Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 February 2021
Sec. Health Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychophysiology of Stress View all 8 articles

Manipulating Levels of Socially Evaluative Threat and the Impact on Anticipatory Stress Reactivity

  • 1Stress Research Group, Department of Psychology, Northumbria University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom
  • 2Faculty of Medical Sciences, Population Health Sciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom

Previous work suggests that relative increases in socially evaluative threat modulate the psychobiological stress response. However, few studies have compared stressors which manipulate the level of socially evaluative threat to which the participant is exposed. Here we present two studies. In the first, we assessed the integrity of an ecologically valid, laboratory stressor (direct socially evaluated multitasking) and its effects on acute psychobiological reactivity and ability to evoke an anticipatory response prior to participation. Specifically, we assessed whether the expectation and experience of direct social evaluation (multitasking while standing and facing an evaluator) evokes greater reactivity than indirect evaluation (over-the-shoulder evaluation). In the second study, we sought to replicate the findings regarding acute stress reactivity whilst extending the assessment window to assess the extent to which the stressor evokes anticipatory responses. As hypothesized, greater reactivity was observed following direct social evaluation compared with indirect observation. Increases in anxiety, heart rate and blood pressure were demonstrated across both studies and the paradigm therefore provides an ecologically valid technique for the activation of psychological and cardiovascular stress responding. Additionally, anticipation of experiencing socially evaluated multitasking led to increases in anxiety, tension, and worry prior to the event itself, supporting previous suggestions that threat anticipation may prolong the activation of stress mechanisms. In the present studies we assessed whether the expectation and experience of direct social evaluation evokes greater reactivity than indirect evaluation. The findings have demonstrated that direct social evaluation of multitasking is a more potent stressor than multitasking with indirect evaluation. Furthermore, our findings indicate that the period of anticipation of stressful events may be critical to understanding the process of stress regulation, and as such we recommend extending the sampling window to allow for the investigation of these processes.

Introduction

Exposure to a situation perceived as challenging or threatening, which exceeds an individual's ability to cope, leads to a range of physiological responses which assist in managing the demand and mobilizing the required metabolic resources (Tomaka et al., 1993). When activated in the short-term these stress responses mediate adaption (McEwen, 2003); however, repeated or inappropriate activation is associated with a plethora of well-documented adverse effects on cardiovascular, immune, metabolic, and psychological health (McEwen, 1998). One of the determinants of vulnerability to stress-related ill-health is how individuals respond to daily stressors. Therefore, in order to understand the pathways by which exposure to stress leads to deleterious health outcomes it is necessary to develop tools which facilitate the observation of individuals while they are experiencing stress (Wetherell et al., 2006).

Previous studies have employed a variety of physiological and psychological stressors to assess responses of both the sympathetic adrenal medullary (SAM) and hypothalamic pituitary adrenal (HPA) axes, and provide an insight into their effects on health, cognition, and emotion (Schwabe et al., 2008). These stressors elicit a range of responses depending on their paradigm components; some activating the SAM axis, whilst others prompt activation of the slower reacting HPA axis. Whilst SAM reactivity is evidenced by more immediate responses to a stressor (such as cardiovascular reactivity and the release of adrenaline), HPA axis activity, detected through the secretion of cortisol from the adrenal glands, is typically observed in response to prolonged or more challenging (or resource-demanding) stressors. In particular, stressors that require motivated performance during social-evaluative conditions are associated with the most robust HPA reactivity in laboratory conditions (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004). However, whilst many previously employed stressors have successfully elicited a range of stress responses, the development of a tool allowing desired procedural control, whilst also obtaining ecological validity, remains a challenge.

Beyond eliciting stress reactivity, laboratory stressors should provide insight into how an individual would respond to a stressor encountered in a real-life setting (Wetherell et al., 2006). Therefore, to achieve this ecological validity it is important that individuals engage with laboratory stressors representative of their experiences in natural settings. For example, daily activity typically involves exposure to multiple sources of stress (Chida and Hamer, 2008), and therefore single task stressors are of limited utility. Ecologically valid laboratory stressors should therefore encompass multiple stimuli in order to replicate the environments experienced in daily life (Wetherell et al., 2006). In addition to exposure to multiple sources of stress, many commonly encountered stressors include an interval of “preparing for” or “anticipating” the planned event (e.g., Neubauer et al., 2018). Studies examining the anticipatory response preceding forthcoming acute, naturalistic stressors suggest that this period is often perceived as highly stressful, and sometimes even more so than the event itself (e.g., Greco and Roger, 2003). Further, it has been suggested that this process can prolong the activation of stress mechanisms designed for short-term arousal only. For example, in studies assessing anxiety in patients attending hospital for surgery, orthopedic surgery patients reported the greatest levels of anxiety the day before hospital admission, 2 days prior to the operation (Johnston, 1980). A further study assessing patients awaiting surgery suggests that uncertainty and fear were more stressful for patients awaiting heart surgery than the symptoms of the heart condition itself (Bengtson et al., 1996). Similarly, women awaiting diagnoses following an abnormal mammogram described the waiting period as a type of “limbo” whereby their lives were seriously disrupted with “panic attacks, insomnia, inability to concentrate at work, inability to plan, gastrointestinal upset, tearfulness, and preoccupation of fears” (p. 45, Thorne et al., 1999). Finally, women undergoing an emergency ultrasound in early pregnancy reported significantly higher levels of anxiety before, compared with after the ultrasound (Richardson et al., 2017). This was even the case when they subsequently received a conclusive diagnosis, regardless of whether they received a positive or negative result, further demonstrating that anticipation may be perceived as a more potent stressor than the planned event itself.

