Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 07 October 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Achievement Emotions in University Teaching and Learning, Students’ Stress and Well-being View all 15 articles

Differential Effects of Self- vs. External-Regulation on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement, and Satisfaction in Undergraduate Students

  • 1School of Education and Psychology, University of Navarra, Pamplona, Spain
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Almería, Almería, Spain
  • 3School of Psychology, Teesside University, Middlesborough, United Kingdom
  • 4Medical University of the Americas − Nevis, Devens, MA, United States
  • 5University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, NE, United States

The aim of this research was to determine the degree to which undergraduate students’ learning approach, academic achievement and satisfaction were determined by the combination of an intrapersonal factor (self-regulation) and a interpersonal factor (contextual or regulatory teaching). The hypothesis proposed that greater combined regulation (internal and external) would be accompanied by more of a deep approach to learning, more satisfaction and higher achievement, while a lower level of combined regulation would determine a surface approach, less satisfaction and lower achievement. Within an ex post facto design by selection, 1036 university students completed validated questionnaires using an online tool. Several multivariate analyses were conducted. Results showed that the combination of self-regulation and external regulation can be ordered as levels along a five-point scale or heuristic. These levels linearly determine type of learning approach, academic achievement and satisfaction. Implications are established for quality and improvement of the teaching and learning process at university.

Introduction

The analysis of learning approaches, academic achievement and satisfaction at university, as well as their predictive factors, has been a constant in recent research in Educational Psychology (Balloo et al., 2017; Barattucci, 2017). Every university wants its students to experience good learning processes and attain high achievement and satisfaction with the educational experience; these matters impact institutional prestige and social desirability, not to mention their frequent use as criteria for assessing teaching quality (Browne et al., 1998; Elassy, 2015). Moreover, the degree of perceived satisfaction with the university forms part of the rankings that are published annually in many national and international listings (Douglas et al., 2015).

For all of the above, the choice of one model or another to explain academic achievement and the role of learning approaches is highly important in the practice of Educational Psychology at university (Green, 2014; Hazan and Miller, 2017). The present research study seeks to offer an alternative conceptual view, as well as empirical evidence to contribute to an integrated analysis of learning approaches, achievement and academic satisfaction, considering these as variables that depend on both learning and teaching processes in the formal university context (Biggs, 2001; Biggs and Tang, 2011; Barattucci et al., 2017; Kember et al., 2020).

Self-Regulation (SR) and Regulatory Teaching (RT) as Variables of the Teaching and Learning Process: A Heuristic for Analysis

Self-Regulation (SR) has been defined as an intrapersonal (individual) variable that allows people to manage their decisions, making it possible for them to plan, exercise control over such decisions, and evaluate their effects (Brown, 1998). In psychology research on health and academic well-being, SR has been considered a variable at the molecular level (de la Fuente et al., 2019a). It is predictive of various specific regulatory behaviors, such as coping strategies (de la Fuente et al., 2019b) or achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2020b, c). In the realm of educational psychology, it has been conceptualized as a meta-behavioral, student variable (presage), predictive of Self-Regulated Learning (process variable), achievement and academic satisfaction (product variables). Previous research has consistently established these relationships (Dinsmore et al., 2008; Kaplan, 2008; Antonelli et al., 2020). Thus, self-regulation (SR) as a personal variable may be considered a precursor to Self-Regulated Learning (SRL) (de la Fuente et al., 2008, 2015b).

Regulatory teaching (RT) has been defined as a contextual variable, referring to the degree to which the teaching process promotes and externally favors students’ SRL. It has been conceptualized as a meta-instructional variable; regulatory teaching encourages self-regulation in students and is characteristic of effective teaching. There have been many approaches to effective teaching in the research (for a review, see Goe et al., 2008; Baeten et al., 2013; Karagiannopoulou and Milienos, 2015). Empirical research identifies high quality teachers as those who positively influence their students’ engagement with learning activities, as well as students’ performance in learning (self-regulation, social competencies, academic achievement). Mediating factors in student performance must be considered (Roehrig and Chistesen, 2010): (1) Organization of the content and activities; (2) Planning for the majority of the class; (3) Encouraging deep processing and self-regulation. Recent research has shown that variables of the perceived classroom learning environment were good predictors of students’ self-regulation. Moreover, teacher variables (effective teaching) were found to be directly related to students’ self-regulation, and there were moderate relationships between learning environment and self-regulation variables (Kossak, 2019; Yerdelen and Sungur, 2019).

The theory of Self-Regulated vs. Externally Regulated Learning, SRL vs. ERL (de la Fuente, 2017) has attempted to identify and organize the different real-life combinations that result from the interaction between different types of university students and teachers (Azevedo et al., 2008). Specifically, this theory suggests that during any teaching-learning process, we find different levels of student self-regulation (low-medium-high) in combination with different levels (low-medium-high) of regulatory teaching. Consequently, a heuristic with five possible combination ranks has been put forward (see Table 1). This heuristic of combinations has been successfully evaluated in reference to the effect of its regulation levels on university students’ achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2019b) and their coping strategies (de la Fuente et al., 2020b). However, its effect on learning approaches, satisfaction and achievement has yet to be reported, and this is the aim of the present research study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Heuristic of five combinations of the Utility ModelTM hypothesized by SRL vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017).

The Vermunt model (Vermunt, 1998; Vermunt and van Rijswijk, 1988), similar in part to SRL vs. ERL Theory (op cit., 2017), distinguished between three different strategies of regulation: self-regulation, external regulation and lack of regulation of learning (Lindblom-Ylänne et al., 2011):

(1) Self-Regulated Learning: referring to what students do to plan and monitor their learning activities, diagnose the cause of any problems that occur while learning, and progress toward the learning goals they have set for themselves. This definition is similar to other definitions or theories of learning, such as Winne (1995), Winne and Hadwin (1997), or Zimmerman (1998, 2000), who defined self-regulated learning as the systematic effort to direct one’s thoughts, feelings and actions toward meeting academic goals. Biggs (1985) used the term meta-learning to describe the state of being aware of and exercising self-control over one’s own learning.

Self-Regulated Learning vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017) uses an identical concept of SRL, while also assuming that a prior variable (SR) may be what determines the level of SRL during learning. Self-regulated learning is assumed to be present at three levels: adequate, non-existent and low.

(2) External Regulation of Learning: External regulation refers to situations where students depend on a teacher’s guidance and control (or a text book, or classmates) to regulate learning processes. In this model, the teacher takes on the regulatory activities of the students.

In SRL vs. ERL Theory (2017), however, external regulatory actions are designed to assist and promote students’ internal self-regulation–not to exercise external control over them. As such, this type of external regulation may be present at three levels: high or adequate, non-existent, or low. The concept identified as external regulation in Vermunt’s model would be considered a dysregulatory context in the SRL vs. ERL model, because it encourages a lack of internal self-regulation.

(3) Lack of regulation: This refers to certain students’ difficulty in regulating their own learning processes. In the SRL vs. ERL model (2017), these students’ level of self-regulation would be categorized as non-regulatory or dysregulatory.

Learning Approaches (LA) as a Variable in the Teaching and Learning Process

The SAL model, Student Approaches to Learning (Marton, 1976; Biggs, 1979; Entwistle et al., 1979; Richardson, 2015; Fryer and Ginns, 2017) established the concept of learning approaches (deep vs. surface) as a student variable, with a great amount of empirical evidence (for a review, see Asikainen and Gijbels, 2017). Biggs (1988) defined learning approaches as learning processes that emerge from students’ perceptions of academic tasks, influenced by their personal characteristics. Learning approaches are characterized by the influence of metacognitive processes as a mediating element between the students’ intention or motive and the learning strategy they use in order to study. Biggs indicated two different levels of study in approaches to learning: one is more specific and directed toward a concrete task (a surface approach seen as a process used to pass exams) and the other is more general (a deep approach seen as the motivation to understand). Previous research has associated this variable of learning approaches with learning conceptions (Monroy and González-Geraldo, 2018), with motivational-affective and personal factors (Trigwell et al., 2012; de la Fuente et al., 2013b; Cetin, 2015; Karagiannopoulou et al., 2018), and even with lifelong learning (Barros et al., 2012).

