Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 11 December 2019
Sec. Cognitive Neuroscience
Volume 13 - 2019 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00424

Identification of Visual Attentional Regions of the Temporoparietal Junction in Individual Subjects using a Vivid, Novel Oddball Paradigm

  • 1Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA, United States
  • 2Department of Health and Human Performance, Stanford University Medical School, Stanford, CA, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Seattle, WA, United States
  • 4Department of Cognitive, Linguistic and Psychological Sciences, Brown University, Providence, RI, United States

The Temporoparietal Junction (TPJ) of the cerebral cortex is a functionally heterogeneous region that also exhibits substantial anatomical variability across individuals. As a result, the precise functional organization of TPJ remains controversial. One or more regions within TPJ support visual attention processes, but the “attention TPJ” is difficult to functionally observe in individual subjects, and thus is typically identified by averaging across a large group of subjects. However, group-averaging also blurs localization and can obscure functional organization. Here, we develop and test an individual-subject approach to identifying attentional TPJ. This paradigm employs novel oddball images with a strong visual drive to produce robust TPJ responses in individuals. Vivid, novel oddballs drive responses in two TPJ regions bilaterally, a posterior region centered in posterior Superior Temporal Sulcus (TPJSTS) and an anterior region in ventral Supramarginal Gyrus (TPJSMG). Although an attentional reorienting task fails to drive TPJ activation in individuals, group analysis of the attentional reorienting contrast reveals recruitment of right TPJSTS, but not right TPJSMG. Similarly, right TPJSTS, as identified in individual subjects by the vivid, novel oddball contrast, is activated by attentional reorienting, but right TPJSMG is not. These findings advance an individual-subject based approach to understanding the functional organization of TPJ.

Introduction

The human cortical temporoparietal junction (TPJ) has been implicated in a wide range of cognitive processes including attentional reorienting (Corbetta et al., 2000; Kincade et al., 2005; Astafiev et al., 2006; Fox et al., 2006; Shulman et al., 2009; Han and Marois, 2014), contextual updating (DiQuattro and Geng, 2011; Geng and Vossel, 2013; DiQuattro et al., 2014), and social cognition (Young and Saxe, 2008, 2009; Saxe, 2010; but see Mitchell, 2008). The human TPJ also demonstrates a high degree of individual variability both anatomically and in its connectivity profile (Hasson et al., 2004; Van Essen, 2005; Mueller et al., 2013). The TPJ is one of the areas of largest cortical expansion from macaques to humans (Buckner and Krienen, 2013), and thus non-human primate models provide fewer clues to functionality in this region of the human brain. It is likely that TPJ consists of multiple, functionally distinct regions. For instance, a posterior TPJ may serve Theory of Mind functions, while an anterior TPJ serves attention functions (Krall et al., 2015; Trautwein et al., 2016). However, other studies suggest that finer-scale organization, with as many as five distinct regions in a hemisphere, may exist within TPJ (Igelström et al., 2015, 2016; Dugué et al., 2018). In this manuscript, we investigate and localize in individual subjects a set of visually-responsive TPJ sub-regions that are modulated by attention.

The location of the attentional TPJ is highly variable between individuals (Hasson et al., 2004; Van Essen, 2005; Niogi et al., 2010; Mueller et al., 2013; Dugué et al., 2018) and thus between-subject group averaging is not the ideal approach to localizing this region. Although sophisticated data analysis techniques have been applied to group resting-state (Mars et al., 2012b; Igelström et al., 2015) or functional data (Igelström et al., 2016) to produce detailed TPJ subdivisions, these approaches have yielded somewhat inconsistent parcellations of TPJ. Additionally, given the apparent proximity of a Default Mode Network region (which is suppressed by many attentionally demanding tasks) to a region activated by attentional reorienting (e.g., Mars et al., 2012a), analyses that rely on group-averaging techniques to identify a single TPJ could easily be partly confounded by the inclusion of heterogeneous functional domains into the region of interest (ROI). We suggest that careful within-subject mapping across multiple functional tasks (e.g., Somers and Sheremata, 2013; Michalka et al., 2015) could prove useful for yielding a clearer understanding of the functional organization of the TPJ region. A key goal of the present study is to validate an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) method to reliably localize the visual attentional TPJ region or regions across individual subjects.

In prior work, attention functions in TPJ typically have been localized with one of three broad paradigms. The most commonly employed paradigm is a Posner-style spatial cueing and reorienting task (Posner, 1980; Thiel et al., 2004; Weissman and Prado, 2012; de Haan et al., 2015) in which participants report the appearance of a stimulus at an attended (valid) or unattended (invalid) location; attentional TPJ can be identified by the contrast of invalid trials geater than valid trials because correct performance on invalid trials requires a reorienting of attention. A second approach involves the presentation of infrequent, salient, unexpected “oddball” stimuli; oddball stimuli induce attentional capture and thus the contrast of oddball greater than non-oddball trials can be used to identify attentional TPJ (Sutton et al., 1965; Linden et al., 1999; Clark et al., 2000; Bledowski et al., 2004; Asplund et al., 2010; Kim, 2013; Warbrick et al., 2013). A third, less commonly applied approach is to use direct visual drive to elicit responses in TPJ (Horiguchi et al., 2016; Dugué et al., 2018). This visual drive approach is very promising in that it has been successful in identifying TPJ in individual subjects. Dugué et al. (2018) observed two bilateral and one right hemisphere visually-driven TPJ. Horiguchi et al. (2016) consistently observed only a single right hemisphere TPJ, indicating that their approach may have been underpowered to observe the full set of regions reported by Dugué et al. (2018). We summarize findings from all approaches used to identify TPJ in a meta-analysis, displayed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies included in meta-analysis.

Here, we combine all three elements, spatial reorienting, oddballs, and visual drive in a single scan task paradigm. Our key manipulation utilizes vivid, novel oddball stimuli that drive both strong visual and strong novelty responses. Our rationale is that by combining both factors we may elicit a robust enough transient fMRI response that it can be reliably detected in individual subjects. We use full-screen vivid images (outdoor scenes) in order to produce a strong visual drive. Due to prior work indicating that repeated presentation of a stimulus weakens the oddball response (Asplund et al., 2010), we present a novel image for every oddball occurrence. We refer to this as a “vivid, novel oddball” (ViNO) paradigm. The inclusion of a spatial cueing task within the same scan runs permits us to compare activation across task components within individual subjects.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Ten healthy right-handed individuals (four males), ages 27–33 with normal or corrected-to-normal vision participated. All participants had no history of neuropsychological disorders. All were recruited from Boston University and the greater Boston community and gave written informed consent. All experiments were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Boston University. Participants were compensated monetarily for their time.

