology of primates. Mouton, The Hague, pp 371–390

    Google Scholar 

  • Fouts RS (1997) Next of kin: my conversations with chimpanzees. Harper Collins, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Furuichi T (1989) Social interactions and the life history of female Pan paniscus in Wamba, Zaire. Int J Primatol 10(3):173–197

    Google Scholar 

  • Gellner E (1988) Origins of society. In: Fabian AC (ed) Origins: the Darwin college lectures. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2000) Strong reciprocity and human sociality. J Theor Biol 206:169–179

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Alden Smith E, Bowles S (2001) Costly signalling and cooperation. J Theor Biol 213:103–119

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Bowles S, Boyd R, Fehr E (2003) Explaining altruistic behaviour in humans. Evol Hum Behav 24:153–172

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen A (1990a) Sexual selection unhandicapped by the Fisher process. J Theor Biol 144:473–516

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen A (1990b) Biological signals as handicaps. J Theor Biol 144:517–546

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice HP (1989) Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gürerk Ö, Irlenbusch B, Rockenbach B (2006) The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science 312:108–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour I, II. J Theor Biol 7:1–52

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris R (1988) Language, Saussure and Wittgenstein: how to play games with words. Routledge, London/New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD (1992) Costs of deception: cheaters are punished in rhesus monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 82:12137–12139

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K (1990a) Showing off: tests of an hypothesis about men’s foraging goals. Ethol Sociobiol 12:29–54

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K (1990b) Why do men hunt? Some benefits for risky strategies. In: Cashdan E (ed) Risk and uncertainty. Westview Press, Boulder Colarado, pp 145–166

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K (1993) Why hunter–gatherers work: an ancient version of the problem of public goods. Curr Anthropol 34:341–361

    Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K (1996) Foraging differences between men and women: behavioural ecology of the sexual division of labour. In: Steele J, Shennan S (eds) The archaeology of human ancestry: power, sex and tradition. Routledge, London, pp 283–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R (2001) Why people punish defectors: weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. J Theor Biol 208:79–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Gil-White FJ (2001) The evolution of prestige: freely conferred deference as a mechanism for enhancing the benefits of cultural transmission. Evol Hum Behav 22:165–196

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas J, MGurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer D, Ziker J (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312:1768–1770

    Google Scholar 

  • Irons W (2001) Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In: Nesse R (ed) Evolution and the capacity for commitment. Russell Sage, New York, pp 292–309

    Google Scholar 

  • Key CA, Aiello LC (1999) The evolution of social organisation. In: Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 15–33

    Google Scholar 

  • Key C, Aiello LC (2000) A prisoner’s dilemma model of the evolution of paternal care. Folia Primatol 71:77–92

    Google Scholar 

  • Knauft BB (1991) Violence and sociality in human evolution. Curr Anthropol 32:391–428

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight C (1991) Blood relations: menstruation and the origins of culture. Yale University Press, New Haven/London

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight C (2000) Play as precursor of phonology and syntax. In: Knight CD, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford JR (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 99–119

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight C, Power C (2005) Grandmothers, politics, and getting back to science. In: Voland E, Chasiotis A, Schienfenhövel W (eds) Grandmotherhood: the evolutionary significance of the second half of female life. Rutgers, New Brunswick, pp 81–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight C, Power C, Watts I (1995) The human symbolic revolution: a Darwinian account. Camb Archaeol J 5:75–114

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature (Ldn) 435:673–676

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs JR, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: mind-reading and manipulation. In: Krebs JR, Davies NB (eds) Behavioural ecology: an evolutionary approach. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, pp 380–402

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee RB (1988) Reflections on primitive communism. In: Ingold T, Riches D, Woodburn J (eds) Hunters and gatherers, vol. I: history, evolution and social change. Aldine, Chicago, pp 252–268

    Google Scholar 

  • Leech G (1980) Language and tact. In: Explorations in semantics and pragmatics: pragmatics and beyond. John Benjamins, Amsterdam, pp 79–117

  • Lévi-Strauss C (1969) The elementary structures of kinship. Eyre and Spottiswoode, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin RD (1990) Primate origins and evolution. Chapman and Hall, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith J, Szathmáry E (1995) The major transitions in evolution. WH Freeman, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Mellars PA, Stringer C (1989) The human revolution: behavioural and biological perspective in the origins of modern humans. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan LH (1871) Systems of consanguinity and affinity of the human family. Smithsonian Institution, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Newmeyer FJ (2003) Review article. Language 79(3):583–599

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA, Sigmund K (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature (Ldn) 437:1291–1298

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell JF, Hawkes K, Blurton Jones N (1999) Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. J Hum Evol 36:461–485

