Skip to main content
Log in

Communication as Navigation: A New Role for Consciousness in Language

  • Published:
Topoi Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Classical cognitive science has been characterized by an association with the computational theory of mind. Although this association has produced highly significant results, it has also limited the scope of scientific psychology. In this paper, we analyse the limits of the specific kind of computational model represented by the Chomskian-Fodorian tradition in the study of mind and language. In our opinion, the adhesion to the principle of formality imposed by this specific computational model has motivated the exclusion of consciousness in the reflection on language and, consequently, has led to the inability to account for some aspects of language functioning at the processing level of discourse. The aim of this article is to restore the role of consciousness in discourse comprehension and production processes. Specifically, we argue that the ability to produce and understand discourses is based on individuals’ capacity for navigation in space and time. We will show that the space–time orientation is guaranteed by the projection of the self, which involves a special kind of consciousness.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We thank an anonymous reviewer for this comment.

References

  • Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2007) Remembering the past and imagining the future: common and distinct neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. Neuropsychologia 45:1363–1377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Addis DR, Wong AT, Schacter DL (2008) Age-related changes in the episodic simulation of future events. Psychol Sci 19:33–41

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aguirre GK, D’Esposito M (1999) Topographical disorientation: a synthesis and taxonomy. Brain 122:1613–1628

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreasen, NC (1984). Scale for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS). University of lowa, lowa city

  • Andreasen NC, Hoffman RE, Grove WM (1985) Mapping abnormalitiesin language and cognition. In: Alpert A (ed) Controversies in schizophrenia: Changes and constancies. Guilford Press, New York, pp 199–226

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou LW (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behav Brain Sci 22:577–609

    Google Scholar 

  • Botzung A, Denkova E, Manning L (2008) Experiencing past and future personal events: functional neuroimaging evidence on the neural bases of mental time travel. Brain Cogn 66:202–212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyer P (2008) Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends Cogn Sci 12(6):219–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner RL, Carroll DC (2007) Self-projection and the brain. Trends Cogn Sci 11:49–57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ciaramelli E, Rosenbaum RS, Solcz S, Levine B, Moscovitch M (2010) Mental space travel: damage to posterior parietal cortex prevents egocentric navigation and reexperiencing of remote spatial memories. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 36(3):619–634

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corballis M (2002) From hand to mouth: the origins of language. PrincetonUniversity Press, Princeton

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosentino, E (2008). Il tempo della mente. Linguaggio, evoluzione e identità personale. Macerata: Quodlibet

  • Cosentino E (2011) Self in time and language. Conscious Cogn 20(3):777–783

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Argembeau A, Raffard S, Van der Linden M (2008) Remembering the past and imagining the future in schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol 117:247–251

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio A (1999) The feeling of what happens: body, emotion and the making of consciousness. Heinemann, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis G, O’Neil-Pirozzi T, Coon M (1997) Referential cohesion and logical coherence of narration after right hemisphere stroke. Brain Lang 56:183–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denis M, Pazzaglia F, Cornoldi C, Bertolo L (1999) Spatial discourse and navigation: an analysis of route in the city of Venice. Appl Cogn Psychol 13:145–174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1991) Consciousness explained. Little, Brown and Company, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (2000) Making tools for thinking. In: Sperber D (ed) Metarepresentations. A multidisciplinary perspective. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 17–29

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferretti F (2010) Alle origini del linguaggio umano. Il punto di vista evoluzionistico. Laterza, Roma-Bari

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferretti, F (in press). Navigation, Discourse and the Origin of Language. In: A. Marini, Brambilla P. (Eds.), Brain evolution, language, and psychopathology in schizophrenia, Psychology Press

  • Ferretti F, Cosentino E (2013) Time, language and flexibility of the mind. The role of mental time travel in linguistic comprehension and production. Philos Psychol 26(1):24–46

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferretti F, Adornetti I, Cosentino E, Marini A (2013) Keeping the route and speaking coherently: the hidden link between spatial navigation and discourse processing. J Neurolinguistics 26(2):327–334

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer MH, Zwaan RA (2008) Embodied language: a review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. Q J Exp Psychol 61(6):825–850

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1980) Methodological solipsism considered as a research strategy in cognitive Psychology. Behav Brain Sci 3:63–109

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor JA (1983) The modularity of mind. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith C (1992) The cognitive neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hove

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher S (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci 4(1):14–21

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese V, Lakoff G (2005) The brain’s concepts: the role of the sensorymotor system in conceptual knowledge. Cogn Neuropsychol 22(3–4):455–479

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallistel CR (1990) The organization of learning. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbs RW Jr (2006) Embodiment and cognitive science. Cambridge Univ. Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. & the ABC Research Group (1999) Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Giora R (1985) Notes towards a theory of text coherence. Poetics Today 6(4):699–715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg AM (2010) Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdiscip Rev: Cogn Sci 1:586–596

    Google Scholar 

  • Grant JB, Suddendorf T (2009) Preschoolers begin to differentiate the times of events from throughout the lifespan. Eur J Dev Psychol 6:746–762

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassabis D, Maguire EA (2007) Deconstructing episodic memory with construction. Trends Cogn Sci 11(7):299–306