Combined Spatial Cueing and Vivid, Novel Oddball (ViNO) Paradigm

Participants performed an endogenous, covert spatial attention task that was interrupted by infrequent and task-irrelevant oddball events in the form of vivid, novel, full-screen images (Figure 1). The attention task required participants to maintain central fixation and to indicate the identity of a target letter (T or L, presented at 7 degrees eccentricity and at either 0° or 180° of rotation) following a spatial cue period.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cue-ball task trial structure and timing. (A) Examples of each of three trial types (Valid, Invalid, Oddball). Each trial began with a spatial cue indicating one of six possible locations (dashed lines in A, dashed lines were not visible during the experiment). Following an inter-stimulus interval, the target appeared. The target was the letter “T” or “L” and could appear upright or inverted. The cue could be valid (target appears at the cued location) or invalid (appears in an uncued location). Following the target, a mask of superimposed “T”s and “L”s was presented at all possible target locations. On a randomly selected 1/6 of trials, a task-irrelevant RSVP stream of five vivid, full-screen, session-unique “oddball” images appeared behind the letter mask, each with 100 ms duration. (B) Trial timing: the orienting phase was 5.2 s in total, with cue duration randomly jittered from 1.5 to 3.0 s and the fixation cross ISI accounting for the remaining 5.2 s. Following the orienting phase, the target appeared for 500 ms, followed by the mask or oddball mask for 500 ms. The mask/oddball phase was followed by an inter-trial interval of 1.6 s. Participants were asked to indicate the identity of the target letter (T or L).

Each trial lasted 7.8 s and consisted of the following phases: orienting phase (cue and ISI), target phase (letter on screen), mask phase (mask on screen) and ITI (fixation cross on screen). During the orienting phase, a red arrow cue appeared and indicated one of six possible target locations on the screen. To encourage attention to only the cued location, participants were instructed in advance that the letter target would appear at the spatially cued location “more often than not,” and that the letter target would never appear at a location other than the six possible cued locations. The timing of the orienting phase was jittered such that the duration of the cue and ISI together always summed to 5.2 s, with the cue taking a random value between 1.5 and 3.0 s and the ISI filling the rest of the 5.2 s; the unpredictable interval from cue offset to target onset encouraged the deployment of sustained attention over the full ISI. Following the orienting phase, the target (upright or inverted T or L) appeared for 500 ms either at the cued (valid) or an uncued (invalid) location. Invalid targets always appeared in a random location (one of three) in the uncued hemifield. Spatial cues were valid on 68.75% of trials (33 out of 48 trials per run). Participants responded with one of two keys on a button box to indicate the identity of the letter target regardless of its orientation or location. The target was followed by a 500 ms mask of superimposed T’s and L’s at all of the six possible target locations (Figure 1). Following the mask, participants were shown a fixation cross for the ITI of 1.6 s. Responses were recorded from target onset to the start of the following trial (a 2.6 s total possible response period). Each run consisted of 48 trials, and participants completed either 10 (s05, s06) or 12 (all other subjects) runs per scan session.

In each run, on a fraction of trials, the post-target mask was displayed with a background of five full-screen (approximately 22 × 16.5 degrees), full-color, unique “oddball” images each presented for 100 ms (Figure 1). Prior work indicates that distractor surprise effects rapidly diminish with repeated presentation of the stimulus (Asplund et al., 2010). In order to keep the surprise effects strong across the scan session, each oddball image was displayed only once to any subject. An additional design consideration was the frequency of oddball trials. A greater number of oddball trials could potentially increase statistical power; however, at high frequency oddballs might come to be expected by subjects, thus reducing BOLD activation per presentation. To examine this issue empirically, we performed a pilot study with four participants, in which we varied the frequency of oddball events parametrically from 1/8 to 1/3 of total trials. The comparison of net BOLD signal vs. oddball frequency exhibited an “inverted-U” shape, indicating that the effects were primarily driven by surprise effects rather than by stimulus drive. The most consistent robust effects were exhibited when oddballs occurred on 1/6 of trials (see Supplementary Figure S1). Therefore, in all subsequent sessions, oddballs were presented on a randomly selected 1/6 of trials. Oddball images were presented in a “rapid-fire” or rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) burst (five images, 100 ms per image). The intention of the “rapid-fire” oddball sequence was to produce a strong, unexpected attentional capture effect. During 12 runs, participants saw 576 total trials including 96 oddball trials (480 unique oddball images). A diverse variety of vivid and lively scenes that serve as strong exogenous cues to attention were selected for the oddball images. Because prior work (Engell and Haxby, 2007; Tsao and Livingstone, 2008; Nasr et al., 2014) indicates the presence of a visually responsive face-processing region in posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS), images with prominent human faces were excluded from the oddball image set.

Each run also included 7.8 s of fixation (no stimuli other than a fixation cross) at both the start and the end of the run. Each participant was scanned on either 480 or 576 total trials, including 80 or 96 total oddball trials, plus one practice run outside of the scanner. The practice runs did not contain any oddball images and participants were not informed that oddball images would occur, only that they should indicate the identity of the target letter despite variations in its location or orientation, or any other stimuli that might occur. Task paradigm code is available upon request (somers@bu.edu, kdevaney@stanford.edu).

Test-Retest

Two participants were scanned on two pilot versions of the task [one in which oddball frequency was varied parametrically, as described in “Materials and Methods” section, and one with oddball stimuli occurring in only four runs (time point 1 in Figure 4)]. The low-level stimulus features of the oddball images differed over these three sessions: for each session, the participants saw a random selection of images from a pool of 500 total images.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Spatial cueing group-averaged activation. A group-average analysis (N = 10) of invalidly (hot colors) vs. validly (cool colors) cued targets (p < 0.01, uncorrected). Activation is observed in right hemisphere around the posterior end of the superior temporal sulcus (STS) near the border with the parietal lobe.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Vivid, Novel Oddball effect in group average and in individual subjects. (A) Group average analysis (N = 10; cluster correction threshold of p < 0.01) of oddball vs. non-oddball trials reveals robust activation in bilateral temporoparietal junction (TPJ) and indicates two distinct patches of activation falling within the region of the TPJ: a large bilateral posterior patch (“TPJSTS,” cyan circle), spanning both banks of the STS, and a smaller bilateral anterior patch (“TPJSMG,” orange circle) in the ventral portion of the Supramarginal Gyrus (SMG). (B) Zoom-in of the same analysis shown in each individual subject (p < 0.01, uncorrected; see also Supplementary Figure S4 for contrast effect sizes).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Test-Retest of oddball contrast in two individual subjects. Two subjects were scanned on the task three times. (A) Data from s01 at t = 0, t = 7 months, and t = 19 months. (B) Data from s02 at t = 0, t = 5 months, and t = 11 months (p < 0.01, uncorrected). Note the consistency across time in the anatomical locations. Data from tp1 reflects four runs of the task.

