Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 30 November 2018
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards an Understanding of the Relationship between Spatial Processing Ability and Numerical and Mathematical Cognition View all 11 articles

Numerical and Non-numerical Predictors of First Graders’ Number-Line Estimation Ability

  • Department of Psychology, Georgetown University, Washington, DC, United States

Children’s ability to map numbers into a spatial context has been shown to be a powerful predictor of math performance. Here, we investigate how three types of cognitive abilities – approximate number processing ability, symbolic number processing ability, and non-numerical cognitive abilities – predict 0–100 number-line estimation performance in first graders. While each type of measure predicts number-line performance when considered individually, when considered together, only symbolic number comparison and non-verbal reasoning predicted unique variance in number-line estimation. Moreover, the relation between symbolic number comparison and number-line ability was stronger for male students than for female students, suggesting potential gender differences in the way boys and girls accomplish mapping numbers into space. These results suggest that number-line estimation ability is largely reflective of the precision with which symbolic magnitudes are represented (at least among boys). Our findings therefore suggest that promoting children’s understanding of symbolic, rather than non-symbolic, numerical magnitudes may help children learn better from number-lines in the classroom.

Introduction

Children’s ability to map numbers into a spatial context has been shown to be a powerful predictor of math performance (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2008; Sasanguie et al., 2013; Lyons et al., 2014; Friso-van den Bos et al., 2015; Schneider et al., 2018). Past research using number-line estimation tasks, in which children mark the spatial location of a given number (e.g., “72”) on a horizontal line (typically with only the endpoints indicated, e.g., with 0 at the left end and 100 at the right end), has been shown to predict performance on other measures of basic numeracy (Laski and Siegler, 2007; Maertens et al., 2016) and arithmetic (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2008; Lyons et al., 2014; Schneider et al., 2018). Moreover, experimental research has demonstrated that playing board games meant to bolster the visuospatial representation of numerical values in children improves numerical knowledge and performance on a range of numerical and mathematical tasks (Ramani and Siegler, 2008; Siegler and Ramani, 2009; Ramani et al., 2012; Maertens et al., 2016). The precision with which children perform number-line estimation tasks has been argued to reflect the precision with which children represent numerical magnitudes (Laski and Siegler, 2007; Booth and Siegler, 2008), which has been proposed by some researchers to serve as a key foundation for more complex mathematical processing (e.g., Feigenson et al., 2013; Siegler and Braithwaite, 2017). Given both the predictive and potentially causal role that visuospatial representations of numerical magnitude play in the development of mathematics, an important question is what basic numerical abilities contribute to the early development of these visuospatial representations.

Past work examining number-line estimation ability has in part focused on pinpointing when key developmental shifts occur (Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2008; Siegler and Ramani, 2009). Of particular importance, multiple studies have found that by the time children are in second grade, students have developed a fairly linear 0–100 mental number-line, whereas children in first grade are, on average, still in the process of linearizing their visuospatial representations of 0–100 (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2006). The development of this mental number-line has been theorized to have a core role in broader numerical cognition (Siegler et al., 2011, 2013). Siegler et al. (2011) have argued for an integrated theory of numerical development in which numerical development involves coming to understand that “all real numbers have magnitudes that can be ordered and assigned specific locations on number-lines.”

While the development of a precise mental number-line is thought to play an important role in broader numerical development, it is important to note that performance on the number-line task is not a pure reflection of children’s numerical understanding. Recent work has shown that non-numerical factors, particularly strategy selection, play a substantial role in children’s number-line performance (Barth and Paladino, 2011; Cohen and Blanc-Goldhammer, 2011; Slusser et al., 2013; Rouder and Geary, 2014; Dackermann et al., 2015; Peeters et al., 2016, 2017; van’t Noordende et al., 2016). The role that individual differences in strategy selection play in number-line performance makes it important to consider non-numerical factors, such as non-verbal reasoning ability, that may impact children’s performance on the number-line task.

Given the centrality with which increasing precision of the mental number-line is theorized to play in more general numerical development, understanding what basic numerical and non-numerical cognitive abilities predict the ability to precisely map numbers into space during key developmental shifts can give us insight into possible mechanisms that could underlie core numerical abilities. The goal of the present research is to understand what basic numerical and non-numerical cognitive abilities predict the ability to precisely map numbers into space during a key developmental period.

Here we consider three main hypotheses about what types of basic numerical and non-numerical cognition may support visuospatial number-line estimates in early grade school. According to one view, approximate number processing has been argued to be the foundation upon which more complex numerical abilities are grounded (Dehaene, 1997; Libertus et al., 2011, 2012, 2013; Feigenson et al., 2013). Because number-line estimation abilities are still developing in first graders (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2006), it may be the case that individual differences in approximate number processing at this age are predictive of number-line abilities. More specifically, this view predicts that a common measure of approximate number processing (i.e., determining which of two arrays of dots contains the greater quantity) should be a robust predictor of number-line estimation accuracy.