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell JF, Hawkes K, Lupo KD, Blurton Jones NG (2002) Male strategies and Plio-Pleistocene archaeology. J Hum Evol 43:831–872

    Google Scholar 

  • Parish A, de Waal FBM (2000) The other ‘closest living relative’: how Bonobos (Pan paniscus) challenge traditional assumptions about females, dominance, intra- and intersexual interactions, and hominid evolution. Ann N Y Acad Sci 907:97–113

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepperberg I (2005) An avian perspective on language evolution: implications of simultaneous development of vocal and physical object combinations by a Grey parrot (Psittacus erithacus). In: Tallerman M (ed) Language origins: perspectives on evolution. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 239–261

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S, Jackendoff R (2005) The faculty of language: what’s special about it? Cognition 95:201–236

    Google Scholar 

  • Power C (2000) Secret language use at female initiation: bounding gossiping communities. In: Knight C, Studdert-Kennedy M, Hurford JR (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language: social function and the origins of linguistic form. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 81–98

    Google Scholar 

  • Power C, Aiello LC (1997) Female proto-symbolic strategies. In: Hager LD (ed) Women in human evolution. Routledge, New York/London, pp 153–171

    Google Scholar 

  • Power C, Watts I (1997) The woman with the zebra’s penis: gender, mutability and performance. J R Anthrop Inst 3(NS):537–560

    Google Scholar 

  • Price M, Cosmides L, Tooby J (2002) Punitive sentiment as an anti-free rider psychological device. Evol Hum Behav 23:203–231

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport RA (1999) Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Rousseau JJ (1973 [1762]) The social contract. In: Rousseau JJ (ed) The social contract and discourses, trans GDH Cole. Dent, London/Melbourne, pp 179–309

  • Runciman WG (2001) From nature to culture, from culture to society. In: Runciman WG (ed) The origin of social institutions. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 235–254

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins MD (1960) The origin of society. Sci Am 203(3):76–87

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure F (1974 [1915]) Course in general linguistics, trans W Baskin. Fontana/Collins, London

  • Savage-Rumbaugh S, Shanker G, Taylor TJ (1998) Apes, language and the human mind. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Searcy WA, Nowicki S (2005) The evolution of animal communication. Princeton University Press, Princeton/Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle JR (1971) The philosophy of language. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle JR (1996) The construction of social reality. Penguin, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB, Brosnan SF, Vonk J, Henrich J, Povinelli DJ, Richardson AS, Lambeth SP, Mascaro J, Schapiro SJ (2005) Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature (Ldn) 437:1357–1359

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (1986) Relevance: communication and cognition. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Stompka P (1999) Trust. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Call J, Nagell K, Olguin R, Carpenter M (1994) The learning and use of gestural signals by young chimpanzees: a trans-generational study. Primates 35:137–154

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby J, DeVore I (1987) The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modelling. In: Kinzey WG (ed) The evolution of human behavior: primate models. State University of New York Press, Albany, pp 183–237

    Google Scholar 

  • Trager GL (1972) Language and languages. Chandler, San Francisco

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Q Rev Biol 46:35–57

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1974) Parent-offspring conflict. Am Zool 14:249–264

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulbaek I (1998) The origin of language and cognition. In: Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C (eds) Approaches to the evolution of language. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 30–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Gennep A (1960 [1909]) The rites of passage. Routledge & Kegan Paul, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts I (1999) The origin of symbolic culture. In: Dunbar R, Knight C, Power C (eds) The evolution of culture. Edinburgh University Press, Edinburgh, pp 113–146

    Google Scholar 

  • Watts I (2005) ‘Time, too, grows on the Moon’: some evidence for Knight’s theory of a human universal. In: James W, Mills D (eds) The qualities of time: anthropological approaches. Berg, New York, pp 95–118

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and natural selection: a critique of some current evolutionary thought. Princeton University Press, Princetone

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein L (1968) Philosophical investigations, trans Anscombe GEM. Blackwell, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A (1993) The fallacy of conventional signalling. Phil Trans R Soc Ldn 340:227–230

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A (2003) Indirect selection and individual selection in sociobiology: my personal views on theories of social behaviour. Anim Behav 65:859–863

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi A, Zahavi A (1997) The handicap principle: a missing piece in Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University Press, Oxford/New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Zak PJ, Knack S (2001) Trust and growth. Econ J 111:295–321

    Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman S (1932) The social life of monkeys and apes. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner, London

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chris Knight.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Knight, C. Language co-evolved with the rule of law. Mind & Society 7, 109–128 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-007-0039-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-007-0039-1

Keywords

Navigation