The impact of forthcoming demand on resources has also been observed in non-patient settings. University students have reported greater anxiety during the period preceding final exams (Lotz and Sparfeldt, 2017) and greater levels of cortisol have been observed in the period preceding sport competitions (van Paridon et al., 2017; Cintineo and Arent, 2019) as well as in veterinary students prior to performing surgery (Stevens et al., 2019). Manipulations of forthcoming demand also provide evidence of the role of the HPA axis, specifically, the Cortisol Awakening Response (CAR) as an anticipatory mechanism. The CAR, a surge in cortisol in the period immediately following awakening is posited to play a role in preparation for forthcoming demand (Stalder et al., 2015). Increases in the magnitude of the CAR have been observed on the day of anticipated stress and demand in ambulatory (Wetherell et al., 2015) and sleep lab (Elder et al., 2018) settings.

Collectively, these findings indicate that real world stressors evoke “thinking about” the event in advance and anticipatory stress responses facilitate adaptive physiological functioning. The assessment of anticipatory periods could therefore be usefully incorporated into ecologically valid laboratory stressor paradigms to allow for the identification of biomarkers which may be associated with adaptive and maladaptive reactivity of stress response mechanisms.

In this paper we present two studies that demonstrate the development of such a paradigm. In the first study we investigate psychobiological reactivity related to an ecologically valid laboratory stressor, the Multitasking Framework (MTF: Purple Research Solutions, UK). The MTF is a motivated performance task that elicits stress through the manipulation of workload intensity and reliably activates indices of stress reactivity (Wetherell and Sidgreaves, 2005; Scholey et al., 2009; Kelly-Hughes et al., 2014; Allen et al., 2019). More recently we have demonstrated that multitasking, whilst being indirectly evaluated (over the shoulder feedback given on performance), leads to increased reports of anxiety and cardiovascular reactivity compared with multitasking alone (Wetherell et al., 2017). The effects of socially evaluative threat on psychobiological stress responses, particularly the impact upon HPA reactivity, are well-established (Dickerson et al., 2008, 2009). However, less is known about whether manipulations in the delivery of socially evaluative threat differentially impacts upon stress reactivity.

In Study 1 we aim to extend the findings of Wetherell et al. (2017), by manipulating the delivery of socially evaluative threat. We assess whether direct social evaluation of multitasking (i.e., participant standing and facing the researcher) leads to acute activation of the HPA axis, measured via the cortisol response to the stressor, relative to indirect social evaluation (i.e., researcher standing behind seated participant). We further aimed to investigate whether there was any evidence of anticipatory stress prior to stressor exposure. Study 2 presents a replication of the acute psychobiological reactivity to direct socially evaluated multitasking, whilst extending the sampling window to assess the day prior to, the day of, and the day following stressor exposure to capture psychobiological indices across an extended anticipatory period.

We hypothesized that (1) direct socially evaluated multitasking would elicit greater psychobiological stress reactivity than indirect socially evaluated multitasking, and (2) anticipation of experiencing direct socially evaluated multitasking would be associated with alterations in state mood and HPA reactivity prior to stress exposure.

STUDY 1

Materials and Methods

Participants

The sample comprised 39 healthy adults (age range 18–37, Mage = 22.0, SDage = 4.62; 5 males, 34 females) from an undergraduate population. Participants were randomly allocated to receive either direct social evaluation (20 in total: three males, 17 females, Mage = 21.9, SDage = 4.36) or indirect social evaluation (19 in total: two males, 17 females, Mage = 22.1, SDage = 5.02) whilst multitasking. There were no significant differences between the two groups in terms of age, sex, perceived stress, or trait anxiety. Participants were screened for the following eligibility criteria: aged between 18 and 40 years, resting blood pressure <140/90 mmHg; not taking steroidal medication; not pregnant or breastfeeding; no history of panic attacks. All recruitment and study procedures were granted ethical approval from the Institutional Ethics Committee.

Materials

The Multitasking Framework (MTF: Purple Research Solutions, UK) is a computerized stressor that requires participants to attend to four tasks simultaneously that vary in terms of time pressure and/or difficulty; tasks are performance driven and demand is manipulated through instructing participants to achieve as high a score as they can. The present study included the following tasks: auditory monitoring, where participants are required to report a target tone; number tap, where participants are required to identify and report the highest digits in a 4 × 4 grid; visual monitoring, where participants are required to monitor and rest a cursor to prevent it leaving a target area; and a Stroop task (for a detailed description of tasks see Wetherell and Carter, 2014).

Cardiovascular Measurements

Heart rate and blood pressure were recorded using the DINAMAP Pro 200 V2 (GE Healthcare), via an inflatable brachial cuff.

Questionnaires

Items from the short-form State Anxiety scale (Marteau and Bekker, 1992) were used (I feel calm; I feel tense; I am upset; I feel relaxed; I feel content; I feel worried) alongside two additional items measuring stress (I feel stressed) and happiness (I feel happy) to assess state mood, with responses ranging from “not at all” to “very much.” Participants also completed items that assessed their feelings of mental alertness and physical tension in bed the night before, their levels of morning wellness and items to assess their thoughts about their forthcoming experiences (“to what extent have you been thinking about the stressor session?” and “to what extent have you been worrying about the stressor session?”).

Salivary Cortisol

Saliva samples were collected using Salivettes (Sarstedt, Germany). Samples were frozen (−20°C) and assayed in duplicate, using the enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay method (Salimetrics Europe, Cambridge UK, intra and inter assay coefficients <10%), in accordance with the manufacturer's instructions. Participants were requested to refrain from eating, drinking (other than water) and smoking for 1 h prior to providing saliva samples (Salimetrics Europe, Newmarket, United Kingdom).

Procedure

Participants satisfying the eligibility criteria were invited to provide written informed consent. Participants attended a 5-min study brief 2–7 days prior to their testing session. During this appointment direct social evaluation participants were notified that they would be required to stand in front of the researcher and receive social evaluation whilst completing a set of challenging tasks on the computer. Those in the indirect social evaluation condition were told that they would complete the tasks whilst seated and receive social evaluation from the experimenter behind them. Participants were provided with the state mood items and asked to complete them on the morning of the stressor session.