Although fewer in number, other studies have reported its relationship to the teaching process (Vermunt, 1998; Marton et al., 2005; Ruohoniemi et al., 2010). Nonetheless –based on the original conceptualization of this construct– it seems plausible that students’ learning approaches depend on both intra-subject (individual) factors and between-subject (contextual) factors, considering that the nature of the variable is quite subjective, sensitive to diverse influences that stem from the student’s own characteristics as well as from the teacher and from the teaching context (for a review, see Vermunt and Donche, 2017). Consequently, if we assume that the teaching process –teaching approach– affects and has a determining influence on how the student learns –learning approach– (Trigwell et al., 1999), especially in formal contexts, then approaches to learning becomes a variable within the teaching-learning process, not something that pertains only to the student who is learning (Entwistle and Ramsden, 1983; Vermunt and Verloop, 1999; Entwistle et al., 2002, 2003; Vermunt, 2007; Entwistle, 2009, 2018; Parpala et al., 2010; Biggs and Tang, 2011; Baeten et al., 2015). This approach, however, has not been addressed as much as one would expect. In the words of certain authors: “Thus, the effect of the teaching-learning environment is not taken into account so much, despite the largely accepted theoretical assumption in the SAL tradition that students’ approaches to learning are not stable but change as a result of the interaction between the contextual aspects of the learning environment and the characteristics of the learners” (Asikainen and Gijbels, 2017; p. 228). The present study, therefore, adopts this more comprehensive view of student approaches to learning, in the context of teaching and learning processes.

Academic Achievement and Academic Satisfaction as Variables of the Teaching and Learning Process

Academic Achievement as a Variable of the Teaching and Learning Process

The classic psychological view of analyzing academic achievement has sought to assess the relative weight of students’ individual psychological factors of different types, observing the weight of personal variables, cognitive variables, and motivational-affective variables, as well as others that are psychosocial or contextual (for a meta-analysis review, see Richardson et al., 2012). The educational psychology perspective has led researchers to establish the role of individual psychological factors within a contextualized, specific learning process. There is a great amount of recent research in this regard (Wibrowski et al., 2016; Köller et al., 2019; Nabizadeh et al., 2019), with marked influence from the satisfaction variable, a variable of positive experience and emotionality, in the academic setting (Vanno et al., 2014).

As in the case of learning approaches (LA), there have also been efforts to contextualize achievement within the teaching and learning process (Vermunt, 1998; Biggs and Tang, 2011; Scevak et al., 2015). This approach assumed that academic achievement is determined by variables from both the teaching process and the learning process–taken in combination. In other words, it is not only a matter of the student’s individual variables. Nonetheless, the prevailing view has been to emphasize student variables, assuming that the teaching process has a contextualized role with lesser weight. While this view, which leans heavily toward factors of the learner and is not interrelational with the teaching process, may be adequate in an individual context of learning, it seems unfitted to explaining phenomena in a formal teaching-learning context. Hence, while it is true that certain studies have analyzed the role of effective teaching factors in the process of learning and achievement (de la Fuente et al., 2017), a systematic demonstration of the possible combinations of students’ learning characteristics and the teacher’s teaching characteristics is yet to be established. Some prior studies have taken this direction, with encouraging results (de la Fuente et al., 2011). Fewer research studies have documented the role of the teaching process as a contributing factor to university students’ academic achievement, despite the fact that most universities assess students’ degree of satisfaction with the teaching process either explicitly or implicitly (Douglas et al., 2015).

Academic achievement as a variable has been conceptualized differently. Its classic conceptualization is that of grade point average. Today’s model of achievement, however, is based on the concept of competence acquisition (Gagné, 1965) and has prompted consideration of academic achievement as a multidimensional variable that includes acquisitions that are conceptual (facts, concepts and principles), procedural (skills and meta-skills), and attitudinal (attitudes, values, and habits) (Roe, 2003; de la Fuente et al., 2004).

Academic Satisfaction as a Variable of the Teaching and Learning Process

Academic satisfaction with the teaching-learning process has been conceptualized as the emotional or attitudinal element of achievement (Biggs, 2001); it addresses the degree that students’ expectations are met, and how well the process responds to their needs. This variable has been repeatedly considered as an element reflecting the quality of the experience. For example, Bobe and Cooper (2017) defined the category of student satisfaction with the experience using five components: teaching quality, learner engagement, learning resources, student support, and skills development. In their sample, Rubin et al. (2018) found that older female students showed the most deep learning, and this effect explained their greater satisfaction with their degree program.

Increasing importance is being given to degree satisfaction (or student satisfaction) for at least two reasons. First, satisfaction predicts student persistence (for a review, see Schertzer and Schertzer, 2004); low satisfaction is an early sign of potential student attrition. Second, satisfaction is a key factor in the rankings of universities, which are commonly used in marketing and funding exercises. Previous findings have shown an association between a deep learning approach and greater satisfaction with teaching and learning environments and methods (Parpala et al., 2010; Gurpinar et al., 2013). Thus, the present study seeks to further our understanding of academic satisfaction, conceptualized as the result of a combination of personal and contextual factors pertaining to the process of teaching and learning.

Aims and Hypotheses

Based on prior theoretical foundations and previous empirical research, the following objectives were identified: (1) to establish whether the university students’ regulation levels (intrapersonal variable) and the regulatory levels of the teaching received (contextual variable), independently, determined their type of learning approach and their academic achievement and satisfaction; (2) to determine whether these levels taken jointly, as described in the combination model proposed by the theory, were associated with the type of learning approach used, academic achievement and satisfaction. Based on these objectives, our hypotheses established that: (1) a graded increase in level of regulation (internal and external) would give rise to an increase in deep learning approach, and a decrease in surface approach; by contrast, a graded decrease in level of regulation (internal and external) would give rise to an increase in surface learning approach and a decrease in deep approach; (2) a graded increase in level of regulation (internal and external) would give rise to an proportionate increase in total achievement and in its three subtypes (conceptual, procedural, and attitudinal), and in satisfaction; a graded decrease in level of regulation (internal and external) would give a proportionate decrease in total achievement and in its three subtypes (conceptual, procedural, and attitudinal) and satisfaction.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total sample of 1036 undergraduate students from two universities of Spain participated in this research. The sample was composed of students enrolled in degree programs in Psychology and Education (Primary Education); 65.7% were women and 34.3% were men. Their ages ranged from 19 to 25, with a mean age of 21.33 (σx = 6.9) years.

Instruments

Self-Regulation

This variable was measured using the Short Self-Regulation Questionnaire (SSRQ) (Brown, 1998; Brown et al., 1999). It has already been validated in Spanish samples (Pichardo et al., 2014; Garzón-Umerenkova et al., 2017). The SSRQ is composed of four factors and 17 items with a consistent confirmatory factor structure (Chi-Square = 250.83, df = 112, CFI = 0.95, GFI = 0.94, AGFI = 0.96, RMSEA = 0.059). It has acceptable validity and reliability values as measured by Cronbach’s alpha [total (α = 0.86; Omega = 0.843); goal setting-planning (α = 0.79; Omega = 0.784), perseverance (α = 0.78; Omega = 0.779), decision making (α = 0.72; Omega = 0.718), and learning from mistakes (α = 0.72; Omega = 0.722)], similar to the English version. Sample items include: “I usually keep track of my progress toward my goals,” “When it comes to deciding about a change, I feel overwhelmed by the choice,” and “I learn from my mistakes.”

Regulatory Teaching

The Scales for Assessment of the Teaching-Learning Process, ATLP, student version (de la Fuente et al., 2012) were used to evaluate the perception of the teaching process in students. The scale entitled Regulatory Teaching is Dimension 1 of the confirmatory model. IATLP-D1 comprises 29 items structured along five factors: Specific regulatory teaching, regulatory assessment, preparation for learning, satisfaction with the teaching, and general regulatory teaching. The scale showed a factor structure with adequate fit indices (Chi-Square = 590.626; df = 48, p < 0.001, CF1 = 0.958, TLI = 0.959, NFI = 0.950, NNFI = 0.967; RMSEA = 0.068) and adequate internal consistency [IATLP 1 Scale (α = 0.830; Omega = 0.821), and the subscales: Specific regulatory teaching (α = 0.897; Omega = 0.852); regulatory assessment (α = 0.883; Omega = 0.876); preparation for learning (α = 0.849; Omega = 0.835); satisfaction with the teaching, (α = 0.883; Omega = 0.861), and general regulatory teaching, (α = 0.883; Omega = 0.858)]. Sample items include: “While we are learning the teacher help us to make clear realistic learning goals,” “The teacher explains the objetives of activities we are going to carry out,” or “The teacher make the class enjoyable.”