MR Imaging Procedures and Data Analysis

MR Scanning

All task data were acquired using a 32-channel Siemens head coil in a horizontal bore 3 Tesla Siemens Tim Trio located at the Harvard University Center for Brain Science in Cambridge, MA, USA. Gradient echo EPI sequences were used for all tasks (TR = 2,600 ms, TE = 30 ms, Flip angle = 90°, voxel size = 3.0 × 3.0 × 3.1 mm, 42 slices, whole-brain coverage). Eight participants completed 12 runs (1,728 volumes) and two participants completed 10 runs (1,440 volumes). Magnetization Prepared Rapid Gradient Echo (MP-RaGE) T1-weighted high-resolution data (TR = 6.6 ms, TE = 2.9 ms, Flip angle = 8°, voxel size = 1.0 × 1.0 × 1.3 mm) was acquired for each participant. For nine participants these data were acquired on the same scanner as the task data and for one participant, it was acquired with identical hardware at the Martinos Center for Biomedical Imaging in Charlestown, MA, USA.

Analysis

Cortical reconstruction and volumetric segmentation of the T1 data was performed with the Freesurfer image analysis suite, which is documented and freely available for download at http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/. The technical details of these procedures are described in prior publications (Dale and Sereno, 1993; Dale et al., 1999; Fischl et al., 1999a,b; Fischl et al., 2001, 2002; Fischl et al., 2004; Fischl and Dale, 2000; Fischl, 2004; Ségonne et al., 2004; Han et al., 2006; Jovicich et al., 2006). All task data were analyzed with fs-fast version 5.1.0 and custom Matlab scripts. For the task data, motion correction, intensity normalization and boundary-based registration (Greve and Fischl, 2009) to the participant’s own anatomical data were performed on a per-run basis. Singular value decomposition reduced the six motion correction vectors to three eigenvectors, which were included as nuisance regressors in the model. Individual subject analysis concatenated data from all runs into a single time-course, using standard FreeSurfer FS-Fast V5.1 methods. Analyses were performed separately in each hemisphere on each subject’s own cortical surface with 3 mm fwhm smoothing and data were analyzed for each voxel using a GLM with each condition as a predictor (i.e., one for Valid Target, Invalid Target, Oddball, Non-Oddball and Passive). Since invalidly cued oddball trials were necessarily rare (5.2% of trials), interaction effects were not examined. The BOLD signal was modeled as a linear, time-invariant system with a γ response function assumed for each condition with a delay δ = 2.25 and a delay time constant τ = 1.25. An estimated response was generated by convolving the response function with the event length (i.e., the time in each condition) and minimizing the residual error (FS-FAST). Event-related GLM analysis was performed for each voxel to obtain a beta weight for each condition. Contrast effect sizes were computed for each voxel for each contrast by comparing the beta values. t-stats for the contrasts between conditions were computed using the contrast effect sizes for the conditions and their variance. Activation differences were visualized on the surface of each subject’s own hemisphere using p-values derived from these t-stats and applying a liberal threshold of p < 0.01 (uncorrected) at the vertex level. Individual subject analysis was restricted to the anatomical vicinity of TPJ, where significant group-level activation was observed (see Figure 3B). Observed clusters of these activated cortical vertices on these maps were used to define individual subject TPJ ROIs that are used in some analyses. The Posner cueing analysis was conducted with the contrast of Invalid vs. Valid Targets and was compared with regions activated for the ViNO contrast of Oddball trials vs. Non-Oddball trials.

For the group average analysis, each participant’s fMRI data were registered to an average cortical surface space (Freesurfer 5.1.0, “fsaverage” brain) using the boundary of the gray matter and white matter (Greve and Fischl, 2009). The GLM analysis methods were the same as for individual subject data, however, the significance of these activation differences was computed on vertices of the fsaverage brain and visualized on that surface. We then employed a random-effects model to compute the group-averaged value for each condition at each vertex before running t-tests at each vertex to compare group-level activation differences per condition. Significant group-level task activation was corrected for multiple comparisons using cluster-based correction, permuting the sign on the design matrix (using the FS-FAST tool mri_glmfit-sim) with cluster-thresholding at a corrected alpha of p < 0.01. This analysis generated a mask of significant clusters and group-level maps reflect the voxelwise p-values within the significant clusters.

Results

Meta-Analysis

We reviewed the literature on fMRI studies of the TPJ, focusing on investigations of visual attention, oddball processing, and related topics. The primary type of task contrast used and the Talairach coordinate of peak activation in the right hemisphere TPJ for each study is summarized in Table 1 (see also Geng and Vossel, 2013; Dugué et al., 2018). Across all studies, the mean Talairach position was 52.1 ± 1.0, −48.4 ± 1.2, 20.9 ± 1.2 (std. err. of mean). Examination of only the spatial cueing tasks yielded a similar mean Talairach position of 52.7 ± 1.5, −50 ± 1.7, 21.3 ± 1.7. Examination of the oddball contrasts yielded a mean Talairach position of 52.3 ± 2.3, −43.2 ± 3.7, 20.3 ± 5.7, which suggests that the activation for oddball stimuli may shift somewhat anterior relative to the activation due to spatial cueing. However, even within the set of spatial cueing studies, the range of reported Talairach coordinates was very large, 38 ≤ × x 61, −65 ≤ y ≤ −36, 11 ≤ z ≤ 42, and encompassed the mean coordinates for the oddball activation.