All laboratory testing took place at least 1 h following awakening and between 12:00 and 16:00 h, when levels of cortisol are typically lower and more stable (Saxbe, 2008). On the morning of the testing session (prior to attending the laboratory) participants completed the morning mood questionnaire. Upon arrival at the laboratory, participants were seated and the DINAMAP cuff was placed on their non-dominant arm. After a seated rest period of 10 min (Balodis et al., 2010), participants were given a 2-min demonstration of the tasks by the researcher (Wetherell and Carter, 2014). Prior to commencement of the 20-min period of multitasking, participants were informed that they needed to work as fast and accurately as possible, and to attain their highest achievable score. Upon cessation of the 20-min stressor participants were left alone in the room for a 20-min relaxation period. During this time, participants watched a nature documentary (Frozen Planet, BBC). In the indirect social evaluation condition, whilst completing the MTF participants received over-the-shoulder evaluation of their performance at set time intervals (see Wetherell et al., 2017, for full protocol). In the direct social evaluation condition, participants completed the MTF whilst standing behind a podium in front of the experimenter. The MTF screen was projected onto the wall behind them, enabling the researcher observation of participants' performance throughout the study. Social evaluation was received as per the indirect evaluation condition. Saliva was collected and state mood recorded at 5 intervals during the testing period: upon arrival at the laboratory; immediately before and after stressor exposure (20 min); 10 and 20 min following stressor cessation. Heart rate and blood pressure were recorded at the end of the rest period; following the MTF demonstration; and immediately, 10 and 20 min following stressor cessation. The procedure lasted 1 h. The protocol timeline and sampling procedure are presented in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Protocol timeline and sampling procedure: time (minutes) of each procedure prior/subsequent to the stress task displayed in the left column.

Treatment of Data

Mood, heart rate, blood pressure, and cortisol were assessed using 2-way repeated measures ANOVAs with group (direct social evaluation; indirect social evaluation) and time (Mood: arrival, post-stressor demonstration, stressor cessation, relaxation +10, relaxation +20/end of study; Cardiovascular parameters: arrival, post-stressor demonstration, stressor cessation, relaxation +10, relaxation +20/end of study; Cortisol: arrival, pre-stressor, post-stressor, relaxation +10, and relaxation +20/end of study). Bonferroni-corrected post-hoc analyses were conducted to assess changes between the 5 time points (arrival; post-stressor demonstration; stressor cessation; relaxation +10; relaxation +20/end of study).

Results

Cardiovascular Parameters

A significant main effect of condition was observed for heart rate, with greater heart rate for the direct compared to the indirect condition [F(1,37) = 4.29, p = 0.045, η2 = 0.10]. There was no significant main effect of condition for systolic [F(1,37) = 1.166, p = 0.287, η2 = 0.03] or diastolic [F(1,37) = 2.63, p = 0.113, η2 = 0.07] blood pressure. Significant main effects of time were observed for heart rate [F(4,34) = 13.56, p < 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.39, η2 = 0.66, see Figure 1A]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significant increases in heart rate from arrival at the laboratory to stressor cessation (p < 0.001). There was a significant main effect of time on systolic blood pressure [F(4,34) = 3.27, p = 0.023, Wilks' Λ = 0.72, η2 = 0.28, see Figure 1B]. Pairwise comparisons revealed a significant increase in systolic blood pressure from arrival to stressor cessation (p = 0.032). A significant main effect of time on diastolic blood pressure was observed [F(4,34) = 5.45, p = 0.002, Wilks' Λ = 0.61, η2 = 0.39, see Figure 1C]. Pairwise comparisons revealed a significant increase from arrival at the laboratory to stressor cessation (p = 0.001). A significant condition × time interaction was observed for heart rate [F(4,34) = 8.95, p = <0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.49, η2 = 0.17] and diastolic blood pressure [F(4,34) = 4.66, p = 0.004, Wilks' Λ = 0.65, η2 = 0.35]. A one-way ANOVA confirmed that cessation of the stressor was the point at which heart rate [F(1,37) = 26.29, p = <0.001, η2 = 0.42], systolic blood pressure [F(1,37) = 4.89, p = 0.033, η2 = 0.12], and diastolic blood pressure [F(1,37) = 19.67, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.35] significantly differed between the groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Mean (and SE) heart rate (A), systolic (B), and diastolic (C) blood pressure for direct and indirect social evaluation conditions.

Mean (and S.E.) heart rate, systolic and diastolic blood pressure across both conditions are presented in Figure 1.

State Mood

There was a significant main effect of time on state anxiety [F(4,34) = 27.36, p < 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.24, η2 = 0.76, see Figure 2]. Pairwise comparisons revealed a significant increase in anxiety from arrival at the laboratory and stressor cessation (p < 0.001) and decrease between arrival and nature documentary cessation (p = 0.001). No significant main effects of condition [F(1,37) = 0.50, p = 0.485, η2 = 0.01], or time × condition interactions were observed [F(4,34) = 0.50, p = 0.739, Wilks' Λ = 0.95, η2 = 0.06]. See Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean (and SE) state anxiety for the direct social evaluation and indirect social evaluation conditions, assessed during exposure to the stressor manipulation.

Cortisol

A significant main effect of time was observed for cortisol, representing a reduction across the testing period [F(4,29) = 6.35, p = 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.53, η2 = 0.47]. There was no significant main effect of condition [F(1,32) = 0.41, p = 0.527, η2 = 0.01] or time × condition interaction [F(4,29) = 1.81, p = 0.154, Wilks' Λ = 0.80, η2 = 0.02].

In order to adjust for the potential confounding influence of diurnal cortisol on cortisol reactivity, salivary cortisol values at each time point were adjusted for the diurnal slope in accordance with the procedure outlined by Elbau et al. (2018) and Kühnel et al. (2020), and the area under the curve was calculated using these adjusted values. There was no significant difference between the direct and indirect social evaluation conditions using this measure of cortisol reactivity [t(32) = 0.17, p = 0.86, d = 0.06].