Learning Approaches

This was measured with the Revised Two-Factor Study Process Questionnaire, R-SPQ-2F (Biggs et al., 2001), in its Spanish validated version (Justicia et al., 2008). It contains 20 items on four subscales (deep motive, deep strategy; surface motive, surface strategy), measuring two dimensions: deep and surface learning approaches, respectively. Students respond to these items on a 5-point Likert-type scale ranging from 1 (rarely true of me) to 5 (always true of me). In the present study Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficients were acceptable: Deep (α = 0.793; Omega = 0.782); Surface (α = 0.751; Omega = 0.721). Sample items include: “I find that at times studying gives me a feeling of deep personal satisfaction,” “My aim is to pass the course while doing as little work as possible,” “I find that studying academic topics can at times be as exciting as a good novel or movie.”

Academic Achievement

Assessment of achievement was based on the academic-professional competency model (Roe, 2003). Total achievement was measured as the final grade given to the student for the subject, on a scale of 1 to 10. The 10 points are a compendium of results obtained on the three levels of subcompetencies: (1) Conceptual scores: these include all scores obtained on exams covering the conceptual content of the subject (4 points); (2) Procedural scores: assessed from the student’s practical work involving procedural content and skills (4 points); (3) Attitudinal scores: scores given for class participation, and for doing optional activities to reach a better understanding of the material (2 points). In the latter case, there were 10 class activities that were turned in at the end of class; the mean of the 10 scores obtained was converted proportionately to a score on the 0−2 point range. Since the three subcompetencies were measured on different ranges (0−4 points, 0−2 points), their scores were converted to an equivalent scale from 1 to 10 in order to perform the different analyses and compare the results.

Satisfaction With Teaching and Learning

The Scales for Interactive Assessment of the Teaching-Learning Process, IATLP, student version (de la Fuente et al., 2012) were used to evaluate students’ perception of the teaching process. The scale entitled Satisfaction of teaching and Learning is Dimension 3 of the confirmatory model (IATLP-D3). This sub-scale comprises 10 items structured along two factors. The scale was validated in university students and showed a factor structure with adequate fit indices (Chi-Square = 590.626; df = 48, p < 0.001, CF1 = 0.938, TLI = 0.939, NFI = 0.950, NNFI = 0.967; RMSEA = 0.058) and adequate internal consistency [IATLP D3 (α = 0.85; Omega = 0.831); Satisfaction with learning process (α = 0.86; Omega = 0.831); and Satisfaction with teaching process (α = 0.87; Omega = 0.861)]. Sample items include: “I am satisfied with the way my teacher has carried out the teaching” and “I am satisfied with the way I have learned.”

Procedure

Students voluntarily completed the scales using an online platform (de la Fuente et al., 2015a). A total of fifteen specific teaching-learning processes were evaluated, each pertaining to a specific university subject that was taught within a 2-year academic period. Presage variables (Self-regulation, SR) were evaluated in September-October of 2017 and of 2018, Process variable (learning approaches, LA) in February-March of 2017 and of 2018, and Product variables (regulatory teaching, satisfaction with teaching and learning process, and academic achievement) in May-June of 2017 and of 2018. Achievement was reported by the teacher, based on the academic grades that students obtained at the end of the school year. In all cases, scores had been assigned for the three types of subcompetencies (conceptual, procedural, and attitudinal). Cases were eliminated if any of these scores were lacking.

At each university, teachers were invited to participate in the research project; once they agreed, they in turn invited the participation of their students. Each group of students evaluated only one teacher and the teaching-learning process of one full-year academic subject. The teachers and students received a certificate acknowledging their hours of participation in the project. In no case was any academic credit given for participation. The procedure was approved by the respective Ethics Committees at each university, in the context of the two R & D Projects (see Funding).

Data Analysis

Design

An ex post facto design was used. There was no intervention of any kind in the teaching-learning processes assessed. Only pre-existing variables were evaluated.

Previous Analysis

Preliminary analyzes were carried out to detect different problems in the sample data. About the potential outliers in the data, univariate outliers were identified by checking standardized scores on any variables which were outside the absolute value of 3.29 (Tabachnick and Fidell, 2013). Complementary, to detect multivariate outliers, Mahalanobis distance (MD) for the predictor variables were used, which is the distance of a data point from the centroid shaped by the cloud of the majority of data points (Mahalanobis, 1930). In this process, 21 cases were eliminated. Regarding the reliability of the scales used, the omegaH index has been recalculated; for those multidimensional variables, it is essential to provide model-based reliability (for both general factor and specific sub-factors) rather than simply reporting Alpha (Reise et al., 2012).

Operationalization of Self- vs. External- Regulation

Using cluster analysis, continuous independent variables were converted into discrete, dependent variables, producing three levels (low-medium-high) for self-regulation and regulatory teaching, respectively. The centroids of low, medium, and high scoring groups were calculated in each variable. Next, we determined the cutoff points between scores. In this way, we established the score ranges for low (L), medium (M), and high (H) (see Table 1, on the left, in boldface).

Inferential Analyses

Different ANOVAs and MANOVAs were carried out, taking high/medium/low levels of SR and RT as independent variables. First, we performed 3 × 1 (simple) and 3 × 3 (cross) analyses.

A Heuristic of Regulation Combinations for the Teaching and Learning Process

Finally, the MANOVA (5 × 1) showed statistically significant differences in the levels of variables SR and RT among the five groups, showing them to be adequately configured. This procedure was similar to that used in other previous reports (de la Fuente et al., 2019b, p. 12; de la Fuente et al., 2020a, p. 5). The multivariate analyses (MANOVAs) showed a statistically significant main effect of the five combination types on low-medium-high levels of SR and of RT (see Table 1):

Combination 1 presented a statistically significant low level in SR and low level in RT (1 and 1 levels). The effects are a high level of surface approach, low level of deep approach, low level of achievement, and low level of satisfaction.

Combination 2 had a statistically significant low level in SR and medium level in RT, or viceversa (1 and 2, or 2 and 1 levels). The effects are a medium-high level of surface approach, medium-low level of deep approach, medium-low level of achievement, and medium-low level of satisfaction.

Combination 3 presented a statistically significant medium SR level (2) and medium RT level (2 and 2 levels). The effects are a medium level of surface approach, medium level of deep approach, medium level of achievement, and medium level of satisfaction.

Combination 4 had a statistically significant medium level in SR and high level in RT, or viceversa (2 and 3, or 3 and 2 levels). The effects are a medium-high level of deep approach, medium-low level of surface approach, medium-high level of achievement, and medium-high level of satisfaction.

Combination 5 presented statistically significant high SR and high RT (3 and 3 levels). The average regulation level is 3.0, and its regulation rank is 5. The effects are a high level of deep approach, low level of surface approach, high level of achievement, and high level of satisfaction.

The proposed five-combination heuristic enables us to analyze all the most common combinations found in the interactive regulation of teaching-learning processes. A regulation average is obtained from the student-teaching interaction by calculating the mean of the student’s regulation level and the regulation level of the teaching process. For example, if the student has a low level of regulation (1 point), and the teaching offers a medium level of regulation (2 points), the resulting regulation average will be 1.5 points (2 + 1 = 3/2 = 1.5 point average). Inversely, a student with medium regulation (2 points) and a teaching process low in regulation (1 point) would result in the same regulation average (2 + 1 = 3/2 = 1.5 average points). In another case, if a student has a high level of regulation (3 points) and interacts with teaching that is low in regulation (1 point), the regulation average will be 2 points (3 + 1 = 4/2 = 2 points). The student-teaching interaction increases from the least favorable to the most favorable: the minimum combination of low student regulation (1 point) with teaching low in regulation (1 point), to a maximum combination of high student regulation (3 points) with highly regulatory teaching (3 points). The heuristic then orders all the possible combinations according to their regulation average, assigning to them a regulation rank (regulation average of 1 = rank 1; regulation average of 1.5 = rank 2; regulation average of 2 = rank 3; regulation average of 2.5 = rank 4; regulation average of 3 = rank 5).