A second view is that symbolic representation of numerical quantities (e.g., Indo-Arabic numerals) serves as a crucial conceptual leap that underpins much of the subsequent development of more complex numerical thinking (e.g., De Smedt et al., 2009; Bugden and Ansari, 2011; Merkley and Ansari, 2016; Vanbinst et al., 2016; Núñez, 2017). A canonical measure of basic symbolic number processing is via numeral comparison tasks in which children indicate which of two numerals (e.g., ‘6’ and ‘8’) represents the greater quantity. Performance on this task has been shown to be a strong predictor of math achievement across a wide range of ages and settings (Holloway and Ansari, 2009; Nosworthy et al., 2013; Vanbinst et al., 2016; Sasanguie et al., 2017; Lyons et al., 2018). Moreover, previous work has shown that improvements in number-line estimation accuracy are associated with improvements in numeral comparison ability (Laski and Siegler, 2007; Ramani and Siegler, 2008), indicating that these two basic numerical abilities may be fundamentally intertwined early in development. However, it remains less clear whether these two abilities are uniquely related – that is, does the relation obtain even after controlling, for example, for approximate number processing, general cognitive ability, and other basic numerical abilities such as counting, ordering and estimation.

A third hypothesis is that reasoning or general cognitive ability – more so than other basic numerical abilities – is the strongest predictor of number-line estimation in early grade-school. As the work demonstrating effects of strategy utilization shows (e.g., Slusser et al., 2013; Peeters et al., 2016), numerical understanding is not the only thing that contributes to number-line performance. It is therefore possible that children with higher levels of general reasoning ability will demonstrate better number-line performance (even after controlling for basic numerical abilities), via the ability to select the most effective strategies.

Of course, the hypotheses outlined above are not mutually exclusive. Indeed, previous studies have demonstrated that measures of all three kinds significantly relate to number-line performance (Opfer and Siegler, 2007; Sasanguie et al., 2012; Fuhs and McNeil, 2013; Fazio et al., 2014; Maertens et al., 2016). However, to our knowledge, no work has examined the unique contributions of these numerical and non-numerical abilities to number-line estimation. Learning what predicts unique variance in number-line estimation ability will allow for a more precise understanding of which aspects of early numeracy are foundational in the development of a precise mental number-line. Such an understanding would allow for the generation of testable hypotheses about how to improve number-line estimation ability (and in turn math skills).

While the measures mentioned above are of primary theoretical interest, assessing the extent to which other basic numerical abilities (i.e., numerical ordering ability or counting proficiency) predicts number-line estimation performance comes with at least two benefits: First, it is possible that the three hypotheses outlined above are incomplete – testing other basic abilities allows us to check for additional factors that may impact number-line estimation performance not covered by those hypotheses. Second, given that other basic numerical abilities have also been shown to predict more complex math (Lyons and Beilock, 2011; Lyons et al., 2014), it is important to control for these other abilities to estimate as precisely and conservatively as possible the unique variance in number-line performance that can be attributed to the measures of primary theoretical interest outlined above.

In this study, we used data from over 200 Dutch first graders to understand what basic numerical and general cognitive factors predict unique variance in 0–100 number-line performance. We chose to focus on first graders because past work has suggested that important developmental shifts in 0–100 number-line performance occur during this year (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2006), and because this age group shows sufficient variability in terms of individual differences in our sample to allow for meaningful inferences to be drawn from a multiple regression approach. Finally, given substantial evidence for gender differences in number-line estimation, especially in first grade (Thompson and Opfer, 2008; Gunderson et al., 2012; Hutchison et al., 2018), we assess whether the strength of the potential relations between basic numerical and non-numerical cognitive abilities and number-line estimation depends on (i.e., interacts with) gender.

Materials and Methods

Participants

235 Dutch children (105 female; mean age = 7.06 years; SD age = 0.44) in first grade participated. Of this initial sample, 24 were removed from analysis for chance performance on any of the tasks and another 3 were removed for scores on any task that were greater than 4 standard deviations away from the mean. Of the initial sample of 235, 27 were removed (11.5%) for a total analytic sample size of 208 (97 female).

It is important to note that the data reported here are part of a larger data set, some of which has been reported on in previous work (e.g., Lyons et al., 2014). Crucially, both the theoretical questions addressed and the analyses described here are novel.

Procedure

The ethics review board at Maastricht University approved the data collection procedure used in this study. Children came from seven different primary schools in the Netherlands, where data collection took place. The schools provided written notification of the purpose and nature of the data collection procedures to parents. Parents could withhold consent by returning the appropriate form. All data were collected one-on-one by trained project workers at the children’s schools. All data were collected in one session. All measures were computerized with the exception of the non-verbal intelligence measure (Ravens), which was in a paper-and-pencil format. Before each numerical task, participants were given 3–6 practice trials. During the main experimental trials, no feedback was given for any of the tasks.

Primary Tasks of Interest

Number-Line Estimation (NumLine)

In the NumLine task, children were shown a horizontal line with 0 marked on the left side and 100 marked on the right. On each trial, participants saw an Arabic numeral centered above the line and heard the same number over headphones. Their task was to click where on the number-line the target number should be placed based on the quantity it represented. All stimuli remained on the screen until the child responded. Children completed a total of 26 trials. Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.90.