Awakening Mood on Day of Stressor

Participants in the direct social evaluation condition reported significantly greater feelings of tension on the morning of the stressor, compared with those who were expecting indirect social evaluation [F(1,37) = 6.12, p = 0.018, η2 = 0.14]. They additionally reported feeling less content [F(1,37) = 5.79, p = 0.021, η2 = 0.17], less calm [F(1,37) = 8.40, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.18], and less happy [F(1,37) = 6.54, p = 0.015, η2 = 0.15] than those exposed to indirect social evaluation.

STUDY 2

Method

Participants

The sample comprised 31 healthy adults (age range 18–38 years, Mage = 24.4, SDage = 5.18) from an undergraduate population (none of whom had taken part in Study 1). One participant withdrew from the study after the first sampling day due to sleep disturbance. Thirty participants remained in the final analysis (10 males, 20 females).

Materials

All materials were identical to those used in Study 1.

Procedure

Participants satisfying the eligibility criteria (reported in Study 1) were invited to the laboratory for a short (10 min) briefing 2–4 days prior to the stressor session. Participants were told that they would be completing a stressor task which would be cognitively demanding, whilst standing in front of a researcher who would be monitoring their behavior and performance throughout the task and that they would receive critical evaluation of their performance throughout. Following receipt of written informed consent, participants were given data collection packs to take away with them. These packs included four sets of salivettes, a questionnaire booklet including state mood measures and a saliva sample collection diary. Participants received training on how to provide the saliva samples and were instructed to accurately record saliva collection times. Additionally, participants were asked to wear wrist-worn actigraph devices for the duration of the study and were told that their waking times were recorded. Unfortunately, problems with data recording prevented analyses of these data. Participants were asked to provide saliva samples immediately upon awakening; 30, 45, and 60 min following awakening; 6 h following awakening and immediately prior to bed on 3 consecutive days: the day before returning to the laboratory for the stressor (day 1), the day of the stressor (day 2), the day after (day 3), and on a control day (day 4). Additionally, participants recorded state mood upon awakening, 6 h after awakening and before bed and recorded the times at which they woke and provided each saliva sample. The Study 1 protocol for direct social evaluation was replicated. Participants provided all completed saliva samples upon arrival at the laboratory and returned the final day of samples after completion.

Treatment of Data

For acute stress reactivity, mood, heart rate, blood pressure, and cortisol were assessed using one-way repeated measures ANOVAs (variables and time points as in Study 1). Participants with missing cortisol data were excluded from acute cortisol analysis.

Daily variation in mood was assessed using two-way repeated measures ANOVAs with day (pre-stressor, stressor, post-stressor, control) and time (waking, waking +6 h, bed time). CAR magnitude was assessed using a repeated measures ANOVA (day: pre-stressor, stressor, post-stressor, control), and was expressed as the increase in cortisol observed between waking and the maximum secretion post-awakening.

Diurnal cortisol secretion was assessed by area under the curve with respect to ground (AUCg). AUCg was calculated for each participant on each day using the cortisol level (nmol/l) at each sampling point and the time (min) between each sample (Pruessner et al., 2003). Diurnal cortisol secretion was not calculated for participants who did not provide sufficient samples for AUCg calculation. Therefore, due to missing samples (insufficient saliva volume and/or missing samples) considerably reducing sample size for this analysis, AUCg analysis was conducted for the pre-stressor and stressor days only.

Results

Stressor Effects

Cardiovascular Parameters

Significant main effects of time were observed for heart rate [F(8,20) = 19.82, p < 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.11, partial η2 = 0.89]; SBP [F(8,20) = 15.31, p < 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.14, η2 = 0.86]; and DBP [F(8,20) = 5.36, p = 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.32, η2 = 0.68]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significant increases from arrival at the laboratory, to during and post-stress measures across all parameters (p = 0.001–0.03). See Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean (and SE) heart rate (A), systolic (B), and diastolic (C) blood pressure assessed during exposure to the stressor manipulation.

State Anxiety

There was a significant main effect of time on state anxiety [F(4,23) = 19.03, p < 0.001, Wilks' Λ = 0.23, η2 = 0.77]. Post-hocs revealed significant increases in anxiety post-stressor demonstration (p = 0.02) and post-stressor (p < 0.001) and reductions during relaxation (p = 0.01). See Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean (and SE) state anxiety, assessed during exposure to the stressor manipulation.

Cortisol

There were no significant changes in cortisol across the stressor period [F(4,21) = 1.96, p = 0.138, Wilks' Λ = 0.73, η2 = 0.27].

Non-acute Variables

Psychological Indices

A significant main effect of day was observed for state anxiety [F(3,21) = 4.05, p = 0.020, Wilks' Λ = 0.63, η2 = 0.37]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly greater reports of state anxiety on the day of the stressor, compared with the post-stressor day (p = 0.024), and control day (p = 0.026). See Figure 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Mean (and SE) state anxiety assessed in the morning, at waking +6 h and bedtime, over the four sampling days.

There was a significant main effect of day on self-reported stress [F(3,25) = 4.32, p = 0.014, Wilks' Λ = 0.66, η2 = 0.34]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly greater reports of stress on the pre-stressor day compared with control day (p = 0.009), day of stressor compared with post-stressor day (p = 0.040), and the day of the stressor compared with the control day (p = 0.004). See Figure 6A.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Mean (and SE) morning self-reported stress (A), happiness (B), “thinking about” the stressor (C), and “worrying about” the stressor (D), assessed over four sampling days.

There was also a significant main effect of day on self-reported happiness [F(3,25) = 4.34, p = 0.014, Wilks' Λ = 0.66, η2 = 0.34]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly higher scores for happiness reported on: post-stressor day compared with pre-stressor day (p = 0.006), the control day compared with the pre-stressor day (p = 0.040), and on the post-stressor day compared with the day of the stressor (p = 0.005). See Figure 6B.