Behavior

Target discrimination (T vs. L) performance was analyzed for three different classes of trials: invalidly cued targets, validly cued targets, and targets followed by vivid, novel oddball stimuli. Median reaction times for each condition in individual subjects were entered into paired t-tests across conditions (non-oddball valid vs. invalid, oddball valid vs. invalid). Mean overall accuracy (across all trials and all subjects) was 86 ± 1.6%. There was a trend toward a speed-accuracy tradeoff in this dataset (speed-accuracy correlation r = 0.57, p = 0.08), therefore only correct trials were included in the RT analysis. As expected, reaction time on non-oddball invalidly cued trials was significantly longer than non-oddball validly cued trials (mean invalid = 724 ± 19 ms, mean valid = 683 ± 18 ms, mean difference = 40 ± 6 ms, t(9) = −3.54, p < 0.01). Mean reaction time on oddball image trials was more variable (mean = 679 ± 40 ms) and was not significantly different from either type of non-oddball trial.

fMRI Analysis

Analysis of the spatial cueing conditions contrasted invalidly cued and validly cued conditions. Group-average comparison of invalid to valid cue trials (n = 10; p < 0.01, uncorrected) revealed activation in the vicinity of the TPJ. The most robust activation was observed in right hemisphere (rh), spanning both banks of a posterior segment of the STS, near the border with the parietal lobe (see Figure 2). With this liberal threshold, weaker activation can be observed in the left hemisphere in a symmetric region in posterior STS and in Supramarginal Gyrus (SMG). Modest group-level activation is also observed in parietal and frontal regions of the dorsal attention network. When analyzed with cluster-correction methods (p < 0.01), only the rh STS region survives (see Supplementary Figure S2A). Activation maps for individual subjects were typically blank brain images (see Supplementary Figure S2B). The pattern and amplitude of the activations observed here are similar to prior fMRI studies of spatial cueing (see Table 1). We note that the weak activation in the invalid vs. valid contrast and the inability to observe activation for this contrast in individual subjects was expected based on prior studies and was a primary motivator of our effort to develop a more robust TPJ localizer.

Comparison of trials on which oddball images appeared to non-oddball trials yields much stronger activation than does the spatial cueing contrast. This “ViNO contrast” of vivid, novel oddball trials to non-oddball trials localizes TPJ structures at the group-average level and in individual subjects. Figure 3A shows the group average task-evoked activity maps for the oddball trials compared to non-oddball trials (n = 10, random effects model; cluster-corrected p < 0.01; see Supplementary Figure S3 for medial surface). The visual attentional TPJ, localized with vivid, novel oddball stimuli, is divided into a posterior region centered on the fundus of and spanning both banks of the STS, that we refer to as TPJSTS (see cyan outline) and an anterior region in the ventral portion of SMG, that we refer to as TPJSMG (see orange outline). These regions are observed bilaterally. Activation was also observed in regions of frontal, occipital, and dorsal parietal lobe, but analysis of these regions is beyond the scope of this manuscript.

In order to better reveal the visual attentional functional organization around TPJ, we examined activation in the oddball vs. non-oddball contrast in each of 10 individual subjects (see Figure 3B); this analysis was anatomically restricted to the anatomical vicinity of the TPJ activation observed in the group analysis. In order to define TPJ ROIs individual subject hemispheres, these maps were liberally thresholded at p < 0.01, uncorrected (see Supplementary Figure S4 for contrast effect sizes). Note that equivalent analysis of individual subjects in spatial cueing trials (invalid vs. valid) yielded very few activated vertices (see Supplementary Figure S2B). The oddball-responsive TPJ in individual subjects consists of posterior and anterior regions with variable anatomical location and magnitude of activation. The posterior TPJ (TPJSTS) lies toward the dorsal end of the STS (with a caudal border with occipital areas defined by the caudal aspect of STS). Most commonly, activation spans opposing banks of a small segment of STS. The anterior TPJ (TPJSMG) lies in the ventral portion of the SMG. All subjects (10/10) display a right posterior TPJ (TPJSTS) and 9/10 subjects display a left TPJSTS. 6/10 subjects display a right anterior TPJ (TPJSMG) and 5/10 subjects display a left TPJSMG (Table 2). In many subjects, TPJSTS activation splits into two distinct regions, one on each bank of STS; however, this could reflect a partial-volume effect as the two STS banks are often in such close proximity that our voxel resolution does not clearly distinguish them. On the other hand, neither bank of STS is consistently more strongly or extensively activated, as would be expected in partial-voluming and in some subjects a single contiguous pattern of activation is observed to span both banks STS.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Vivid, Novel Oddball-localized TPJ areas in individual subjects.

Test–Retest

In order to demonstrate the reliability and reproducibility of the TPJ results observed with vivid, novel oddballs, we performed a test-retest analysis with two participants, scanned in three separate sessions each. Subject 01 was scanned 5 months and 11 months after the first session. Subject 02 was scanned 7 months and 19 months after the first session. Test-retest results are shown in Figures 4A,B. The bilateral activity in TPJSTS and TPJSMG is stable over long time intervals. Note the small but stable patch of activation in the LH TPJSMG. There were minor differences in the oddball paradigm across sessions (see “Materials and Methods” section), but the regions were robustly identified in each scan. This is an important validation of the utility of the ViNO paradigm for TPJ identification in individual subjects.

Comparison of Spatial Cueing and Vivid, Novel Oddball Activation in TPJ

As noted above, the spatial cueing contrast failed to reveal substantial TPJ activation clusters in individual subjects, but group-level analysis did reveal activation. In order to examine the Spatial Cueing task contrast activation at a finer scale, we employed the vivid, novel oddball contrast as an individual subject TPJ localizer to identify rh TPJSTS, rh TPJSMG, lh TPJSTS, lh TPJSMG and conduct ROI analyses. We examined cue validity effects in individual-subject defined oddball responsive TPJ, in the individuals for whom these ROIs could be defined (see Figure 5). Cue validity effects were evident in bilateral TPJSTS (rh: n = 10, t(9) = 5.89, p = 0.0002; lh: n = 9, t(8) = 5.57, p = 0.0005) and in TPJSMG in the lh (n = 5; t(4) = 7.88, p = 0.0014), but not the rh (n = 6, t(5) = 0.71, p = 0.5094). The three significant ROIs survive Holm-Bonferroni correction (p < 0.01). Since not all individual subjects had all ROIs defined, we repeated this analysis using group-defined ROIs applied to all subjects and obtained very similar results (Figure 5). The invalid vs. valid contrast was significant in the TPJSTS in both the right hemisphere (t(9) = 5.77, p = 0.0003) and the left hemisphere (t(9) = 6.13, p = 0.0002). In the TPJSMG, the invalid vs. valid contrast was significant in the left (t(9) = 4.93, p = 0.0008) but not the right hemisphere (t(9) = 1.68, p = 0.1273, trending negative). Again, the same three significant ROIs survive Holm-Bonferroni correction (p < 0.01). The individual-subject and group-level ROI analyses of these data yield the same conclusions. These results demonstrate that the ViNO contrast is effective in defining TPJ ROIs that capture cue invalidity reorienting effects in individual subjects. Notably, rh TPJSTS was identified in all subjects in this study. This also indicates that rh TPJSTS, but not rh TPJSMG exhibits the functional properties of the TPJ identified in spatial cueing studies.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Cue validity effects in oddball-defined regions of interest (ROIs). Individual subject ROIs (black), group-defined ROIs (gray). Percent signal change for invalid vs. valid contrast, *p > 0.05 or **p > 0.01.