Consistent with previous research on the 0–100 number-line task (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2006), performance on this task was near ceiling for children above first grade in the broader dataset from which this study is drawn. Ceiling-level performance dramatically reduces variability of scores in older children, making individual-differences-based results with this task in older children largely uninterpretable. On the other hand, we did see substantial variability in performance among first graders; this coupled with the observation that meaningful developmental changes are still occurring on this task in first graders (see Introduction) prompted us to focus on first graders for the purposes of the present research.

Numeral Comparison (NumComp)

In the NumComp task, children were shown two Arabic numerals presented horizontally, and their task was to decide which number was greater. A total of 64 trials were presented, comprised of 32 one-digit and 32 two-digit trials. Four ratio (R = min/max) ranges were used: R < = 0.5, R = 0.5, 0.5 < R < 0.7, and R > = 0.7. Each ratio range occurred equally across one- and two-digit trials. All stimuli remained on the screen until the child responded. Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.92.

Dot Comparison (DotComp)

In the DotComp task, children were shown two dot arrays, and their task was to decide which array contained more dots. 64 trials were presented, and quantities and ratios used were identical to those in the NumComp task. Overall area and average individual dot-size were always incongruent with number such that the array with fewer dots always had greater overall area and larger average dot-size. This was done to preclude participants from using strategies based on surface area or dot size to determine which array contained the greater quantity of dots. Additional stimulus details for this task, including manipulation checks, can be found in Lyons et al. (2014). All stimuli remained on the screen until the child responded. Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.92.

Non-verbal Intelligence (Ravens)

The Ravens task is a normed, timed, visuospatial reasoning test for children (Raven et al., 1995). A colored pattern appeared and children were asked to select the missing piece out of six choices. The task was comprised of a total of 36 trials, and the total number answered correctly was the child’s score. Van Bon (1986) reported reliabilities of 0.80 or higher for the Dutch version of this task.

Additional Numerical Tasks of Secondary Interest and Covariates

Numeral Ordering (NumOrd)

In the NumOrd task, children were shown three single-digit Arabic numerals presented horizontally. On half of the trials, the three numbers were in increasing order from left to right. On the other half of trials, numbers were either in decreased or mixed order. Children were instructed to indicate with a button press whether the numbers were in increasing order or not. All stimuli remained on the screen until the child responded. The 28 trials were roughly divided into distances of 1–3. For example, an in-order trial with distance 1 may contain the numbers “4, 5, and 6” whereas an in-order trial with distance 3 may contain the numbers “2, 5, and 8.” Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.82.

Object Matching (ObjMatch)

In the ObjMatch task, children were presented with a sample array of common objects (including animals and fruits) and two test arrays. The children’s task was to select the test array that contained the same number of items as the sample array. A total of 45 trials were shown: in 15 trials, all objects in each of the arrays were the same; in 15 trials, each array contained different types of objects (but the objects within an array were of the same type); and in the remaining 15 trials, each array contained a mixture of object types. The number of objects in the arrays ranged from 1 to 6, and the difference in the number of objects between the two test arrays was 1 or 2. All stimuli remained on the screen until the child responded. Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.92.

Dot Quantity Estimation (DotEst)

In the DotEst task, children saw a single array of dots presented for a very short time (750 ms) – too quickly to be counted individually – followed by a visual mask. The task was to estimate the amount of dots present in the array with a verbal response, which was manually recorded by the experimenter. This task contained a total of 84 trials, made up of 12 trials each with the quantities 1, 2, 3, 4, 7, 11, and 16. Note that results do not substantially change if only quantities 7, 11, and 16 are used. Reliability on this task was acceptable: alpha = 0.76.

Counting (Counting)

In the Counting task, children were presented with between 1 and 9 dots, and their task was to count the number of dots as quickly and accurately as possible. This task contained a total of 45 trials, 5 with each quantity. Children responded verbally, and their responses were manually recorded by the experimenter. Children were instructed to press a button as they gave their response in order to estimate response times. Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.90.

Visual-Audio Matching (VisAud)

In the VisAud task, children heard a number word spoken aloud and were immediately presented with an Arabic number on the screen. The task was to indicate by button press whether the numbers were the same. This task was comprised of 64 trials, half involving one-digit numbers and the other half involving two-digit numbers. On trials in which the numbers did not match, the ratio between the numbers ranged from 0.25 to 0.89. Moreover, non-matching trial stimuli avoided tens-ones confusion items (e.g., 32 and “twenty-three”). Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.90.

Reading Ability (Reading)

The Reading task was part of the Maastricht Dyslexia Differential Diagnosis battery (Blomert and Vaessen, 2009). Children completed three subtasks that contained high-frequency words, low-frequency words, or pseudo-words. For each subtask, participants were shown up to five screens, each with up to 15 items, for a total of 75 items per subtask. Children were tasked with reading each item aloud as quickly and accurately as possible in 30 s. This task was included to control for basic reading fluency in the multiple regression analyses. The Reading score was the total number of words correctly read across each subtask. Test-retest reliability reported for this task is 0.95 (Blomert and Vaessen, 2009).