There was also a significant main effect of day on the extent to which participants reported (in the morning) thinking about the stressor session [F(3,24) = 6.33, p = 0.003, Wilks' Λ = 0.56, η2 = 0.44]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly greater reports of thinking about the stressor session on: the day of the stressor compared with the pre-stressor day (p = 0.004), the day of the stressor compared with the post-stressor day (p = 0.048), the post-stressor day compared with the control day (p = 0.010), and the day of the stressor compared with the control day (p = <0.001). See Figure 6C.

There was a significant effect of day on self-reported worrying about the stressor [F(3,20) = 4.50, p = 0.014, Wilks' Λ = 0.60, η2 = 0.40]. Pairwise comparisons revealed significantly greater reports of worrying on the morning of the stressor compared with the pre-stressor day (p = 0.001) the day of the stressor compared with the post-stressor day (p = 0.006), and the day of the stressor compared with the control day (p = 0.003). See Figure 6D.

Basal Cortisol Indices

Diurnal Secretion

A paired samples t-test revealed no significant differences in AUCG between pre-stressor day (M = 5816.67, SD = 3753.40) and day of stressor (M = 5751.38, SD = 2959.98), t(16) = 0.12, p = 0.909.

CAR Magnitude

There were no significant differences in CAR magnitude [F(3,18) = 1.50, p = 0.248, Wilks' Λ = 0.80, partial η2 = 0.20], across the days: pre-stressor day (M = 8.57, SD = 8.58); day of stressor (M = 6.44, SD = 7.95); post-stressor day (M = 3.55, SD = 6.78); control day (M = 5,99, SD = 6.89).

Discussion

The aims of this paper were 2-fold: firstly, to present the development of an ecologically valid laboratory stressor (direct socially evaluated multitasking) and its effects on acute psychobiological reactivity and its ability to elicit anticipatory responses prior to participation. Specifically, we assessed whether the expectation and experience of direct social evaluation evokes greater reactivity than the indirect social evaluation reported previously (Wetherell et al., 2017). Secondly, we sought to replicate the findings regarding acute stress reactivity whilst extending the assessment window to assess the extent to which the stressor evokes anticipatory responses. In relation to the first aim, as hypothesized, greater reactivity was observed following direct social evaluation compared with indirect observation. Specifically, direct social evaluation elicited significantly greater heart rate and diastolic blood pressure reactivity. This pattern also emerged for systolic blood pressure; however, the difference between the stressors was not statistically significant. This may reflect differences in the underlying mechanisms involved in physiological functioning (e.g., Allen et al., 1987; Willemsen et al., 1998) or the modest samples size being underpowered to detect between group differences. The large effect sizes and the demonstration of significant reactivity of all cardiovascular measures in Study 2 suggest the latter.

In addition to increased cardiovascular activity as a function of direct social evaluation, other differences between the conditions should also be considered. Specifically, changes in orthostatic pressure from seating to standing may also contribute to the greater cardiovascular reactivity observed in the direct socially evaluated condition. That is, unlike the indirect social evaluation condition where participants remain seated throughout, the direct social evaluation condition required participants to stand for the duration of the stressor and this process can lead to increases in cardiovascular parameters (e.g., Freeman et al., 2011). It is therefore important to acknowledge that these orthostatic changes may have additional effects on cardiovascular responses over and above that of direct social evaluation. This issue however is not specific to the current paradigm, indeed the current cardiovascular measurement protocol was based on the standard Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) procedure where following a period of seated baseline, participants complete the stressor task while standing (e.g., Kirschbaum et al., 1993).

The cardiovascular reactivity observed across both studies is consistent with previous studies using similar measurement protocols comprising social evaluation (e.g., TSST: Allen et al., 2014). Taken together, these provide further evidence that social observation during performance tasks elicits physiological reactivity; however, social evaluation elicited by direct observation is a more potent stressor than indirect, over-the shoulder evaluation. This supports the notion that situations involving a perceived threat to the “social self,” as would be experienced through direct evaluation of multitasking performance, are potent stressors (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004; Dickerson et al., 2009).

In line with this notion, we hypothesized that direct social evaluation would also elicit acute cortisol reactivity. This, however, was not the case even following statistical adjustment for the impact of the diurnal decline across this assessment period. The stressor task was developed to incorporate the key components present in laboratory stressors: motivated performance, uncontrollability, and social evaluation. Moreover, the paradigm was manipulated to increase the level of social evaluation, experienced through delivering social feedback directly to the participant, rather than over the shoulder. The absence of cortisol reactivity may therefore be explained by evaluating how this stressor paradigm differs from other stressors that reliably elicit cortisol reactivity, specifically, the TSST (Kirschbaum et al., 1993). The TSST requires participants to deliver a free speech and verbal mental arithmetic for 10 min to a panel of assessors following a 5 min preparation period. In common with the TSST, the current stressor involves the completion of challenging tasks; however, the responses are entered via a computer rather than vocalized to a panel of assessors. As other stressors have demonstrated HPA reactivity to single and even virtual assessors (Turner-Cobb et al., 2019), it seems likely that public speaking plays a crucial role in activating the HPA axis. Public speaking presents a potent threat to the “social self” and carries a significant risk of social embarrassment and humiliation leading to distress (Garcia-Leal et al., 2014). As such, public speaking is a well-documented stressor, causing cardiovascular activation (Elsenbruch et al., 2006), and cortisol reactivity (Auer et al., 2018). HPA reactivity has been observed in some individuals in response to other stressors that do not comprise a verbal component (e.g., the imaging stress task used within an MRI scanner, Kühnel et al., 2020). However, the absence of a public speaking element may be an explanation for the lack of salivary cortisol reactivity in response to the current stressor.