We also examined the robustness of vivid, novel oddball activation within the TPJ region defined by the spatial cueing validity contrast. As summarized in the “Introduction” section, a right hemisphere region in TPJ is the region most commonly identified in prior spatial cueing fMRI studies (see Table 1). In order to further examine the extent to which the oddball trials activate the same TPJ region as the attention reorienting trials, we defined a rh TPJSTS ROI based on the group average contrast of invalid to valid trials. The group-defined ROI was mapped onto each individual subject in our set, resulting in an ROI with a mean surface area of 833 mm2. Although the ROI was defined using the “invalid vs. valid” contrast and was thus biased to see a robust effect of cue validity, oddball trials nonetheless drove the ROI more strongly than non-oddball trials (valid cue mean = 0.357 ± 0.134, valid cue followed by oddball mean = 0.696 ± 0.198, t(9) = −5.1, p < 0.01; invalid cue mean = 0.490 ± 0.146, invalid cue followed by oddball mean = 0.715 ± 0.199, t(9) = −3.4, p < 0.01; Figure 6). There was no rh TPJSTS ROI in which the cue validity contrast yielded stronger activation than did the ViNO contrast. These results demonstrate that the vivid, novel oddball contrast is highly effective in identifying the rh TPJ region activated by the spatial cueing task.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Percent signal change in right TPJSTS for each individual subject, using group-defined ROI from the cueing task. The right TPJSTS ROI was defined using the group average contrast of invalid vs. valid, as shown, in Figure 2. In every subject, oddball stimuli drive right TPJSTS more than invalid cues, even though invalid vs. valid cues were used to define the ROI. Subject labels are the same as Figure 2. Each subject is represented by a vertical line, with percent signal change to valid targets shown with circles, invalid targets with triangles, and oddball images with squares.

Comparison to Meta-Analysis

The group analyses largely appear to replicate prior observations of attention-related activation in the vicinity of the TPJ. In order to put our findings in context, we also compared them to a meta-analysis of prior examinations of the TPJ region (see above). Two main contrasts were used in prior studies: spatial cueing and oddball-target tasks. In Figure 7A we plot the Talairach coordinates of prior attention-related localizations of TPJ with the localization method color-coded. Note that all of these studies are group averages. We compare this meta-analysis with probabilistic ROIs created from the individual subject ROIs defined from the vivid, novel oddball contrast (oddball trials vs. non-oddballs trials) in Figure 7B. In Figure 7C; we compare the meta-analysis with the group-activation pattern for valid vs. invalid (Figure 7C) trials. There is considerable overlap between our group average and the group averages of prior studies. The meta-analysis clustering is more strongly centered over TPJSTS than TPJSMG, but some prior oddball studies do overlap with TPJSMG as activated in the ViNO contrast here.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Meta-analysis of prior attention studies identifying TPJ compared with current group average task activation. (A) Meta-analysis of 45 prior studies using either spatial cueing (white), oddball tasks (black), or another attentional method (teal) to localize the right TPJ. (B,C) Zoom-in of meta-analysis overlaid on (B) probabilistic labels built from individual subject responses to the vivid, novel oddball contrast (see Supplementary Figure S5 and Supporting Information for further details) and (C) spatial cueing group-average activation maps. See Table 1 for a list of studies depicted.

Discussion

Attentional reorienting functions have long been attributed to a region where the temporal and parietal cortices abut on the lateral surface of the brain (Kincade et al., 2005; Fox et al., 2006; Corbetta et al., 2008; Shulman et al., 2009; Doricchi et al., 2010; Corbetta and Shulman, 2011; de Haan et al., 2015); however, the precise location of the attentionally activated region of the TPJ has proven to be very challenging due to anatomical variability across individuals and the general functional heterogeneity of this region of cortex (Hasson et al., 2004; Van Essen, 2005; Mueller et al., 2013). A challenge in functionally parsing TPJ is that it is very difficult to robustly drive fMRI BOLD responses in the visual attentional TPJ in individual subjects; a primary goal of the present study was to develop and validate a paradigm that supports identification of visual attentional responsive regions of TPJ in individual subjects. Here, we demonstrate that the presentation of vivid, novel visual oddball stimuli recruits two spatially distinct regions in the TPJ, a posterior region (TPJSTS) that spans both banks of the STS and an anterior region (TPJSMG) that lies in the ventral portion of the SMG of the parietal lobe. Moreover, we demonstrate that these regions can be reliably identified in individual subjects. Since our task paradigm included both oddball events and spatial cueing components, we were able to validate that bilateral TPJSTS, identified in individual subjects with oddball images, also displays cue validity effects in a spatial reorienting task. These data demonstrate that right and left TPJSTS are visually driven regions that are modulated by exogenous attention. Our findings also support a similar account for left TPJSMG. In contrast, right TPJSMG did not conclusively exhibit modulation by exogenous attention and activation of this region in the ViNO paradigm may reflect only the visual drive or other factors unrelated to exogenous attention.

Our approach to developing an effective visual TPJ localizer builds on the findings of three prior studies in particular (Asplund et al., 2010; Horiguchi et al., 2016; Dugué et al., 2018). The use of visual drive to activate one or more visual TPJ regions has been shown to be effective in individual subjects (Horiguchi et al., 2016; Dugué et al., 2018). The reliance solely on the visual drive may be somewhat underpowered. Horiguchi et al. (2016) observed one visual TPJ in the right hemisphere of six of seven tested subjects but did not consistently observe visual drive within left hemisphere TPJ. However, Dugué et al. (2018) reliably observed multiple visual TPJ regions in each hemisphere of five subjects. Here, we sought to maximize visual drive by using highly vivid, full-field image stimuli. Horiguchi et al. (2016) additionally observed that visual oddball stimuli drove TPJ responses more strongly than did expected visual stimuli. Our approach combined the visual drive (sensory along with possible semantic and scene processing effects) and oddball factors (attention, surprise) by making each of the vivid images themselves the oddball stimuli. Our task paradigm was also inspired by the results of Asplund et al. (2010) who found that TPJ responses strongly attenuate with repeated oddball stimulus presentations; novelty is key. In order to maximize the response to each vivid oddball stimulus, we utilized a large set of oddball images and presented each stimulus only once to a subject. This vivid, novel oddball approach proved to be very effective in producing robust cortical responses in individual subjects. TPJSTS was identified in 10/10 right hemispheres and 9/10 left hemispheres, while TPJSMG was identified in 6/10 right hemispheres and 5/10 left hemispheres. One caveat is that since eye movements were not monitored in these experiments, we cannot rule out possible contributions from eye movements. We suggest that because the oddball component of this task was task-irrelevant, the ViNO paradigm could be adapted for integration with other tasks. Task-irrelevant oddballs could be embedded in a wide variety of perceptual or cognitive tasks during fMRI scanning to localize TPJ regions in individual subjects while also collecting data in a primary task.