Results

Interdependent Effects of Levels of Personal Self-Regulation (SR) and Levels of Regulatory Teaching (RT) on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement, and Satisfaction

Effects on Dimensions and Factors of Learning Approaches (LA)

There was a statistically significant main effect of Self-Regulation (SR) on the two dimensions of learning approach (LA): Deep Approach and Surface Approach. The effect of SR (low-medium-high levels) was statistically significant for both deep approach and surface approach. A higher level of SR determined a higher level of deep approach and a lower level of surface approach. Complementarily, a lower level of SR determined a lower level of deep approach and a higher level of surface approach. See Table 2 (first part of the table, on the left).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Simple interdependent relations of low-medium-high levels of Self-Regulation (SR) and of Regulatory Teaching (RT), as independent variables, on Learning Approaches (n = 1209).

Complementarily, there was a statistically significant main effect of SR (low-medium levels) on learning approach factors. The partial effect of SR (low-medium-high levels) was statistically significant for the factors of deep motivation, deep strategy, surface motivation, and surface strategy. A higher level of SR determined a higher level of the factors deep motivation and deep strategy, and a lower level of surface motivation and surface strategy. A lower level of SR determined the opposite case, that is, a lower level of deep motivation and deep strategy, and a higher level of surface motivation and surface strategy. See Table 2 (first part of the table, on the left) and Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Combined effects (3 × 3) between levels of Self-Regulation (SR) and levels of Regulatory Teaching (RT) on Learning Approches (n = 972).

There was a statistically significant main effect of Regulatory Teaching (RT) (low-medium-levels) on Learning Approach dimensions. The partial effect of RT (low-medium-high levels) was statistically significant for both deep approach (DA) and surface approach (SA). Thus, a higher level in regulatory teaching determined a higher level in DA and a lower level in SA; by contrast, a lower level in RT determined a higher level in SA and lower level in DA. Complementarily, there was a statistically significant main effect of RT (low-medium levels) on learning approach factors. The partial effect of RT (low-medium-high levels) was statistically significant for the factors of deep motivation (DM), deep strategy (DS), surface motivation (SM), and surface strategy (SS). Accordingly, a higher level of RT determined a higher level of DM and DS, and lower levels of SM and SS. By contrast, lower levels of RT determined higher levels of SM and SS and lower levels of DM and DS. See Table 2 (first part of the table, on the right) and Table 3.

Effects on Academic Achievement (ACH) and Satisfaction (SAT)

There was a statistically significant main effect of SR (low-medium levels) on total academic achievement (ACH). A higher level of SR determined a higher total achievement score, and a lower level did the opposite. Complementarily, there was a statistically significant main effect of SR (low-medium levels) on the ACH factors. The partial effect of SR (low-medium-high levels) was statistically significant for the factors of conceptual achievement, procedural achievement, and attitudinal achievement. In other words, a higher score in SR determined a higher level in the three types of achievement. Complementarily, there was a statistically significant effect of SR (low-medium levels) on academic satisfaction (SAT). In similar fashion, a higher level of SR determined a higher level of SAT, and a lower level did the opposite. See Table 2 (second part, on the left), Table 3, and Figures 1, 2.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Graphic representation of the effect of levels in the IV Self-Regulation (GRUPSR: Low = 1; Medium = 2; High = 3) and level in the IV Regulatory Teaching (GRUPRT: Low = 1; Medium = 2; High = 3) on Learning Approaches (LA). DEEP, Deep approach; SURFACE, Surface approach; DEEPMOT, Deep motivation; DEEPSTRAT, Deep strategies; SURFMOT, Surface motivation; SURFSTRAT, Surface strategies.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Graphic representation of the effect of low(1)-medium(2)-high(3) levels in the IV self-regulation (GRUPSRQ) and low(1)-medium (2)-high (3) levels in the IV regulatory teaching (GRUPER) on academic achievement (conceptual, procedural, and attitudinal) and satisfaction with the teaching and learning process.

There was a statistically significant main effect of RT (low-medium levels) on total ACH. A higher level in RT determined a higher level in ACH. Complementarily, there was a statistically significant main effect of RT (low-medium levels) on the ACH factors. The partial effect of RT (low-medium-high levels) was statistically significant for the factors of conceptual achievement, procedural achievement, and attitudinal achievement. A high level of RT, therefore, was a determinant of higher levels in all three types of achievement. Complementarily, a statistically significant effect of RT (low-medium-levels) was noted in academic satisfaction. Thus, a higher level of RT determined a higher level of SAT. See Table 2 (second part), Table 3, and Figures 1, 2.

It is important to emphasize that interaction effects between SR and RT were not produced, but main effects from each variable independently, making an additive effect. The following section documents this summative effect using the combination heuristic.

Combination Heuristic of SR vs. ER: Understanding Its Effect on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement, and Satisfaction

Effects of the Combination Heuristic on Learning Approaches

A statistically significant main effect of the five combinations of SR and RT was observed in learning approaches (LA). In the dimensions of deep approach (DA) [5 > 4 > 3 > 2,1] and surface approach (SA) [1,2 > 3 > 4,5], a significant statistical effect also appeared, but in opposing directions. These results show that higher levels of the heuristic combination determined higher levels of DA and lower levels of SA; by contrast, lower levels of the combination heuristic determined lower levels of DA and higher levels of SA. See Figure 1 and Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Effects of the Five Types of Combinations on Learning Approaches (LA) and Academic Achievement and Satisfaction.

The statistically significant partial effect was maintained for each factor: deep motivation (DM) [5,4 > 3,2 > 1] and deep strategies (DS) [5,4 > 3,2 > 1], surface motivation (SM) [1,2 > 3 > 4,5], and surface strategies (SS) [1,2 > 3 > 4,5]. High levels of the heuristic determined high levels in DM and DS, as well as low levels in SM and SS; however, low levels of the heuristic determined low levels in DM and DS, as well as high levels in SM and SS. See Figure 3 and Table 4. A graphic representation of the differential progressive effect of the combinations of SR and RT levels is shown in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Graphical representation of the effects of the combination types (heuristic levels 1–5) on learning approaches.

Effects of the Combination Heuristic on Academic Achievement and Satisfaction

A statistically significant main effect of the five combinations of IVs SR and RT was noted on total achievement [5 > 4, 3 > 2,1]. The statistically significant partial effect was maintained for each factor: conceptual achievement [5,4 > 3,2 > 1], procedural achievement [5 > 4 > 3,2 > 1], and attitudinal achievement [5,4 > 3 > 2,1]. Complementarily, a statistically significant main effect of the five combinations of the IVs SR and RT was noted on satisfaction [5 > 4 > 3 > 2 > 1]. See Figure 4 and Table 4. A graphic representation of the differential progressive effect of the combinations of SR and RT levels is shown in Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Graphical representation of the effects of the combination types (heuristic levels 1–5) on academic achievement and satisfaction.

Discussion

Implications for the Knowledge of This Research Topic

Effects on Learning Approaches

Self-Regulated Learning vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017) predicted that university students’ learning approaches, academic achievement and satisfaction could be determined, jointly, by the students’ degree of self-regulation (SR) and by the level of contextual or external regulation (RT). Furthermore, this type of interaction could be understood by the combination of low-medium-high levels of the two factors (SR and RT), as supported by prior evidence in this direction (de la Fuente et al., 2017).

With respect to the first hypothesis, the evidence found upholds the theory that a surface vs. deep learning approach is a student-dependent variable, depending on the student’s pre-existing level of self-regulation (Heikkilä and Lonka, 2006; de la Fuente et al., 2008). Interestingly, however, other novel data presented here have shown that a high level of SR more strongly determines the level of deep motivation, but not so much the level of deep strategies, and viceversa, a low level of SR determines a greater number of surface strategies. These differentiating details had not been clearly established to date, and have implications for assessment and improved psychoeducational intervention – to be further discussed below. This result is consistent with the evidence showing that excellent students have a higher level of deep approach in comparison to average students (Gargallo et al., 2015).

In complementary fashion, a high level of RT (regulatory or effective teaching) has been shown to promote a greater degree of the deep learning approach; a low level of RT promotes a surface learning approach. Moreover, a differentiating effect was found, where highly regulatory teaching was clearly seen to have a greater effect on deep motivation than on deep strategy, while low regulatory teaching has more effect on surface strategy than on surface motivation. In other words, good (regulatory) teaching encourages motivation more than high-level cognitive processes, while less regulatory teaching (non-regulatory or dysregulatory) seems to lead to poorer cognitive processes, and learning processes per se, more than it affects surface motivation. This effect is novel, and seems to allude to a differential effect of teaching on cognitive and motivational processes, which must be analyzed in greater depth.