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Maguire EA (2007a) Using imagination to understand the neural basis of episodic memory. J Neurosci 27:14365–14374

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hassabis D, Kumaran D, Vann SD, Maguire EA (2007b) Patients with hippocampal amnesia cannot imagine new experiences. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 104:1726–1731

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kintsch W, e Van Dijk TA (1978) Toward a model of text comprehension and production. Psychol Rev 85(5):363–394

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein SB, Loftus J, Kihlstrom JF (2002) Memory and temporal experience: the effects of episodic memory loss on an amnesiac patient’s ability to remember the past and imagine the future. Soc Cogn 20:353–379

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff G, Johnson M (1980) Metaphors we live by. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakusta L, Dessalegn B, Landau B (2010) Impaired geometric reorientation caused by genetic defect. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 16, 107(7):2813–2817

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau B (2002) Spatial cognition. In: Ramachandran V (ed) Encyclopedia of the human brain, vol IV. Academic Press, San Diego, pp 395–418

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Landau B, Zukowsky A (2003) Objects, motions, and paths: spatial language in children with Williams syndrome. Develop Neuropsychol 23:105–137

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maddalena E (2009) Orienteering. Hoepli, Milano

    Google Scholar 

  • Marini A, Spoletini I, Rubino IA, Ciuffa M, Banfi G, Siracusano A, Bria P, Caltagirone C, Spalletta G (2008) The Language of schizophrenia: analysis of micro- and macrolinguistic abilities and their neuropsychological correlates. Schizophr Res 105:144–155

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marini A, Martelli S, Gagliardi C, Fabbro F, Borgatti R (2010) Narrative language in Williams’ Syndrome and its neuropsychological correlates. J Neurolinguistics 23(2):97–111

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles LK, Karpinska K, Lumsden J, Macrae CN (2010a) The meandering mind: vection and mental time travel. PLoS ONE 5(5):e10825

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miles LK, Nind LK, Macrae NC (2010b) Moving through time. Psychol Sci 21:222

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neisser U (1998) Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philos Psychol 1:35–59

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noë A (2004) Action in perception. Mass, The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Okuda J, Fujii T, Ohtake H, Tsukiura T, Tanji K, Suzuki K, Kawashima R, Fukuda H, Itoh M, Yamadori A (2003) Thinking of the future and the past: the roles of the frontal pole and the medial temporal lobes. NeuroImage 19:1369–1380

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pecher D, Zwaan R (eds) (2005) Grounding cognition: the role of perception and action in memory, language, and thought. Cambridge Univ. Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL (2007) The prospective brain: remembering the past to imagine the future. Nat Rev Neurosci 8:657–661

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter DL, Addis DR, Buckner RL (2008) Episodic simulation of future events: concepts, data, and applications. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1124:39–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci 21(5):188--194

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G., Sinigaglia, C (2006).So quel che fai. Il cervello che agisce e i neuroni specchio. Milano: Raffaello Cortina

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (1986) Relevance: communication and cognition. Blackwell, Oxford Second edition 1995

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber D, Wilson D (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mindreading. Mind & Language 17: 3–23. Special issue on pragmatics and cognitive science

  • Suddendorf T, Busby J (2005) Making decisions with the future in mind: developmental and comparative identification of mental time travel. Learn Motiv 36(2):110–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf T, Corballis M (2007) The evolution of foresight: what is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behav Brain Sci 30:299–313

    Google Scholar 

  • Szpunar KK, Watson JM, McDermott KB (2007) Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 104:642–647

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenbrink T, Wiener JM (2007) Way finding strategies in behavior and language: a symmetric and interdisciplinary approach to cognitive processes. In: Barkowsky T, Knauff M, Ligozat G, Montello D (eds) Spatial cognition V: reasoning, action, interaction. Springer, Berlin, pp 401–420

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (1972) Episodic and semantic memory. In: Tulving E, Donaldson W (eds) Organization of memory. Academic Press, New York, pp 381–403

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (1985) Memory and consciousness. Can Psychol 26:1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving E (2005) Episodic memory and autonoesi: uniquely human? In: Terrace HS, Metcalfe J (eds) The missing link in cognition: evolution of self-knowing consciousness. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp 3–56

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk TA (1980). The semantics and pragmatics of functional coherence in discourse. In: A. Ferrara (Ed.), Speech act theory: Ten years later (pp. 49–66). Special issue of Versus (Milano), 26/27

  • Wheeler MA, Stuss D, Tulving E (1997) Toward a theory of episodic memory: the frontal lobes and autonoetic consciousness. Psychol Bull 121:331–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev J (2008a) The dependence of language on consciousness. J Conscious Stud 15:34–62

    Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev J (2008b) The dialectics of consciousness and language. J Conscious Stud 15:5–14

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work is the outcome of a collaborative effort. However, for the final draft Erica Cosentino is mainly responsible for Sects. 2.2, 2.2.1 and 2.2.2, and Francesco Ferretti for Sects. 1, 2 and 2.1. Section 1.1 and the conclusions were written together.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Erica Cosentino.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Cosentino, E., Ferretti, F. Communication as Navigation: A New Role for Consciousness in Language. Topoi 33, 263–274 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-013-9166-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11245-013-9166-y

Keywords

Navigation