Both stressors increased state anxiety, and this finding was replicated for direct observation in Study 2. However, there were no differences in acute anxiety responses between direct and indirect social evaluation. This finding is also consistent with Dickerson and Kemeny (2004) who report that socially evaluative stress may elicit different patterns of physiological reactivity but may not increase self-reports of distress. Moreover, they suggest that socially evaluative stress may lead to more specific cognitive and emotional reactions. In support, we observed changes in a number of emotional responses in anticipation of completing a socially evaluative stressor. Those who anticipated forthcoming direct social evaluation reported feeling more tense, and less calm, content and happy on the morning of the stressor than those expecting to experience indirect social evaluation. These differences suggest that the appraisal mechanics engaged during stressor exposure may also be engaged during the anticipatory period prior to exposure (Everly and Lating, 2019). This concept was therefore assessed more comprehensively in Study 2. In support, participants reported significantly greater state anxiety and stress on the day of the stressor than the day following. Participants also reported both “thinking about” and feeling significantly more worried about the stressor session on the morning of the laboratory session compared to both the day prior and post-stressor exposure. This finding is consistent with previous work investigating anticipation of a socially evaluative laboratory stressor, whereby participants reported greater levels of tension, perceived stress, and anxiety on the morning of the anticipated stressor (Wetherell et al., 2015). Unlike this study, however, anticipation of the stressor in the current study did not alter cortisol secretion following awakening (CAR) or levels across the day (AUCg). Although in both studies participants were expecting a socially evaluative threat, public speaking may have been perceived as a greater threat than the observed multitasking anticipated in the current study. It has been argued that the cognitive preoccupation with upcoming tasks acts as a stressor in its own right (Ennis et al., 2001; Schlotz et al., 2004) and can, therefore, subsequently promote HPA axis activation (Roger and Najarian, 1998).

We have previously discussed the adaptive role of the anticipatory stress response in preparing for forthcoming demand, with specific attention to observing greater anticipatory reactivity prior to planned threat and challenge. However, the absence of anticipatory cortisol responses in this study may also be considered from an evolutionary perspective; the ability to conceptualize and accurately anticipate potential challenges or threats is adaptive, as the negative affect associated with anticipating a stressful event allows for appropriate modifications to be made with regards to behavior, cognition and physiology. This process means the individual is prepared for the forthcoming demand, but in some situations the forward-planning this requires, combined with the motivation to take measures to avoid the event, could result in mitigating the threat altogether (Aspinwall and Taylor, 1997; Schulkin, 2011), or merely appraising the event to be less threatening, due to having time to prepare. Dickerson and Kemeny (2004) describe a dose-response relationship between the degree of social evaluation involved in a stressor, and the magnitude of cortisol response, and therefore the degree to which the event is deemed “threatening” has a direct impact on the subsequent response. The earlier examples reporting greater cortisol in the period preceding events (e.g., sport competitions; van Paridon et al., 2017; Cintineo and Arent, 2019; veterinary students performing surgery; Stevens et al., 2019) were more personal with greater perceived consequences. In comparison, the threat of forthcoming social evaluated multitasking may have been sufficient to modify emotional responses, but not potent enough to alter HPA function.

These findings should be considered in light of limitations. First, the sample sizes were of sufficient size to observe predicted changes in the psychobiological variables that have been previously evaluated in relation to this paradigm. However, other related issues should be considered, in particular, the majority of participants were female. There are well-documented sex differences in stress responding, males typically demonstrate greater cortisol reactivity than females (Kudielka et al., 2009); females produce greater cortisol responses during the luteal than during the follicular phase of the menstrual cycle (Childs et al., 2010); and hormonal contraceptive use may also contribute to variability in stress reactivity (Nielsen et al., 2013). Our sample size, compounded by instances of missing menstrual cycle data prevented meaningful analyses to correct for these factors, and potential differences related to the sample should therefore be acknowledged. Second, other individual differences may have impacted upon stress reactivity. For example, although the Multitasking Framework is performance driven, actual performance was not recorded. It may be the case that better performance would impact upon stress reactivity; however, the robust increases in psychological and cardiovascular stress reactivity demonstrate the capacity of the paradigm to induce stress. Finally, the assessment of the CAR relies upon accurate sampling and adherence to the sampling protocol, particularly the timing of waking in relation to the provision of samples. In line with guidelines (Stalder et al., 2015), strategies were implemented to facilitate protocol adherence; participants were given full training on appropriate collection, were made aware of the importance of following the protocol and were asked to provide a record of waking and sample provision times. Attempts were made to objectively monitor waking and sampling times through wrist-worn actigraphy devices, unfortunately, these data could not be used as intended. The wearing of the devices alongside information regarding monitoring can increase adherence (Broderick et al., 2004), and in support, there were no instances of non-adherence based on participants' self-reported timings. However, the absence of objective monitoring to verify participant waking and sample provision may impact upon the accuracy samples obtained during the CAR period.

Notwithstanding these limitations, the present studies have demonstrated that direct social evaluation of multitasking is a more potent stressor than multitasking with indirect social evaluation. The stressor does not involve public speaking, and this is a likely explanation for the absence of cortisol reactivity. Nonetheless, increases in anxiety, heart rate and blood pressure were demonstrated across two studies and the paradigm therefore provides an ecologically valid technique for the activation of psychological and cardiovascular stress responding. Additionally, anticipation of experiencing socially evaluated multitasking led to alterations in state factors prior to the event itself. Although the personal salience of the forthcoming task is a likely explanation of the absence of HPA reactivity during this period, changes in state mood and cognitive appraisal provide evidence that the period prior to forthcoming events is subject to anticipatory processes. The period of anticipation of stressful events may be critical to understanding the process of stress regulation (Ottaviani, 2018), and as such, we recommend the use of paradigms that extend the sampling window around an anticipated stressor to allow for the investigation of these processes.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Department of Psychology, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, Northumbria University Newcastle. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

OC, MS and MW designed the study protocols and prepared the manuscript. OC executed the study protocols and ran the preliminary analyses. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was funded internally by Northumbria University as part of a PhD research programme.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Anthea Wilde and Karen Walker for conducting cortisol assays.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.622030/full#supplementary-material