From our point of view, however, the most interesting effect found here is the effect produced by the combination of student characteristics and characteristics of the teaching process, in determining university students’ learning approach. All the cross analyses and especially the heuristic-based analyses themselves (graded combinations 1−5), have consistently supported our combination hypothesis, with reference to university students’ learning approaches. In general, there are several research reports that confirm this, in the case of achievement emotions (de la Fuente et al., 2019b), and coping strategies of stress (de la Fuente et al., 2020a). Although learning approaches depend on individual characteristics, they are also fed by characteristics of the teaching process (Howie and Bagnall, 2013), especially in formal teaching-learning contexts at university, an aspect that Biggs (2001) had suggested and which has received consistent empirical support in other recent research reports (Lodewyk et al., 2009; Kember et al., 2020).

Effects on Academic Achievement and Satisfaction

Regarding the second hypothesis, results allowed us to reject the null hypothesis, since both the established independent variables (SR level and ER level) and their combinations determined levels of total achievement and the sub-types of achievement, as well as determining satisfaction with the teaching-learning experience. This combination effect has already been seen in similar fashion in other previous samples (de la Fuente et al., 2017; Moghimi et al., 2020; Paloş et al., 2020), though the greater effect of the combination on procedural achievement (practical performance subcompetencies: practical problem solving) is a novel finding. By comparison, the greatest effect was seen in total and conceptual achievement, and was determined by regulatory teaching. One plausible explanation for this result is that the regulatory component (times, materiales, learning aids, strategies, meaningful assessment, etc.) is ultimately materialized in better conceptual learning. Notwithstanding, these specific aspects are worthy of further attention and should be clarified in future research.

Limitations and Future Lines of Research

An initial limitation to this study refers to the sample. Given that the sample is not a heterogeneous group from different disciplines and degree programs, the results should be interpreted with caution. Prior research has shown that whether a teacher’s approach encourages self-regulation, offers external regulation or is lacking in regulation, is dependent on the degree program and the teaching styles of different departments (Kreber et al., 2005; Lindblom-Ylänne et al., 2011). In addition, the concepts of teaching regulation presented in the Approaches to Teaching Inventory (Trigwell and Prosser, 2004; Cao et al., 2019) and the concept used in this research on Regulatory Teaching are not identical. The former focuses more on an analysis of teaching style, looking at transmission and conceptual change, in order to verify the learning style that it promotes (surface vs. deep approach), while the latter seeks to evaluate whether teaching promotes self-regulation strategies in the students, and thereby affects their learning approach. Future research must accurately establish the relationship between the two concepts of regulation in teaching, as well as the relationship between the teacher’s own self-regulation characteristics and his or her implementation of regulatory teaching (Randi, 2004; Capa-Aydin et al., 2009).

One important limitation of this study is that the assessment system consists exclusively of student self-reports. However, a strength of this study is that both self-assessment (self-regulation, learning approaches, satisfaction) and contextual assessment (regulatory teaching) were included. Nonetheless, future research studies should incorporate complementary assessment systems (Goe et al., 2008; Entwistle and Karagiannopoulou, 2014).

Finally, we are limited in identifying implications for different cultural contexts, because there may be cultural differences in self-regulation, regulatory teaching, and in the relationship between these two variables. Prior research has brought this factor to light, as part of understanding regulation processes (Trommsdorff, 2012; Jaramillo et al., 2017).

Implications for the Practice of Educational Psychology

These results are of great interest to research and professional practice, allowing us to reconceptualize certain prior evidence and the evaluation of teaching and learning processes at university.

First, there are two important implications for research in this topic. On the one hand, these consistent and recurring results (some of them reported previously in this Research Topic) indicate the value of analyzing the student’s level of regulation and the level of regulatory teaching in combination, for determining hypothetical levels of cognitive variables, emotional variables, coping and the emotional states of engagement-burnout at university (de la Fuente et al., 2017, 2019b, 2020a, b). These results thus provide empirical support to SRL vs. ERL Theory (de la Fuente, 2017) as a theoretical model for molar analysis, and position the model as a complementary view and a step forward from the SRL model (Zimmerman and Schunk, 2001), taking a more molecular view of analyzing university students’ learning.

In reference to the topic of learning approaches, the present results confirm the strength of this construct, given that they document how learning approaches are sensitive to the effects of the teaching process, which influences the way students pursue their process of learning at university. This idea was already sufficiently recognized in the SAL model, but insufficiently demonstrated in prior research (Biggs, 2001). The prevailing SRL models (see Panadero, 2017) have encouraged research that limits its attention to the student’s intrapersonal variables, leading to large quantities of research production built on the construct of learning approaches and its associated inventory (Asikainen and Gijbels, 2017).

The present results, however, encourage us to continue to move forward in integrating both sides into an explanatory analysis of interactive learning behavior, using the proposed combination heuristic.

A second, practical implication for applied professional practice has to do with having well-adjusted conceptions about how learning approaches are produced in the university context. If we continue to further the idea that learning approaches depend largely on individual variables, to the detriment of context, we will not recognize the important role of the teaching process, just as its authors conceptualized (Biggs, 2001). Without denying the plentiful prior evidence of associated individual characteristics that are determinants of learning approaches, we must progress toward a more interactive, contextualized view of the two processes of learning and teaching (Vermunt, 1998, 2007; Vermunt and Verloop, 1999; Vermunt and Donche, 2017).

A third, practical implication refers to assessing teaching-learning processes at university, since this is directly related to the issue we have been addressing. Students often participate in assessments of their degree of satisfaction with the teaching process at university, and quality criteria adopted by universities include students’ achievement and their learning approach. If commonly used assessment models continue to focus attention on the teaching process, while overlooking the characteristics of the students who do the assessing, biases are quite likely to exist. Previous research has shown that university students with a surface learning approach, having higher likelihood of poor achievement, tend to give their teachers lower ratings, while students with a deep approach, with greater expectations of success, tend to perceive the teaching process as better in quality (de la Fuente et al., 2011). Furthermore, this assessment practice has another undesirable effect: it is not a contextualized activity for self development, given that students are not assessing themselves with regard to their own characteristics or aspects for improvement in learning, nor with regard to execution of the learning process, but they focus their attention on the teacher and on the teaching process. In this way, students are unlikely to feel that they are equal agents in the process. Using the same logic, teachers likewise are not learning to self-assess their teaching process. For both reasons, it is highly probable that the external attribution of errors and self-attribution of positive aspects adds a bias to this incomplete process.

A final practical implication refers to formative processes of university teachers (Paris and Winograd, 2003). When implementing innovations in the university teaching process, it is important to consider what type of context is being designed (de la Fuente et al., 2013a). If the context is non-regulating or dysregulating, it will probably not help students improve their learning process, especially if students are low in self-regulation. As seen in prior evidence, students with little self-regulation are the ones that require greater external regulation. Certain prior evidence has shown results that concur with this idea (Shaw et al., 2017; Bingen et al., 2019). In addition, the teacher’s level of self-regulation (Capa-Aydin et al., 2009) increases the likelihood of regulatory teaching (Randi, 2004; Monshi-Toussi et al., 2011), although this relationship has not been addressed in the present study. In an effective teaching process, or regulatory teaching, it is the teacher’s responsibility to design learning environments. To implement such designs, evidence-based recommendations are needed (Roehrig et al., 2012).