Basic Stimulus-Response Processing (StimResp)

In the StimResp task, children were presented with four boxes arranged horizontally on the screen. On each trial, a fish appeared in one of the four boxes, and the children’s task was to press the corresponding key on the response box as quickly and accurately as they could. Children completed a total of 20 trials. This task was included to control for basic stimulus-response processing in the multiple regression analyses. All stimuli remained on the screen until the child responded. Reliability on this task was high: alpha = 0.88.

Task Scoring

For the NumLine and the DotEst task, we used percent absolute errors: PAE = | EstTarget| /Scale, where Est is the child’s estimate, Target is the target number, and Scale is the range of target numbers. The range was 100 for the NumLine task and 16 for DotEst. For the NumLine task, note that results were highly similar if degree of linearity (a child’s R2 indicating the linear fit between their estimates and the actual value) was used instead of PAE. A higher value thus indicates poorer performance on these tasks; for this reason, values were multiplied by -1 before being entered into regression models.

For tasks in which error rate and response time data was available (NumComp, DotComp, Counting, NumOrd, VisAud, ObjMatch, and StimResp), we used a composite of error rates and response times on correct trials: P = RT(1 + 2ER), where RT is a child’s mean response-time for that task and ER is the child’s error-rate for that task (Lyons et al., 2014). This was done to account for speed-accuracy tradeoffs and to cut down on the number of analyses required, thus minimizing the risk of Type 1 errors. A higher value thus indicates poorer performance on these tasks; for this reason, values were multiplied by -1 before being entered into regression models.

We used total number of correct responses for both the Ravens and Reading tasks, hence a higher value indicates better performance on these tasks.

Results

Basic Descriptives

Table 1 shows mean performance levels for each task (before multiplying relevant scores by -1), and Figure 1 shows zero-order correlations between all measures (and Age).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptives.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Zero-order correlation matrix. The figure shows zero-order correlations.

Unique Predictors of Number-Line Estimation

We first entered all numerical measures, all non-numerical measures, and a dummy variable for gender (0 = male, 1 = female) into a regression model to predict NumLine performance. Age was also included as a control measure. Table 2 shows results of the initial model, and Figure 2 visualizes relative partial correlation coefficients taken from the multiple-regression model. Results of the initial model show that only NumComp, Ravens, Gender, and Age explain unique variance in NumLine performance.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Initial multiple regression model.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Unique NumLine predictors. The figure shows partial-r values from the initial (full) model (see Table 2) predicting NumLine performance. The vertical line indicates the partial-r value corresponding to p = 0.05.

We next aimed to identify the most parsimonious model possible by removing predictors that failed to predict unique variance in NumLine performance, removing predictors with the lowest p-values in a step-wise fashion until all predictors were significant at p < 0.05. Table 3 shows the progression of model reduction. In the process of model reduction, all predictors were removed with the exception of NumComp, Ravens, and Gender. Because of significant theoretical interest in the DotComp task, we decided to retain it in the final model (shown in Table 4) despite its not predicting unique variance in NumLine performance (indeed, it would have been the second predictor omitted in the process of model reduction). Age was also retained as an important control variable despite not predicting unique variance in NumLine performance.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Progression of model reduction.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Final model details.

Modulation by Gender

In this section, we assessed whether the relations between the predictors of interest retained in the final model (NumComp, DotComp, and Ravens) and NumLine were modulated by (interacted with) gender. To do so, we ran a model predicting NumLine in which we interacted NumComp, DotComp, and Ravens with gender. Results (shown in Table 5) demonstrate a significant NumComp x Gender interaction (p = 0.023). Results did not show a significant interaction with gender for either DotComp or Ravens (both ps > 0.45).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Gender interaction model.

To decompose the significant NumComp x Gender interaction, we next ran multiple-regression models predicting NumLine from NumComp, DotComp, and Ravens, separately by gender. Results (plotted in Figure 3 and shown in Table 6) show that while NumComp was the strongest predictor of NumLine for boys, it did not predict unique NumLine variance for girls. Note that Ravens was a significant predictor for both boys and girls; DotComp was not significant for either.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Final NumLine model predictors by gender. The figure shows partial-r values predicting NumLine, plotted separately for girls (orange) and boys (green). The partial-r values that correspond to p = 0.05 is partial-r = 0.185 for boys (N = 111) and partial-r = 0.200 for girls (N = 97).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Separate models by gender.

Discussion

Across a range of ages and contexts, children’s ability to map numbers into a spatial context has been shown to be a powerful predictor of math performance (Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2008; Sasanguie et al., 2013; Lyons et al., 2014; Friso-van den Bos et al., 2015; Schneider et al., 2018). The goal of the present work was to assess which numerical and non-numerical cognitive abilities predict unique variance in 0–100 number-line estimation ability in first graders. Results indicated that symbolic number processing, but not non-symbolic number processing, predicted unique variance in number-line estimation ability. Moreover, within the realm of symbolic number processing, it was numerical magnitude comparison that was predictive of unique number-line variance, while other symbolic measures, like numeral ordering, did not predict unique variance. The number-line task has been conceptualized as indexing children’s underlying representation of numerical magnitude (Laski and Siegler, 2007; Booth and Siegler, 2008); the present work suggests that number-line estimation is indeed best predicted by measures of numerical magnitude. Crucially, however, our work here indicates that this interpretation is specific to measures of symbolic magnitude representation. Furthermore, results showed that non-verbal reasoning ability also predicted unique variance in number-line estimation, suggesting a role for non-numeric, domain-general cognitive abilities in number-line performance. Interestingly, we also found that the relationship between number-line estimation ability and numeral comparison ability was modulated by gender such that numeral comparison was predictive for boys, but not girls. Our results help clarify the nature of the numerical magnitude representations indexed by number-line estimation tasks in early grade-school. Moreover, as number-lines are a ubiquitous visualization device found in early mathematics classrooms, our results may also point to practical implications for the kinds of basic abilities that permit children to get the most out of this common pedagogical tool.