References

Allen, A. P., Kennedy, P. J., Cryan, J. F., Dinan, T. G., and Clarke, G. (2014). Biological and psychological markers of stress in humans: focus on the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 38, 94–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M. T., Sherwood, A., Obrist, P. A., Crowell, M. D., and Grange, L. A. (1987). Stability of cardiovascular reactivity to laboratory stressors: a 212 year follow-up. J. Psychosom. Res. 31, 639–645. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(87)90043-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, S. F., Wetherell, M. A., and Smith, M. A. (2019). An experimental investigation into cardiovascular, haemodynamic and salivary alpha amylase reactivity to acute stress in Type D individuals. Stress 22, 428–435. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1583741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aspinwall, L. G., and Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in time: self-regulation and proactive coping. Psychol. Bull. 121, 417–436. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.3.417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auer, B. J., Calvi, J. L., Jordan, N. M., Schrader, D., and Byrd-Craven, J. (2018). Communication and social interaction anxiety enhance interleukin-1 beta and cortisol reactivity during high-stakes public speaking. Psychoneuroendocrinology 94, 83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balodis, I. M., Wynne-Edwards, K. E., and Olmstead, M. C. (2010). The other side of the curve: examining the relationship between pre-stressor physiological responses and stress reactivity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 1363–1373. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bengtson, A., Herlitz, J., Karlsson, T., and Hjalmarson, A. (1996). Distress correlates with the degree of chest pain: a description of patients awaiting revascularisation. Heart 75, 257–260. doi: 10.1136/hrt.75.3.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broderick, J. E., Arnold, D., Kudielka, B. M., and Kirschbaum, C. (2004). Salivary cortisol sampling compliance: comparison of patients and healthy volunteers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 636–650. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(03)00093-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chida, Y., and Hamer, M. (2008). Chronic psychosocial factors and acute physiological responses to laboratory-induced stress in healthy populations: a quantitative review of 30 years of investigations. Psychol. Bull. 134, 829–885. doi: 10.1037/a0013342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Childs, E., Dlugos, A., and De Wit, H. (2010). Cardiovascular, hormonal, and emotional responses to the TSST in relation to sex and menstrual cycle phase. Psychophysiology 47, 550–559. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2009.00961.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cintineo, H. P., and Arent, S. M. (2019). Anticipatory salivary cortisol and state anxiety before competition predict match outcome in division I collegiate wrestlers. J. Strength Condit. Res. 33, 2905–2908. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0000000000003376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickerson, S. S., Gruenewald, T. L., and Kemeny, M. E. (2009). Psychobiological responses to social self threat: functional or detrimental? Self Identity 8, 270–285. doi: 10.1080/15298860802505186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickerson, S. S., and Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors and cortisol responses: a theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research. Psychol. Bull. 130, 355–391. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.3.355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickerson, S. S., Mycek, P. J., and Zaldivar, F. (2008). Negative social evaluation, but not mere social presence, elicits cortisol responses to a laboratory stressor task. Health Psychol. 27, 116–121. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elbau, I. G., Brücklmeier, B., Uhr, M., Arloth, J., Czamara, D., Spoormaker, V. I., et al. (2018). The brain's hemodynamic response function rapidly changes under acute psychosocial stress in association with genetic and endocrine stress response markers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, E10206–E10215. doi: 10.1073/1804340115/pnas.1804340115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elder, G. J., Barclay, N. L., Wetherell, M. A., and Ellis, J. G. (2018). Anticipated next day demand affects the magnitude of the cortisol awakening response but not subjective or objective sleep. J. Sleep Res. 27, 47–55. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsenbruch, S., Lucas, A., Holtmann, G., Haag, S., Gerken, G., Riemenschneider, N., et al. (2006). Public speaking stress-induced neuroendocrine responses and circulating immune cell redistribution in irritable bowel syndrome. Am. J. Gastroenterol. 101, 2300–2307. doi: 10.1111/j.1572-0241.2006.00837.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ennis, M., Kelly, K. S., and Lambert, P. L. (2001). Sex differences in cortisol excretion during anticipation of a psychological stressor: possible support for the tend–and–befriend hypothesis. Stress Health 17, 253–261. doi: 10.1002/smi.904

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everly, G. S., and Lating, J. M. (2019). “The anatomy and physiology of the human stress response,” in A Clinical Guide to the Treatment of the Human Stress Response (New York, NY: Springer), 19–56.

Google Scholar

Freeman, R., Wieling, W., Axelrod, F. B., Benditt, D. G., Benarroch, E., Biaggioni, I., et al. (2011). Consensus statement on the definition of orthostatic hypotension, neurally mediated syncope and the postural tachycardia syndrome. Clin. Auton. Res. 21, 69–72. doi: 10.1007/s10286-011-0119-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Leal, C., Graeff, F. G., and Del-Ben, C. M. (2014). Experimental public speaking: contributions to the understanding of the serotonergic modulation of fear. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 46, 407–417. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greco, V., and Roger, D. (2003). Uncertainty, stress, and health. Pers. Individ. Differ. 34, 1057–1068. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00091-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, M. (1980). Anxiety in surgical patients. Psycholol. Med. 10, 145–152. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700039684

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly-Hughes, D. H., Wetherell, M. A., and Smith, M. A. (2014). Type D personality and cardiovascular reactivity to an ecologically valid multitasking stressor. Psychol. Health 29, 1156–1175. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2014.915970

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., and Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., and Wüst, S. (2009). Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 2–18. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kühnel, A., Kroemer, N. B., Elbau, I. G., Czisch, M., Sämann, P. G., Walter, M., et al. (2020). Psychosocial stress reactivity habituates following acute physiological stress. Hum. Brain Mapp. 41, 4010–4023. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lotz, C., and Sparfeldt, J. R. (2017). Does test anxiety increase as the exam draws near?–Students' state test anxiety recorded over the course of one semester. Pers. Individ. Differ. 104, 397–400. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.08.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marteau, T. M., and Bekker, H. (1992). The development of a six-item short-form of the state scale of the Spielberger State—Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI). Br. J. Clin. Psychol. 31, 301–306. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1992.tb00997.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (1998). Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N. Engl. J. Med. 338, 171–179.