Corbetta and Shulman (2002); in delineating a Ventral Attention Network for processing of stimulus-driven attention, identified a node spanning the TPJ that ran from the superior temporal gyrus dorsally and anteriorly into the inferior parietal lobule. A number of more recent studies have suggested the existence of multiple functional subdivisions of TPJ (e.g., Igelström et al., 2015, 2016; Krall et al., 2015; Trautwein et al., 2016; Dugué et al., 2018); however, a consensus on nomenclature and functionality has yet to emerge. We identified two TPJ subdivisions serving attention and use anatomical landmarks to name them, TPJSTS and TPJSMG. While both TPJSTS and TPJSMG were identified bilaterally (see also DiQuattro and Geng, 2011; de Haan et al., 2015), our results in Figure 2 are also consistent with prior reports that TPJ attentional reorienting is stronger in right hemisphere (Kincade et al., 2005; Fox et al., 2006; Corbetta et al., 2008; Shulman et al., 2009; Doricchi et al., 2010).

Although some prior studies examined more diverse cognitive phenomena, such as theory of mind, episodic memory retrieval, and default mode network effects, there are some noteworthy comparisons. Igelström et al. (2016) identified five TPJ subdivisions, with TPJc and TPJa exhibiting some functional similarities to TPJSTS and TPJSMG, respectively.

Dugué et al. (2018) also observed multiple visually responsive TPJ regions, two in the LH (vTPJant, vTPJpost) and three in the RH (vTPJant, vTPJcent, vTPJpost). In contrast, Horiguchi et al. (2016) observed a single right-lateralized “visual TPJ” that responds to simple visual stimuli (drifting gratings, low-contrast dartboards) independent of context or expectation. Based on location and functional responses, our TPJSMG may correspond to vTPJant (Dugué et al., 2018) and/or vTPJ (Horiguchi et al., 2016). Dugué et al. (2018) reported that vTPJant was not modulated by exogenous nor endogenous attention in either hemisphere. Relatedly, we observed that the spatial cueing paradigm failed to attentionally modulate RH TPJSMG; however, in an intriguing hemispheric asymmetry, LH TPJSMG was attentionally modulated. Dugué et al. (2018) reported visually-driven attentional modulations in two subregions (vTPJcent, vTPJpost) lying on opposite banks of the STS. These regions appear similar to the present report of bilateral TPJSTS, but there are notable differences. Dugué et al. (2018) reported that vTPJpost was observed bilaterally, while vTPJcent was observed in only one of five LHs. Although we also observed weaker TPJSTS in LH than in RH, we observed no activation differences between banks of LH TPJSTS in contrast to the Dugué et al. (2018) report. We observed distinct activation domains on each bank of STS in some individuals and single activation domains spanning both banks in other individuals. Our data do not resolve the question of whether these regions are functionally distinct from each other; however, we do not find evidence to support their reported hemispheric asymmetry for vTPJcent, despite examining twice as many subjects. The discrepancies between the two sets of findings deserve further investigation; however, the similarities are notable.

The ability to localize TPJ regions in individual subjects should prove useful in more thoroughly characterizing the functionality of these regions. Attentional capacities vary across individuals (e.g., Fan et al., 2002) and these individual differences are relevant for Hemispatial Neglect Syndrome (e.g., Corbetta and Shulman, 2011; Rengachary et al., 2011) and other clinical patient groups (e.g., Bayliss and Tipper, 2010). The vivid, novel oddball contrast provides a novel approach to examine structure-function correlations. Moreover, the ability to identify attentional-driven regions of TPJ in individual subjects should be useful to address controversies over the precise functional organization of the TPJ. Notably, rh TPJ has been suggested to play a key role in temporal attention (Agosta et al., 2017). Another intriguing alternative hypothesis of TPJ function is the contextual updating model (DiQuattro and Geng, 2011; Geng and Vossel, 2013; DiQuattro et al., 2014), which relates to the P300 in the electroencephalography (EEG) literature (Donchin, 1981; Knight et al., 1989). The P300, or “oddball response,” is hypothesized to reflect updating of an internal model of the environment based on new sensory information or rare, unexpected events. Geng and Vossel (2013) propose that the computations performed by the TPJ are not right-lateralized or specific to attentional reorienting, but are instead reflective of contextual updating to an internal model of the environment. Although further research is required, the results described here are consistent with a contextual updating function of TPJ.

In summary, we demonstrate that the use of novel and vivid visual oddball stimuli can reliably reveal two bilateral visual attention regions in TPJ of individual subjects. Importantly, one region, the RH TPJSTS corresponds to the region activated in a Posner-style spatial cueing task. The vivid, novel oddball contrast is task-irrelevant and could be integrated with a broad range of other tasks. We suggest that careful within-subject mapping across multiple functional tasks (e.g., attention, theory of mind) could prove useful for yielding a clearer understanding of the functional organization across the broad extent of the TPJ.

Ethics Statement

All research participants were recruited from Boston University and the greater Boston community and gave written informed consent. All experiments were approved by the Institutional Review Board of Boston University. Participants were compensated monetarily for their time.

Author Contributions

KD and DS designed the study and drafted the article. KD, MR and EL collected the data. KD and EL analyzed the data.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Eye Institute, National Institutes of Health (R01-EY022229 to DS, F32-HD089514) and a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship (DGE-1247312 to KD).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Devin Buckley for assistance in collecting oddball stimuli, Samantha Michalka for assistance with data collection, and Samson Mehl, James Brissenden, and Inderdeep Singh for technical assistance.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00424/full#supplementary-material.