Recent work has demonstrated that symbolic and non-symbolic representations of quantity are distinct in both adults and young children (Verguts et al., 2005; Carey et al., 2017; Lyons et al., 2012, 2015, 2018). Siegler and colleagues have argued that the number-line task assesses underlying magnitude representations (Laski and Siegler, 2007; Booth and Siegler, 2008), but until this point it hasn’t been explicitly tested whether the theorized underlying magnitude is symbolic or non-symbolic. Our findings show it is symbolic magnitude comparison that predicts unique variance in number-line estimation ability, whereas approximate magnitude comparison does not. If the number-line task reflected the representational precision of non-symbolic quantities (i.e., the width or narrowness of non-symbolic tuning curves), the NumLine task should have shown a strong relation with children’s ability to distinguish between two non-symbolic magnitudes (indexed here via the DotComp task). However, our results indicated this was not the case. Instead, we found that number-line estimation precision is more closely associated with children’s ability to judge the relative magnitudes represented by number symbols (indexed here via the NumComp task). Our results thus clarify an important point with respect to a prominent view of what is indexed by number-line estimation tasks (Siegler and Braithwaite, 2017). Namely, while our results are broadly consistent with the view that number-line tasks primarily index relative magnitude processing (Laski and Siegler, 2007; Booth and Siegler, 2008), here we add the important caveat that the operative notion of magnitude is primarily the symbolic aspect of numerical magnitude.

An important question that follows is what exactly is meant by symbolic numerical magnitude (at least in the present context)? As noted above, recent work has indicated that the meaning of number symbols is likely relatively distinct from approximate magnitudes (Verguts et al., 2005; Lyons et al., 2012, 2015, 2018; Carey et al., 2017), and our results here are broadly consistent with this. In response, some have proposed that number symbols are primarily associative in nature, drawing much of their meaning from associations (such as relative order – ‘What comes next?’) with other number symbols (Nieder, 2009; Núñez, 2017; Lyons and Beilock, 2018). However, in the current context of understanding number-line estimation, this associative aspect of number symbols does not appear to be the critical factor either, as we failed to find that performance on the symbolic number ordering task (NumOrd) predicts unique NumLine variance. An alternative hypothesis proposed by Verguts et al. (2005); see also Roggeman et al. (2007) is that exact representation of numbers (as is thought to be the case with number symbols) operates via ‘place coding.’ Numbers are represented with equal precision regardless of numerical magnitude and indexed based on their relative position on a putative internal mental number-line. Perhaps, most intriguingly here, this mental number-line is typically conceptualized in an explicitly visuospatial manner. If it were the case that, rather than just serving as a useful metaphor, children may actually represent numerical magnitudes by placing numbers along a mental line. In such a framework, the precision with which a given quantity is placed on this mental line should translate directly to the precision with which it is placed on an external line, as in number-line tasks. It may be that first graders rely on this place-based coding to represent symbolic quantities. Hence, this place-based coding may underlie both their ability to compare symbolic magnitudes and generate number-line estimates, as indicated by the strong unique relation between these two tasks we see here.

Here we also found that non-verbal reasoning ability (Ravens) predicted unique variance in number-line performance, suggesting a role for non-numerical cognitive ability in number-line estimation. Previous work on number-line estimation has found individual differences in strategy use (Booth and Siegler, 2008; Slusser et al., 2013; Dackermann et al., 2015; Peeters et al., 2016; van’t Noordende et al., 2016), so a potential interpretation of this relation is that stronger non-verbal reasoning skills may allowing children to select more effective strategies. One practical implication is that future work using the number-line estimation task should take care to control for non-verbal reasoning ability in order to ensure that any claims made about the number-line task are not unknowingly driven by its relation with non-verbal reasoning. From a theoretical perspective, the finding that both numerical magnitude representation and non-verbal reasoning ability each predict unique variance in number-line estimation suggests that both types of ability (numerical and non-numerical) work in conjunction to support effective number-line estimation.