Google Scholar

McEwen, B. S. (2003). Mood disorders and allostatic load. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 200–207. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(03)00177-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neubauer, A. B., Smyth, J. M., and Sliwinski, M. J. (2018). When you see it coming: stressor anticipation modulates stress effects on negative affect. Emotion 18, 342–354. doi: 10.1037/emo0000381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, S. E., Segal, S. K., Worden, I. V., Yim, I. S., and Cahill, L. (2013). Hormonal contraception use alters stress responses and emotional memory. Biol. Psychol. 92, 257–266. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ottaviani, C. (2018). Brain-heart interaction in perseverative cognition. Psychophysiology 55:e13082. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13082

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., and Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 29, 563–564. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4530(02)00108-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, A., Raine-Fenning, N., Deb, S., Campbell, B., and Vedhara, K. (2017). Anxiety associated with diagnostic uncertainty in early pregnancy. Ultrasound Obstet. Gynecol. 50, 247–254. doi: 10.1002/uog.17214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roger, D., and Najarian, B. (1998). The relationship between emotional rumination and cortisol secretion under stress. Pers. Individ. Differ. 24, 531–538. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00204-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxbe, D. E. (2008). A field (researcher's) guide to cortisol: tracking HPA axis functioning in everyday life. Health Psychol. Rev. 2, 163–190. doi: 10.1080/17437190802530812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlotz, W., Hellhammer, J., Schulz, P., and Stone, A. A. (2004). Perceived work overload and chronic worrying predict weekend–weekday differences in the cortisol awakening response. Psychosom. Med. 66, 207–214. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000116715.78238.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholey, A., Haskell, C., Robertson, B., Kennedy, D., Milne, A., and Wetherell, M. (2009). Chewing gum alleviates negative mood and reduces cortisol during acute laboratory psychological stress. Physiol. Behav. 97, 304–312. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulkin, J. (2011). Social allostasis: anticipatory regulation of the internal milieu. Front. Evol. Neurosci. 2:111. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2010.00111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, L., Haddad, L., and Schachinger, H. (2008). HPA axis activation by a socially evaluated cold-pressor test. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 890–895. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stalder, T., Kirschbaum, C., Kudielka, B. L., Adam, E. K., Pruessner, J. C., Wüst, S., et al. (2015). Assessment of the cortisol awakening response: expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 63, 414–432. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, B. S., Royal, K. D., Ferris, K., Taylor, A., and Snyder, A. M. (2019). Effect of a mindfulness exercise on stress in veterinary students performing surgery. Vet. Surg. 48, 360–366. doi: 10.1111/vsu.13169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thorne, S. E., Harris, S. R., Hislop, T. G., and Vestrup, J. A. (1999). The experience of waiting for diagnosis after an abnormal mammogram. Breast J. 5, 42–51. doi: 10.1046/j.1524-4741.1999.005001042.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomaka, J., Blascovich, J., Kelsey, R. M., and Leitten, C. L. (1993). Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 65, 248–260. doi: 10.1037/10125-019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner-Cobb, J. M., Asid, M., Turner, J. E., Bevan, C., and Stanton-Fraser, D. (2019). Use of a non-human robot audience to induce stress reactivity in human participants. Comput. Hum. Behav. 99, 76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2019.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Paridon, K. N., Timmis, M. A., Nevison, C. M., and Bristow, M. (2017). The anticipatory stress response to sport competition; a systematic review with meta-analysis of cortisol reactivity. BMJ Open Sport Exerc. Med. 3:e000261. doi: 10.1136/bmjsem-2017-000261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherell, M. A., and Carter, K. (2014). The multitasking framework: the effects of increasing workload on acute psychobiological stress reactivity. Stress Health 30, 103–109. doi: 10.1002/smi.2496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherell, M. A., Craw, O., Smith, K., and Smith, M. A. (2017). Psychobiological responses to critically evaluated multitasking. Neurobiol. Stress 7, 68–73. doi: 10.1016/j.ynstr.2017.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherell, M. A., Crown, A. L., Lightman, S. L., Miles, J. N., Kaye, J., and Vedhara, K. (2006). The four-dimensional stress test: psychological, sympathetic–adrenal–medullary, parasympathetic and hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal responses following inhalation of 35% CO2. Psychoneuroendocrinology 31, 736–747. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherell, M. A., Lovell, B., and Smith, M. A. (2015). The effects of an anticipated challenge on diurnal cortisol secretion. Stress 18, 42–48. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2014.993967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wetherell, M. A., and Sidgreaves, M. C. (2005). Secretory immunoglobulin-A reactivity following increases in workload intensity using the Defined Intensity Stressor Simulation (DISS). Stress Health 21, 99–106. doi: 10.1002/smi.1038

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willemsen, G., Ring, C., Carroll, D., Evans, P., Clow, A., and Hucklebridge, F. (1998). Secretory immunoglobulin A and cardiovascular reactions to mental arithmetic and cold pressor. Psychophysiology 35, 252–259.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: stress, anticipation, social evaluation, multitasking, physiological stress response

Citation: Craw OA, Smith MA and Wetherell MA (2021) Manipulating Levels of Socially Evaluative Threat and the Impact on Anticipatory Stress Reactivity. Front. Psychol. 12:622030. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.622030

Received: 27 October 2020; Accepted: 02 February 2021;
Published: 22 February 2021.

Edited by:

Pablo Ruisoto, Public University of Navarre, Spain

Reviewed by:

Matias M. Pulopulos, University of Zaragoza, Spain
Adam M. Goodman, University of Alabama at Birmingham, United States

Copyright © 2021 Craw, Smith and Wetherell. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Olivia A. Craw, olivia.craw@newcastle.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.