Conclusion

Most universities develop programs –on an intuitive basis– to attract the best students and teachers, based on the correct assumption that a combination of the two produces good learning processes, good academic outcomes, and satisfaction. The practical reality, however, is that different types of students and teachers are found at every university. The present research has offered a conceptual model, a heuristic of measurement, and consistent empirical data for analyzing any teaching-learning process and its most probable effects in a university context, although these can be extrapolated to other stages of education. We must acknowledge that universities admit students who execute “good and not as good” learning processes, and they can be combined with teachers who execute “good and not as good” teaching processes. We recommend that university administrators and organizational politicians, as well as educational psychologists in charge of university quality, take into account the findings presented here, in order to more precisely understand the quality of teaching-learning processes and make appropriate decisions. Not all teachers teach poorly, nor do all students learn well, and viceversa. A detailed analysis of each combination, based on the heuristic presented, should help in making evidence-based decisions in each case (Slavin, 2019).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Comité de Ética de la Investigación (UNAV), ref. 2018.270. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

JF contributed to conceptualization, design, and data analysis. PS did the initial writing. DK performed the revision of the manuscript. MY did the final revision and adjustments to the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by: R&D Project PGC2018-094672-B-I00, University of Navarra (Ministry of Science and Education, Spain), and R&D Project UAL18-SEJ-DO31-A-FEDER (University of Almería, Spain), and the European Social Fund.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Antonelli, J., Jolly, S., Backscheider, S., and Hawkinset, J. (2020). Understanding the self-regulated learning characteristics of first-generation college students. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 61, 67–83. doi: 10.1353/csd.2020.0004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asikainen, H., and Gijbels, D. (2017). Do students develop towards more Deep approaches to learning during studies? A systematic review on the development of students’ deep and surface approaches to learning in higher education. Educ.Psychol. Rev. 29, 205–234. doi: 10.1007/s10648-017-9406-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azevedo, R., Moos, D. C., Greene, J. A., Winters, F. I., and Cromley, J. G. (2008). Why is externally- facilitated regulated learning more effective than self-regulated learning with hypermedia? Educ. Technol. Res. Dev. 56, 45–72. doi: 10.1007/s11423-007-9067-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baeten, M., Dochy, F., and Struyven, K. (2013). The effects of different learning environments on students’ motivation for learning and their achievement. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 83, 484–501. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.2012.02076.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baeten, M., Dochy, F., Struyven, K., Parmentier, E., and Vanderbruggen, A. (2015). Student-centred learning environments: an investigation into student teachers’ instructional preferences and approaches to learning. Learn. Environ. Res. 19, 443–462.

Google Scholar

Balloo, K., Pauli, R., and Worrell, M. (2017). Undergraduates’ personal circumstances, expectations and reasons for attending university. Stud. High. Educ. 42, 1373–1384. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2015.1099623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barattucci, M. (2017). Approach to study as an indicator of the quality of teaching and of learning environment: the contribution of John Biggs. J. E Learn. Knowl. Soc. 13, 77–88.

Google Scholar

Barattucci, M., Pagliaro, S., Cafagna, D., and Bosetto, D. (2017). An examination of the applicability of Biggs’ 3P learning process model to Italian university. J. E Learn. Knowl. Soc. 13, 163–180.

Google Scholar

Barros, R., Monteiro, A., Nejmedinne, F., and Moreira, J. A. (2012). The relationship between students’ approach to learning and lifelong learning. Psychology 4, 792–797. doi: 10.4236/psych.2013.411113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (1979). Individual differences in study processes and the quality of learning outcomes. High. Educ. 8, 381–394. doi: 10.1007/bf01680526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (1985). The role of metalearning in study processes. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 55, 185–212. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1985.tb02625.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (1988). Assessing student approaches to learning. Aust. Psychol. 23, 197–206. doi: 10.1080/00050068808255604

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J. (2001). The reflective institution: assuring and enhancing the quality of teaching and learning. High. Educ. 41, 221–238.

Google Scholar

Biggs, J., Kember, D., and Leung, D. Y. P. (2001). The revised two-factor study process questionnaire: R-SPQ-2F. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 133–149. doi: 10.1348/000709901158433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biggs, J., and Tang, C. (2011). Teaching for Quality Learning at University. Berkshire: Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Bingen, H. M., Steindal, S. A., Krumsvik, R., and Tveit, B. (2019). Nursing students studying physiology within a flipped classroom, self-regulation and off-campus activities. Nurse Educ. Pract. 35, 55–62. doi: 10.1016/j.nepr.2019.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bobe, B. J., and Cooper, B. J. (2017). The effect of language proficiency on approaches to learning and satisfaction of undergraduate accounting students. Account. Educ. 28, 149–171. doi: 10.1080/09639284.2017.1396481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. M. (1998). “Self-regulation and the addictive behaviors,” in Treating Addictive Behaviors, Vol. 2, eds W. R. Miller and N. Heather (New York, NY: Plenum Press), 61–73. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4899-1934-2_5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brown, J. M., Miller, W. R., and Lawendowski, L. A. (1999). “The self-regulation questionnaire,” in Innovations in Clinical Practice: A Source Book, Vol. 17, eds L. Vandecreek and T. L. Jackson (Sarasota, FL: Professional Resources Press), 281–293.

Google Scholar

Browne, B. A., Kaldenberg, D. O., Browne, W. G., and Brown, D. J. (1998). Student as customer: factors affecting satisfaction and assessments of institutional quality. J. Mark. High. Educ. 8, 1–14. doi: 10.1300/j050v08n03_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cao, Y., Postareff, L., Lindblom-Ylanne, L., and Toom, A. (2019). Teacher educators’ approaches to teaching and connections with their perceptions of the closeness of their research and teaching. Teach. Teach. Educ. 85, 125–136. doi: 10.1016/j.tate.2019.06.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capa-Aydin, Y., Sungur, S., and Uzuntiryaki, E. (2009). Teacher self-regulation: examining a multidimensional construct’. Educ. Psychol. 29, 345–356. doi: 10.1080/01443410902927825

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cetin, B. (2015). Academic motivation and approaches to learning in predicting college students’ academic achievement: findings from Turkish and US samples. J. Coll. Teach. Learn. 12, 141-150.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J. (2017). Theory of self- vs. externally- regulated learningTM: fundamentals, evidence, and applicability. Front. Psychol. 8:1675. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01675

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Amate, J., González-Torres, M. C., Artuch, R., García-Torrecillas, J. M., and Fadda, S. (2020a). Effects of levels of self-regulation and regulatory teaching on strategies for coping with academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:22. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Berbén, A. B., and Zapata, L. (2013a). How regulatory teaching impacts university students’ perceptions of the teaching-learning process: the role of teacher training. Infanc. Aprendizaje 36, 375–385. doi: 10.1174/021037013807533016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Cardelle-Elawar, M., Peralta, F. J., Sánchez, M. D., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and Zapata, L. (2011). Students’ factors affecting undergraduates’ perceptions of their teaching and learning process within ECTS experience. Front. Psychol. 2:28. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., González-Torres, M. C., Aznárez-Sanado, M., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., and Vera, M. M. (2019a). Implications of unconnected micro, molecular, and molar level research in psychology: the case of executive functions, self-regulation, and external regulation. Front. Psychol. 10:1919. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Justicia, F., Casanova, P., and Trianes, M. V. (2004). Perception about construction of academic and professional competences in psychologists. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 3, 33–34.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., López, M., Zapata, L., Sollinas, G., and Fadda, S. (2015a). “Improving mental health trough and online self-assessment and self-help e-utility in university students,” in Progress in Education, Vol. 33, ed. R. V. Nata (New York, NY: Nova Publisher), 63–74.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., Vera, M. M., and Paoloni, P. (2019b). Applying the SRL vs. ERL theory to the knowledge of achievement emotions in undergraduate university students. Front. Psychol. 10:2070. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Paoloni, P. V., Vera-Martínez, M. M., and Garzón-Umerenkova, A. (2020b). Effect of levels of self-regulation and situational stress on achievement emotions in undergraduate students: class, study and testing. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17:4293. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Peralta-Sánchez, F. J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M. V., Sander, P., Garzón-Umerenkova, A., and Zapata, L. (2020c). Effects of self- vs external-regulation on the factors and symptoms of academic stress in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 11:22. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17124293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Pichardo, M. C., Justicia, F., and Berbén, A. (2008). Learning approaches, self-regulation and achievement in three European universities. Psicothema 20, 705–711.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Vera, M. M., Garzón, A., and Fadda, S. (2017). Combined effect of levels in personal self-regulation and regulatory teaching on meta-cognitive, on meta-motivational, and on academic achievement variables in undergraduate students. Front. Psychol. 8:232. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Sander, P., and Putwain, D. (2013b). Relationship between undergraduate student confidence, approach to learning and academic performance: the role of gender. Rev. Psicodidáctica 18, 375–393. doi: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.7078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Cardelle-Ellawar, M., Sander, P., Justicia, F., et al. (2012). Regulatory teaching and self-regulated learning in college students: confirmatory validation study of the IATLP scales. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 10, 839–866.