References

Agosta, S., Magnago, D., Tyler, S., Grossman, E., Galante, E., Ferraro, F., et al. (2017). The pivotal role of the right parietal lobe in temporal attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 805–815. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Asplund, C. L., Todd, J. J., Snyder, A. P., and Marois, R. (2010). A central role for the lateral prefrontal cortex in goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 507–512. doi: 10.1038/nn.2509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2006). Visuospatial reorienting signals in the human temporo-parietal junction are independent of response selection. Eur. J. Neurosci. 23, 591–596. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.04573.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayliss, A. P., and Tipper, S. P. (2010). Gaze and arrow cueing of attention reveals individual differences along the autism spectrum as a function of target context. Br. J. Psychol. 96, 95–114. doi: 10.1348/000712604X15626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bledowski, C., Prvulovic, D., Goebel, R., Zanella, F. E., and Linden, D. E. J. (2004). Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study. NeuroImage 22, 530–540. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., and Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. Cereb. Cortex 11, 825–836. doi: 10.1093/cercor/11.9.825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., and Krienen, F. M. (2013). The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 648–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., Weidner, R., Vossel, S., Weiss, P. H., and Fink, G. R. (2012). Neural mechanisms of attentional reorienting in three-dimensional space. J. Neurosci. 32, 13352–13362. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1772-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, V. P., Fannon, S., Lai, S., Benson, R., and Bauer, L. (2000). Responses to rare visual target and distractor stimuli using event-related fMRI. J. Neurophysiol. 83, 3133–3139. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.83.5.3133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3, 215–229. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2011). Spatial neglect and attention networks. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 34, 569–599. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Neural systems for visual orienting and their relationships to spatial working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 508–523. doi: 10.1162/089892902317362029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Kincade, J. M., Ollinger, J. M., McAvoy, M. P., and Shulman, G. L. (2000). Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 292–297. doi: 10.1038/73009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., Patel, G., and Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: from environment to theory of mind. Neuron 58, 306–324. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., and Sereno, M. I. (1993). Improved localizadon of cortical activity by combining EEG and MEG with MRI cortical surface reconstruction: a linear approach. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 5, 162–176. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.2.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dale, A. M., Fischl, B., and Sereno, M. I. (1999). Cortical surface-based analysis: I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. NeuroImage 9, 179–194. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, B., Christie, J., and Rorden, C. (2009). Temporal order judgments activate temporal parietal junction. J. Neurosci. 29, 3182–3188. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5793-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Haan, B., Bither, M., Brauer, A., and Karnath, H.-O. (2015). Neural correlates of spatial attention and target detection in a multi-target environment. Cereb. Cortex 25, 2321–2331. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiQuattro, N. E., and Geng, J. J. (2011). Contextual knowledge configures attentional control networks. J. Neurosci. 31, 18026–18035. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4040-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiQuattro, N. E., Sawaki, R., and Geng, J. J. (2014). Effective connectivity during feature-based attentional capture: evidence against the attentional reorienting hypothesis of TPJ. Cereb. Cortex 24, 3131–3141. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Donchin, E. (1981). Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!‥.Surprise? Psychophysiology 18, 493–513. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb01815.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doricchi, F., Macci, E., Silvetti, M., and Macaluso, E. (2010). Neural correlates of the spatial and expectancy components of endogenous and stimulus-driven orienting of attention in the posner task. Cereb. Cortex 20, 1574–1585. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downar, J., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J., and Davis, K. D. (2001). The effect of task relevance on the cortical response to changes in visual and auditory stimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 14, 1256–1267. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downar, J., Crawley, A. P., Mikulis, D. J., and Davis, K. D. (2002). A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. J. Neurophysiol. 87, 615–620. doi: 10.1152/jn.00636.2001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dugué, L., Merriam, E. P., Heeger, D. J., and Carrasco, M. (2018). Specific visual subregions of TPJ mediate reorienting of spatial attention. Cereb. Cortex 28, 2375–2390. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhx140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engell, A. D., and Haxby, J. V. (2007). Facial expression and gaze-direction in human superior temporal sulcus. Neuropsychologia 45, 3234–3241. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.06.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B. (2004). Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex. Cereb. Cortex 14, 11–22. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhg087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., and Dale, A. M. (2000). Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 97, 11050–11055. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Liu, A., and Dale, A. M. (2001). Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans. Med. Imaging 20, 70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., Busa, E., Albert, M., Dieterich, M., Haselgrove, C., et al. (2002). Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron 33, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00569-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Salat, D. H., van der Kouwe, A. J. W., Makris, N., Ségonne, F., Quinn, B. T., et al. (2004). Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage 23, S69–S84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., and Dale, A. M. (1999a). Cortical surface-based analysis II: inflation, flattening and a surface-based coordinate system. Neuroimage 9, 195–207. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1998.0396

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischl, B., Sereno, M. I., Tootell, R., and Dale, A. M. (1999b). High-resolution intersubject averaging and a coordinate system for the cortical surface. Hum. Brain Mapp. 8, 272–284. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-0193(1999)8:4<272::aid-hbm10>3.0.co;2-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, M. D., Corbetta, M., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., and Raichle, M. E. (2006). Spontaneous neuronal activity distinguishes human dorsal and ventral attention systems. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 103, 10046–10051. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0604187103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geng, J. J., and Mangun, G. R. (2011). Right temporoparietal junction activation by a salient contextual cue facilitates target discrimination. Neuroimage 54, 594–601. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geng, J. J., and Vossel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: contextual updating? Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 2608–2620. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giessing, C., Thiel, C. M., Rösler, F., and Fink, G. R. (2006). The modulatory effects of nicotine on parietal cortex activity in a cued target detection task depend on cue reliability. Neuroscience 137, 853–864. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillebert, C. R., Dyrholm, M., Vangkilde, S., Kyllingsbæk, S., Peeters, R., and Vandenberghe, R. (2012). Attentional priorities and access to short-term memory: parietal interactions. Neuroimage 62, 1551–1562. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greve, D. N., and Fischl, B. (2009). Accurate and robust brain image alignment using boundary-based registration. Neuroimage 48, 63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, S. W., and Marois, R. (2014). Functional fractionation of the stimulus-driven attention network. J. Neurosci. 34, 6958–6969. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4975-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, X., Jovicich, J., Salat, D., van der Kouwe, A., Quinn, B., Czanner, S., et al. (2006). Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: the effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer. Neuroimage 32, 180–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasson, U., Nir, Y., Levy, I., Fuhrmann, G., and Malach, R. (2004). Intersubject synchronization of cortical activity during natural vision. Science 303, 1634–1640. doi: 10.1126/science.1089506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Himmelbach, M., Erb, M., and Karnath, H.-O. (2006). Exploring the visual world: the neural substrate of spatial orienting. Neuroimage 32, 1747–1759. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horiguchi, H., Wandell, B. A., and Winawer, J. (2016). A predominantly visual subdivision of the right temporo-parietal junction (vTPJ). Cereb. Cortex 26, 639–646. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igelström, K. M., Webb, T. W., and Graziano, M. S. A. (2015). Neural processes in the human temporoparietal cortex separated by localized independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. 35, 9432–9445. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0551-15.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Igelström, K. M., Webb, T. W., Kelly, Y. T., and Graziano, M. S. A. (2016). Topographical organization of attentional, social and memory processes in the human temporoparietal cortex. eNeuro 3:ENEURO.0060-16.2016. doi: 10.1523/eneuro.0060-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Indovina, I., and Macaluso, E. (2007). Dissociation of stimulus relevance and saliency factors during shifts of visuospatial attention. Cereb. Cortex 17, 1701–1711. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jakobs, O., Langner, R., Caspers, S., Roski, C., Cieslik, E. C., Zilles, K., et al. (2012). Across-study and within-subject functional connectivity of a right temporo-parietal junction subregion involved in stimulus-context integration. Neuroimage 60, 2389–2398. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jovicich, J., Czanner, S., Greve, D., Haley, E., van der Kouwe, A., Gollub, R., et al. (2006). Reliability in multi-site structural MRI studies: effects of gradient non-linearity correction on phantom and human data. Neuroimage 30, 436–443. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.09.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, H. (2013). Involvement of the dorsal and ventral attention networks in oddball stimulus processing: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 2265–2284. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kincade, J. M., Abrams, R. A., Astafiev, S. V., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2005). An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study of voluntary and stimulus-driven orienting of attention. J. Neurosci. 25, 4593–4604. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0236-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krall, S. C., Rottschy, C., Oberwelland, E., Bzdok, D., Fox, P. T., Eickhoff, S. B., et al. (2015). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in attention and social interaction as revealed by ALE meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 220, 587–604. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0803-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knight, R. T., Scabini, D., Woods, D. L., and Clayworth, C. C. (1989). Contributions of temporal-parietal junction to the human auditory P3. Brain Res. 502, 109–116. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(89)90466-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kucyi, A., Hodaie, M., and Davis, K. D. (2012). Lateralization in intrinsic functional connectivity of the temporoparietal junction with salience- and attention-related brain networks. J. Neurophysiol. 108, 3382–3392. doi: 10.1152/jn.00674.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. M., and McCarthy, G. (2016). Functional heterogeneity and convergence in the right temporoparietal junction. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1108–1116. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lepsien, J., and Pollmann, S. (2002). Covert reorienting and inhibition of return: an event-related fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 127–144. doi: 10.1162/089892902317236795