Another result of potential interest here is that the relation between symbolic magnitude comparison and number-line estimation ability was modulated by gender: while this relation obtained for boys (rp = 0.436), it did not for girls (rp = 0.164). Given the preceding discussion, one question is thus why girls did not show a significant relation between NumComp and NumLine performance. Boys consistently show a higher spatial skills on average than girls (Voyer et al., 1995; Kimura, 1999; Terlecki and Newcombe, 2005; Feng et al., 2007). Therefore, one possibility is that, owing to lower general spatial skills, girls are less likely on average than boys to develop an explicitly visuospatial place-coding representation of numerical magnitude, or girls may do so later in development than boys. Consistent with this notion, previous work has found an advantage for boys in number-line estimation (Hutchison et al., 2018). For boys, the number-line task is already cognitively aligned to the spatial manner in which they represent numbers. By contrast, if girls do not primarily represent numbers spatially, there will be an additional cost of translating from a non-spatial representation in order to plot a number in space. Moreover, this putative difference in number representation would also explain the lack of a unique relation between numerical magnitude representation (as indexed by the NumComp task) and number-line performance among girls. If girls do not represent numerical magnitudes spatially, then the ability that allows them to compare symbolic magnitudes would not relate to the ability to plot numbers on a line.

While the idea that boys and girls may vary in the extent to which their representations of numerical magnitudes are spatial in nature is admittedly a post hoc interpretation of our results, it does generate some useful hypotheses that may guide future work. First, it suggests that boys’ performance on a number-line task would be harmed more by visuospatial load or by changing the format of the number line (from horizontal to vertical, for instance) than girls’ performance (controlling for general spatial ability). Second, it may be the case that differences in general spatial ability may explain the gender difference in performance on number-line tasks and the simultaneous absence of gender differences on less explicitly spatial measures of numeracy (Hutchison et al., 2018). Moreover, differences in the extent to which representations of numerical magnitude are spatial may also have an impact on how well children learn about numbers and math from spatial pedagogical strategies (discussed below). Finally, it should be noted that we did not find a gender interaction for the Ravens task, suggesting that non-numerical cognitive abilities – regardless of how symbolic magnitudes are being represented – play a similar role for boys and girls.

In addition to informing theories of number-line estimation and informing debates on broader numerical development, we note that the present work has potential implications for educational settings. Number-lines of course arise not just in the context of the eponymous cognitive task, but they are a common pedagogical tool found in early grade-school classrooms used to promote development of numerical understanding. While experimental work would need to be done to lend greater support to this idea, our work suggests that working to promote children’s understanding of symbolic, rather than non-symbolic, numerical magnitudes may help children get more out of number-lines as a pedagogical tools. Importantly, however, this may be qualified by gender, applying more strongly to male than female children, on average. Finally, the finding that non-verbal reasoning ability predicts number-line estimation ability (regardless of gender) also suggests that children with lower non-numerical reasoning skills may require additional support when using number-lines as pedagogical tools.

Finally, it is important to note the limitations of the present study. First, this study deals with just one number-line range (0–100). This was done for the practical reason that the majority of children in this age range are familiar with two-digit numbers, but not all may be comfortable with three-digit numbers, so using the range 0–100 is perhaps best suited for the majority of students at this age. Second, the data reported here focused on a single age-range (first graders). This was because number-line estimation ability on 0–100 tasks is still developing for children of this age. As such, focusing on this age range and task presented an opportunity to investigate factors that may affect the development of number-line estimation ability. Furthermore, it should be noted that previous work on number-line estimation has shown that findings from different age groups and different number-line ranges have generalized well to one another (e.g., Siegler and Opfer, 2003; Siegler and Booth, 2004; Booth and Siegler, 2008; Siegler and Ramani, 2009). One might argue a third potential limitation is that our findings were biased to show effects of symbolic number comparison over non-symbolic number comparison because the target magnitudes in the number-line task were presented as symbols rather than dot arrays. However, we controlled for several other symbolic measures, including ordering and number-naming, and, it should be noted, none of those predicted unique variance. This suggests that the effect of numeral comparison we found is not merely driven by the fact that it shares a format with the target magnitude.

Conclusion

Our work shows that unique variance in number-line estimation ability is explained by individual differences in symbolic magnitude processing and non-verbal reasoning ability, but not approximate magnitude processing. This finding refines theories of number-line estimation by clarifying that the representations of numerical magnitude tapped by the number-line task appears to be largely symbolic in nature rather than reflecting the degree of representational precision of approximate tuning curves. However, the relation between performance on a symbolic magnitude task and number-line estimation was found to be stronger for boys than girls, potentially due to differences in the degree to which number representations are spatial in nature among boys and girls. This work suggests that promoting children’s understanding of symbolic, rather than non-symbolic, numerical magnitudes may help children learn better from number-lines in the classroom and that future research should treat number-line estimation tasks as reflecting underlying representations of symbolic magnitude.

Author Contributions

Data for the study came from a preexisting dataset. All authors contributed to the conception of the study and writing of the manuscript. RD completed the data analysis.

Funding

This research was supported by funding from the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR), The Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC), Canada Research Chairs Program (CRC) to Daniel Ansari, as well as Departmental Start-Up Funds (Georgetown University) to IL. Data collection costs were paid in part by Boom Test Uitgevers Amsterdam BV.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to Daniel Ansari for providing data access and to Daniel Ansari and Jane Hutchison for helpful comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.