Google Scholar

de la Fuente, J., Zapata, L., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Sander, P., and Putwain, D. (2015b). “Personal self-regulation, self-regulated learning and coping strategies, in university context with stress,” in Metacognition: Fundaments, Applications, and Trends A Profile of the Current State-Of-The-Art, ed. E. Peña- Ayala (Cham: Springer International Publishing Switzerland), 223–250. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-11062-2_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dinsmore, D. L., Alexander, P. A., and Loughlin, S. M. (2008). Focusing the conceptual lens on metacognition, self-regulation, and self-regulated learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 20, 391–409. doi: 10.1007/s10648-008-9083-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Douglas, J. A., Douglas, A., McClelland, R. J., and Davies, J. (2015). Understanding student satisfaction and dissatisfaction: an interpretive study in the UK higher education context. Stud. High. Educ. 40, 329–349. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2013.842217

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elassy, N. (2015). The concepts of quality, quality assurance and quality enhancement. Qual. Assur. Educ. 23, 250–261. doi: 10.1108/qae-11-2012-0046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Entwistle, N., Hanley, M., and Hounsell, D. (1979). Identifying distinctive approaches to studying. High. Educ. 8, 365–380. doi: 10.1007/bf01680525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Entwistle, N., and Karagiannopoulou, E. (2014). “Perceptions of assessment and their influences on learning,” in Advances and Innovations in University Assessment and Feedback, eds C. Kreber, C. Anderson, N. Entwistle, and J. McArthur (Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press), 75–98. doi: 10.3366/edinburgh/9780748694549.003.0005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Entwistle, N., McCune, V., and Hounsell, J. (2002). Approaches to Studying and Perceptions of University Teaching-Learning Environments: Concepts, Measures and Preliminary Findings. Enhancing Teaching and Learning Environments in Undergraduate Courses Occasional Report, 1. Available online at: http://www.etl.tla.ed.ac.uk/docs/etlreport1.pdf (accessed September 15, 2010).

Google Scholar

Entwistle, N., McCune, V., and Hounsell, J. (2003). “Investigating ways of enhancing university teaching-learning environments: measuring students’ approaches to studying and perceptions of teaching,” in Powerful Learning Environments: Unravelling Basic Components and Dimensions, 1st Edn, eds E. De Corte, L. Verschaffel, N. Entwistle, and J. van Merriënboer (Amsterdam: Pergamon), 89. doi: 10.14705/rpnet.2017.innoconf2016.658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Entwistle, N. J. (2009). Teaching for Understanding at University: Deep Approaches and Distinctive Ways of Thinking. Basingstoke: PalgraveMacmillan.

Google Scholar

Entwistle, N. J. (2018). Student Learning and Academic Understanding: A Research Perspective and Implications for Teaching. New York, NY: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Entwistle, N. J., and Ramsden, P. (1983). Understanding Student Learning. London: Croom Helm.

Google Scholar

Fryer, L. K., and Ginns, P. (2017). A reciprocal test of perceptions of teaching quality and approaches to learning: a longitudinal examination of teaching-learning connections. Educ. Psychol. 38, 1032–1049. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2017.1403568

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gagné, R. M. (1965). Essentials of Learning for Instruction (Spanish Translation: Diana, 1975). New York, NY: The Dryden Press.

Google Scholar

Gargallo, B., Morera, I., and García, E. (2015). Metodología innovadora en la universidad: sus efectos sobre los procesos de aprendizaje de los estudiantes universitarios [Innovative methodology in the university: its effects on the learning processes of university students]. Anal. Psicol. 31, 901–915. doi: 10.6018/analesps.32.1.179871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garzón-Umerenkova, A., de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., Zapata-Sevillano, L., Pichardo, M. C., and García-Berbén, A. B. (2017). Validation of the Spanish short self-regulation questionnaire (SSSRQ) through rasch analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:276. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00276

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goe, L., Bell, C., and Little, O. (2008). Approaches to Evaluating Teacher Effectiveness: A Research Synthesis. Louisville, KY: National Comprehensive Center for Teacher Quality.

Google Scholar

Green, P. (2014). Measuring service quality in higher education: a South African case study. J. Int. Educ. Res. 10, 131–157. doi: 10.19030/jier.v10i2.8515

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gurpinar, E., Kulac, E., Tetik, C., Akdogan, I., and Mamakli, S. (2013). Do learning approaches of medical students affect their satisfaction with problem-based learning? Adv. Physiol. Educ. 37, 85–88. doi: 10.1152/advan.00119.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hazan, B., and Miller, P. (2017). The role of psychological capital in academic adjustment among university students. J. Happiness Stud. 20, 51–65. doi: 10.1007/s10902-017-9933-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heikkilä, A., and Lonka, K. (2006). Studying in higher education: students’ approaches to learning, self-regulation, and cognitive strategies. Stud. High. Educ. 31, 99–117. doi: 10.1080/03075070500392433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howie, P., and Bagnall, R. (2013). A critique of the deep and surface approaches to learning model. Teach. High. Educ. 18, 389–400. doi: 10.1080/13562517.2012.733689

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaramillo, J. M., Rendón, M. I., Muñoz, L., Weis, M., and Trommsdorff, G. (2017). Children’s self-regulation in cultural contexts: the role of parental socialization theories, goals, and practices. Front. Psychol. 8:923. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Justicia, F., Pichardo, M. C., Cano, F., Berbén, A. B., and de la Fuente, J. (2008). The revised two-factor study process questionnaire (RSPQ-2F): exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses at item level. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 23, 355–372. doi: 10.1007/bf03173004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, A. (2008). Clarifying metacognition, self-regulation, and self-regulated learning: what’s the purpose? Educ. Psychol. Rev. 20:477. doi: 10.1007/s10648-008-9087-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karagiannopoulou, E., and Milienos, F. S. (2015). Testing two path models to explore relationships between students’ experiences of the teaching–learning environment, approaches to learning and academic achievement. Educ. Psychol. 35, 26–52. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2014.895800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karagiannopoulou, E., Milienos, F. S., and Athanasopoulos, V. (2018). Associations between defense styles, approaches to learning, and achievement among university students. Front. Educ. 3:53. doi: 10.3389/feduc.2018.00053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kember, D., Webster, B. J., and Chan, W. S. (2020). Refocusing the 3P model to incorporate a learning and teaching environment and graduate attributes. Educ. Psychol. 40, 592–607. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2020.1732304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Köller, O., Meyer, J., Saß, S., and Baumert, J. (2019). New analyses of an old topic: effects of intelligence and motivation on academic achievement. J. Educ. Res. Online 1, 166–189.

Google Scholar

Kossak, R. M. (2019). The Effect of “Teach an LA” on Self-Reported Measures of Student Self-Efficacy. Doctoral Dissertation, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO.

Google Scholar

Kreber, C., Castleden, H., Erfani, N., and Wright, T. (2005). Self-regulated learning about university teaching: an exploratory study. Teach. High. Educ. 10, 75–97. doi: 10.1080/1356251052000305543

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindblom-Ylänne, S., Nevgi, A., and Trigwell, K. (2011). Regulation of university teaching. Instr. Sci. 39, 483–495. doi: 10.1007/s11251-010-9141-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lodewyk, K. R., Winne, P. H., and Jamieson-Noel, D. L. (2009). Implications of task structure on self-regulated learning and achievement’. Educ. Psychol. 29, 1–25. doi: 10.1080/01443410802447023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mahalanobis, P. C. (1930). On tests and measures of groups divergence, theoretical formulae. Int. J. Asiat. Soc. Bengal 26, 541–588.

Google Scholar

Marton, F. (1976). “What does it take to learn? Some implications of an alternative view of learning,” in Strategies for Research and Development in Higher Education, ed. N. J. Entwistle (Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger), 32–43.

Google Scholar

Marton, F., Hounsell, D., and Entwistle, N. (eds). (2005). The Experience of Learning: Implications for Teaching and Studying in Higher Education, 3rd Edn. Edinburgh: University of Edinburgh.

Google Scholar

Moghimi, D., Van Yperen, N. W., Sense, F., Zacher, H., and Scheibe, S. (2020). Using the selection, optimization, and compensation model of action-regulation to explain college students’ grades and study satisfaction. J. Educ. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/edu0000466

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monroy, F., and González-Geraldo, J. L. (2018). Measuring learning: discrepancies between conceptions of and approaches to learning. Educ. Stud. 44, 81–98. doi: 10.1080/03055698.2017.1345677

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monshi-Toussi, M. T., Akbar-Boori, A., and Ghanizadeh, A. (2011). The role of EFL teachers’ self-regulation in effective teaching. World J. Educ. 1, 39–48.

Google Scholar

Nabizadeh, N., Hajian, S., Sheikhan, Z., and Rafiei, F. (2019). Prediction of academic achievement based on learning strategies and outcome expectations among medical students. BMC Med. Educ. 19:99. doi: 10.1186/s12909-019-1527-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paloş, R., Sava, S. L., and Vîrgã, D. (2020). The role of teacher support, students’ need satisfaction, and their psychological capital in enhancing students’ self-regulated learning. Stud. Psychol. 62, 44–57.

Google Scholar

Panadero, E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: six models and four directions for research. Front. Psychol. 8:422. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paris, S. G., and Winograd, P. (2003). The Role of Self-Regulated Learning in Contextual Teaching: Principals and Practices for Teacher Preparation. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.

Google Scholar

Parpala, A., Lindblom-Ylänne, S., Komulainen, E., Litmanen, T., and Hirsto, L. (2010). Students’ approaches to learning and their experiences of the teaching-learning environment in different disciplines. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 269–282. doi: 10.1348/000709909x476946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pichardo, M. C., Justicia, F., de la Fuente, J., Martínez-Vicente, J. M., and García-Berbén, A. B. (2014). Factor structure of the self-regulation questionnaire (SRQ) at Spanish universities. Span. J. Psychol. 17:E62.

Google Scholar

Prosser, M., and Trigwell, K. (1999). Understanding Learning and Teaching: The Experience in Higher Education. Suffolk, VA: Society for Research into Higher Education & Open University Press.

Google Scholar

Randi, J. (2004). Teachers as self-regulated learners. Teach. Coll. Rec. 106, 1825–1853. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9620.2004.00407.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reise, S. P., Scheines, R., Widaman, K. F., and Haviland, M. G. (2012). Multidimensionality and structural coefficient bias in structural equation modeling: a bifactor perspective. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 73, 5–26. doi: 10.1177/0013164412449831

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, J. T. E. (2015). Approaches to learning or levels of processing: what did Marton and Säljö (1976a) really say? The legacy of the work of the Göteborg group in the 1970s. Interchange 46, 239–269. doi: 10.1007/s10780-015-9251-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Abraham, C., and Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138, 353–387. doi: 10.1037/a0026838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roe, R. A. (2003). ¿Qué hace competente a un psicólogo? [What makes a psychologist competent?]. Papeles Psicól. 24, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Roehrig, A. D., and Chistesen, E. (2010). “Development and use of tool for evaluating effectiveness in grades K-12,” in Innovative Assessment for the 21st Century: Supporting Educational Assessment, eds V. Schuter and B. Becker (New York, NY: Springer), 207–228. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4419-6530-1_12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roehrig, A. D., Turner, J. E., Arrastia, M. C., Christesen, E., McElhaney, S., and Jakiel, L. M. (2012). “Effective teachers and teaching: characteristics and practices related to positive student outcomes,” in APA Educational Psychology Handbook, Vol. 2, eds K. A. Harris, S. Graham, and T. Urdan (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 501–527. doi: 10.1037/13274-020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, M., Scevak, J., Southgate, E., Macqueen, S., Williams, P., and Douglas, H. (2018). Older women, deeper learning, and greater satisfaction at university: age and gender predict university students’ learning approach and degree satisfaction. J. Divers. High. Educ. 11, 82–96. doi: 10.1037/dhe0000042

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruohoniemi, M., Parpala, A., Lindblom-Ylänne, S., and Katajavuori, N. (2010). Relationships between students’ approaches to learning, perceptions of the teaching? Learning environment, and study success: a case study of third-year veterinary students. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 37, 282–288. doi: 10.3138/jvme.37.3.282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scevak, J., Southgate, E., Rubin, M., Macqueen, S., Douglas, H., and Williams, P. (2015). Equity Groups and Predictors of Academic Success in Higher Education. A 2014 Student Equity in Higher Education Research Grants Project. Newcastle University (UK): Technical Report. Available online at: http://www.researchgate.net/publication/286450707

Google Scholar

Schertzer, C. B., and Schertzer, S. M. (2004). Student satisfaction and retention: a conceptual model. J. Mark. High. Educ. 14, 79–91. doi: 10.1300/j050v14n01_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, M. P., Peart, D. J., and Fairhead, O. J. W. (2017). Perceived stress in university students studying in a further education college. Res. Post Compulsory Educ. 22, 442–452. doi: 10.1080/13596748.2017.1362534

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slavin, R. E. (2019). How evidence-based reform will transform research and practice in education. Educ. Psychol. 54, 1–13.

Google Scholar

Tabachnick, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using Multivariate Statistics, 6th Edn. Boston, MA: Pearson.

Google Scholar

Trigwell, K., Ellis, R. A., and Han, F. (2012). Relations between students’ approaches to learning, experienced emotions and outcomes of learning. Stud. High. Educ. 37, 811–824. doi: 10.1080/03075079.2010.549220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trigwell, K., and Prosser, M. (2004). Development and use of the approaches to teaching inventory. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 16, 409–424. doi: 10.1007/s10648-004-0007-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trigwell, K., Prosser, M., and Waterhouse, F. (1999). Relations between teachers’ approaches to teaching and students’ approaches to learning. High. Educ. 37, 57–70.

Google Scholar

Trommsdorff, G. (2012). Development of “Agentic” regulation in cultural context: the role of self and world views. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 19–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00224.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vanno, V., Kaemkate, W., and Wongwanich, S. (2014). Relationships between academic performance, perceived group psychological capital, and positive psychological capital of Thai undergraduate students. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 116, 3226–3230. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.01.739

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (1998). The regulation of constructive learning processes. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 68, 149–171. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1998.tb01281.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D. (2007). The power of teaching-learning environments to influence student learning. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. Monogr. Ser. 4, 73–90.

Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D., and Donche, V. (2017). A learning patterns perspective on student learning in higher education: state of the art and moving forward. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 29, 269–299. doi: 10.1007/s10648-017-9414-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D., and van Rijswijk, F. A. W. M. (1988). Analysis and development of students’ skill in self-regulated learning. High. Educ. 17, 647–682.

Google Scholar

Vermunt, J. D., and Verloop, N. (1999). Congruence and friction between learning and teaching. Learn. Instr. 9, 257–280. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4752(98)00028-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wibrowski, C. R., Matthews, W. K., and Kitsantas, A. (2016). The role of a skills learning support program on first-generation college students’ self-regulation, motivation, and academic achievement: a longitudinal study. J. Coll. Stud. Ret. 9, 317–332. doi: 10.1177/1521025116629152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winne, P. H. (1995). Self-regulation is ubiquitous but its forms vary with knowledge. Educ. Psychol. 30, 223–228. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3004_9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Winne, P. H., and Hadwin, A. F. (1997). “Studying as self-regulated learning,” in Metacognition in Educational Theory and Practice, eds D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, and A. C. Graesser (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc), 277–304.

Google Scholar

Yerdelen, S., and Sungur, S. (2019). Multilevel investigation of students’ self-regulation processes in learning science: classroom learning environment and teacher effectiveness. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 17, 89–110. doi: 10.1007/s10763-018-9921-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. (2000). “Attaining self-regulation. A social cognitive perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Regulation, eds M. Boekaerts, P. Pintrich, and M. Zeidner (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 13–39. doi: 10.1016/b978-012109890-2/50031-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Academic studying and the development of personal skill: a self-regulatory perspective. Educ. Psychol. 33, 73–86. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3302%263_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmerman, B. J., and Schunk, D. H. (eds). (2001). Self-Regulated Learning and Academic Achievement: Theoretical Perspectives. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Keywords: undergraduate students, satisfaction, academic achievement, learning approaches, SRL vs. ERL theory

Citation: de la Fuente J, Sander P, Kauffman DF and Yilmaz Soylu M (2020) Differential Effects of Self- vs. External-Regulation on Learning Approaches, Academic Achievement, and Satisfaction in Undergraduate Students. Front. Psychol. 11:543884. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.543884

Received: 18 March 2020; Accepted: 14 September 2020;
Published: 07 October 2020.

Edited by:

Sung-il Kim, Korea University, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Mirjam Weis, Technical University of Munich, Germany
Yi Jiang, East China Normal University, China

Copyright © 2020 de la Fuente, Sander, Kauffman and Yilmaz Soylu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jesús de la Fuente, jdlfuente@unav.es; jfuente@ual.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.