References

Barth, H. C., and Paladino, A. M. (2011). The development of numerical estimation: evidence against a representational shift. Dev. Sci. 14, 125–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00962.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blomert, L., and Vaessen, A. (2009). 3DM Differential Diagnostics for Dyslexia: Cognitive Analysis of Reading and Spelling. Netherlands: Boom Test.

Google Scholar

Booth, J. L., and Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Dev. Psychol. 42:189.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Booth, J. L., and Siegler, R. S. (2008). Numerical magnitude representations influence arithmetic learning. Child Dev. 79, 1016–1031. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01173.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugden, S., and Ansari, D. (2011). Individual differences in children’s mathematical competence are related to the intentional but not automatic processing of Arabic numerals. Cognition 118, 32–44. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, S., Shusterman, A., Haward, P., and Distefano, R. (2017). Do analog number representations underlie the meanings of young children’s verbal numerals? Cognition 168, 243–255. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, D. J., and Blanc-Goldhammer, D. (2011). Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 18, 331–338. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0059-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dackermann, T., Huber, S., Bahnmueller, J., Nuerk, H. C., and Moeller, K. (2015). An integration of competing accounts on children’s number line estimation. Front. Psychol. 6:884. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00884

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense: How the Mind Creates Mathematics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

De Smedt, B., Verschaffel, L., and Ghesquière, P. (2009). The predictive value of numerical magnitude comparison for individual differences in mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 469–479. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., Thompson, C. A., and Siegler, R. S. (2014). Relations of different types of numerical magnitude representations to each other and to mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 123, 53–72. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feigenson, L., Libertus, M. E., and Halberda, J. (2013). Links between the intuitive sense of number and formal mathematics ability. Child Dev. Perspect. 7, 74–79. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, J., Spence, I., and Pratt, J. (2007). Playing an action video game reduces gender differences in spatial cognition. Psychol. Sci. 18, 850–855. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01990.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friso-van den Bos, I., Kroesbergen, E. H., Van Luit, J. E. H., Xenidou-Dervou, I., Jonkman, L. M., Van der Schoot, M., et al. (2015). Longitudinal development of number line estimation and mathematics performance in primary school children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 134, 12–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuhs, M. W., and McNeil, N. M. (2013). ANS acuity and mathematics ability in preschoolers from low-income homes: contributions of inhibitory control. Dev. Sci. 16, 136–148. doi: 10.1111/desc.12013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender-related math attitudes. Sex Roles 66, 153–166. doi: 10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holloway, I. D., and Ansari, D. (2009). Mapping numerical magnitudes onto symbols: the numerical distance effect and individual differences in children’s mathematics achievement. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 17–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchison, J. E., Lyons, I. M., and Ansari, D. (2018). More similar than different: gender differences in children’s basic numerical skills are the exception not the rule. Child Dev. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13044 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Laski, E. V., and Siegler, R. S. (2007). Is 27 a big number? Correlational and causal connections among numerical categorization, number line estimation, and numerical magnitude comparison. Child Dev. 78, 1723–1743.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., and Halberda, J. (2011). Preschool acuity of the approximate number system correlates with school math ability. Dev. Sci. 14, 1292–1300. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01080.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libertus, M. E., Feigenson, L., and Halberda, J. (2013). Is approximate number precision a stable predictor of math ability? Learn. Individ. Differ. 25, 126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Libertus, M. E., Odic, D., and Halberda, J. (2012). Intuitive sense of number correlates with math scores on college-entrance examination. Acta Psychol. 141, 373–379. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, I. M., Ansari, D., and Beilock, S. L. (2012). Symbolic estrangement: Evidence against a strong association between numerical symbols and the quantities they represent. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 141, 635–641. doi: 10.1037/a0027248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, I. M., Ansari, D., and Beilock, S. L. (2015). Qualitatively different coding of symbolic and nonsymbolic numbers in the human brain: neural coding of numbers. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 475–488. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22641

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, I. M., and Beilock, S. L. (2018). Characterizing the neural coding of symbolic quantities. NeuroImage 178, 503–518. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, I. M., and Beilock, S. L. (2011). Numerical ordering ability mediates the relation between number-sense and arithmetic competence. Cognition 121, 256–261. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.07.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, I. M., Bugden, S., Zheng, S., De Jesus, S., and Ansari, D. (2018). Symbolic number skills predict growth in nonsymbolic number skills in kindergarteners. Dev. Psychol. 54, 440–457. doi: 10.1037/dev0000445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lyons, I. M., Price, G. R., Vaessen, A., Blomert, L., and Ansari, D. (2014). Numerical predictors of arithmetic success in grades 1-6. Dev. Sci. 17, 714–726. doi: 10.1111/desc.12152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maertens, B., De Smedt, B., Sasanguie, D., Elen, J., and Reynvoet, B. (2016). Enhancing arithmetic in pre-schoolers with comparison or number line estimation training: does it matter? Learn. Instr. 46, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.08.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merkley, R., and Ansari, D. (2016). Why numerical symbols count in the development of mathematical skills: evidence from brain and behavior. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10, 14–20. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nieder, A. (2009). Prefrontal cortex and the evolution of symbolic reference. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 19, 99–108. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosworthy, N., Bugden, S., Archibald, L., Evans, B., and Ansari, D. (2013). A two-minute paper-and-pencil test of symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical magnitude processing explains variability in primary school children’s arithmetic competence. PLoS One 8:e67918. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067918

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Núñez, R. E. (2017). Is there really an evolved capacity for number? Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 409–424. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opfer, J., and Siegler, R. (2007). Representational change and children’s numerical estimation. Cogn. Psychol. 55, 169–195. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, D., Degrande, T., Ebersbach, M., Verschaffel, L., and Luwel, K. (2016). Children’s use of number line estimation strategies. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 31, 117–134. doi: 10.1007/s10212-015-0251-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peeters, D., Verschaffel, L., and Luwel, K. (2017). Benchmark-based strategies in whole number line estimation. Br. J. Psychol. 108, 668–686. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramani, G. B., and Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Dev. 79, 375–394.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ramani, G. B., Siegler, R. S., and Hitti, A. (2012). Taking it to the classroom: Number board games as a small group learning activity. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 661–672. doi: 10.1037/a0028995

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., and Raven, J. (1995). “Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales”, in Coloured Progressive Matrices, eds J. C. Raven, J. H. Court, and J. Raven (Oxford: Oxford Psychologist Press).

Roggeman, C., Verguts, T., and Fias, W. (2007). Priming reveals differential coding of symbolic and non-symbolic quantities. Cognition 105, 380–394. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rouder, J. N., and Geary, D. C. (2014). Children’s cognitive representation of the mathematical number line. Dev. Sci. 17, 525–536. doi: 10.1111/desc.12166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., Defever, E., and Reynvoet, B. (2012). Association between basic numerical abilities and mathematics achievement: association between basic numerical abilities and mathmatics achievement. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 30, 344–357. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02048.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., De Smedt, B., and Reynvoet, B. (2017). Evidence for distinct magnitude systems for symbolic and non-symbolic number. Psychol. Res. 81, 231–242. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0734-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sasanguie, D., Göbel, S. M., Moll, K., Smets, K., and Reynvoet, B. (2013). Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number–space mappings: What underlies mathematics achievement? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 114, 418–431. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, M., Merz, S., Stricker, J., De Smedt, B., Torbeyns, J., Verschaffel, L., et al. (2018). Associations of number line estimation with mathematical competence: a meta-analysis. Child Dev. 89, 1467–1484. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., and Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Dev. 75, 428–444. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., and Braithwaite, D. W. (2017). Numerical development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 68, 187–213. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., Fazio, L. K., Bailey, D. H., and Zhou, X. (2013). Fractions: the new frontier for theories of numerical development. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., and Opfer, J. E. (2003). The development of numerical estimation: Evidence for multiple representations of numerical quantity. Psychol. Sci. 14, 237–250. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., and Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games—but not circular ones—improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 545–560. doi: 10.1037/a0014239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. S., Thompson, C. A., and Schneider, M. (2011). An integrated theory of whole number and fractions development. Cogn. Psychol. 62, 273–296. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slusser, E. B., Santiago, R. T., and Barth, H. C. (2013). Developmental change in numerical estimation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 142:193–208. doi: 10.1037/a0028560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terlecki, M. S., and Newcombe, N. S. (2005). How important is the digital divide? The relation of computer and videogame usage to gender differences in mental rotation ability. Sex Roles 53, 433–441. doi: 10.1007/s11199-005-6765-0

CrossRef Full Text

Thompson, C. A., and Opfer, J. E. (2008). Costs and benefits of representational change: effects of context on age and sex differences in symbolic magnitude estimation. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 101, 20–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanbinst, K., Ansari, D., Ghesquière, P., and De Smedt, B. (2016). Symbolic numerical magnitude processing is as important to arithmetic as phonological awareness is to reading. PLoS One 11:e0151045. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0151045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Bon, W. H. J. (1986). Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices: Manual of Dutch norms. Netherlands: Swets Test Services.

Google Scholar

van’t Noordende, J. E., van Hoogmoed, A. H., Schot, W. D., and Kroesbergen, E. H. (2016). Number line estimation strategies in children with mathematical learning difficulties measured by eye tracking. Psychol. Res. 80, 368–378. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0736-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verguts, T., Fias, W., and Stevens, M. (2005). A model of exact small-number representation. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 12, 66–80.

Google Scholar

Voyer, D., Voyer, S., and Bryden, M. P. (1995). Magnitude of sex differences in spatial abilities: a meta-analysis and consideration of critical variables. Psychol. Bull. 117, 250–270.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Keywords: number-line estimation, spatial processing, early numeracy, gender differences, number symbols

Citation: Daker RJ and Lyons IM (2018) Numerical and Non-numerical Predictors of First Graders’ Number-Line Estimation Ability. Front. Psychol. 9:2336. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02336

Received: 16 August 2018; Accepted: 07 November 2018;
Published: 30 November 2018.

Edited by:

Sharlene D. Newman, Indiana University Bloomington, United States

Reviewed by:

Koen Luwel, KU Leuven, Belgium
Luis J. Fuentes, Universidad de Murcia, Spain

Copyright © 2018 Daker and Lyons. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ian M. Lyons, ian.lyons@georgetown.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.