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linden, D. E., Prvulovic, D., Formisano, E., Völlinger, M., Zanella, F. E., Goebel, R., et al. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of target detection: an fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks. Cereb. Cortex 9, 815–823. doi: 10.1093/cercor/9.8.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luks, T. L., Oliveira, M., Possin, K. L., Bird, A., Miller, B. L., Weiner, M. W., et al. (2010). Atrophy in two attention networks is associated with performance on a Flanker task in neurodegenerative disease. Neuropsychologia 48, 165–170. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., and Driver, J. (2002). Supramodal effects of covert spatial orienting triggered by visual or tactile events. Am. J. Infect. Control 14, 389–401. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mars, R. B., Neubert, F.-X., Noonan, M. P., Sallet, J., Toni, I., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2012a). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 189–189. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mars, R. B., Sallet, J., Schuffelgen, U., Jbabdi, S., Toni, I., and Rushworth, M. F. S. (2012b). Connectivity-based subdivisions of the human right “temporoparietal junction area”: evidence for different areas participating in different cortical networks. Cereb. Cortex 22, 1894–1903. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayer, A. R., Dorflinger, J. M., Rao, S. M., and Seidenberg, M. (2004). Neural networks underlying endogenous and exogenous visual-spatial orienting. Neuroimage 23, 534–541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Michalka, S. W., Kong, L., Rosen, M. L., Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., and Somers, D. C. (2015). Short-term memory for space and time flexibly recruit complementary sensory-biased frontal lobe attention networks. Neuron 87, 882–892. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.07.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitchell, J. P. (2008). Activity in right temporo-parietal junction is not selective for theory-of-mind. Cereb. Cortex 18, 262–271. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mueller, S., Wang, D., Fox, M. D., Yeo, B. T. T., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., et al. (2013). Individual variability in functional connectivity architecture of the human brain. Neuron 77, 586–595. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasr, S., Stemmann, H., Vanduffel, W., and Tootell, R. B. H. (2014). Increased visual stimulation systematically decreases activity in lateral intermediate cortex. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4009–4028. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niogi, S., Mukherjee, P., Ghajar, J., and McCandliss, B. D. (2010). Individual differences in distinct components of attention are linked to anatomical variations in distinct white matter tracts. Front. Neuroanat. 4:2. doi: 10.3389/neuro.05.002.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pagnoni, G. (2012). Dynamical properties of bold activity from the ventral posteromedial cortex associated with meditation and attentional skills. J. Neurosci. 32, 5242–5249. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4135-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ptak, R., and Schnider, A. (2011). The attention network of the human brain: relating structural damage associated with spatial neglect to functional imaging correlates of spatial attention. Neuropsychologia 49, 3063–3070. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rengachary, J., He, B. J., Shulman, G. L., and Corbetta, M. (2011). A behavioral analysis of spatial neglect and its recovery after stroke. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 5:29. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2011.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, C. C., and Driver, J. (2006). Attentional preparation for a lateralized visual distractor: behavioral and fMRI evidence. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 18, 522–538. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.4.522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saxe, R. (2010). “The right temporo-parietal junction: a specific brain region for thinking about thoughts,” in Handbook of Theory of Mind., eds A. Leslie, and T. German (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 1–35.

Google Scholar

Scholz, J., Triantafyllou, C., Whitfield-Gabrieli, S., Brown, E. N., and Saxe, R. (2009). Distinct regions of right temporo-parietal junction are selective for theory of mind and exogenous attention. PLoS One 4:e4869. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0004869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ségonne, F., Dale, A. M., Busa, E., Glessner, M., Salat, D., Hahn, H. K., et al. (2004). A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage 22, 1060–1075. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Serences, J. T., Shomstein, S., Leber, A. B., Golay, X., Egeth, H. E., and Yantis, S. (2005). Coordination of voluntary and stimulus-driven attentional control in human cortex. Psychol. Sci. 16, 114–122. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00791.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, G. L., Astafiev, S. V., Franke, D., Pope, D. L. W., Snyder, A. Z., McAvoy, M. P., et al. (2009). Interaction of stimulus-driven reorienting and expectation in ventral and dorsal frontoparietal and basal ganglia-cortical networks. J. Neurosci. 29, 4392–4407. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.5609-08.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shulman, G. L., McAvoy, M. P., Cowan, M. C., Astafiev, S. V., Tansy, A. P., d’Avossa, G., et al. (2003). Quantitative analysis of attention and detection signals during visual search. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3384–3397. doi: 10.1152/jn.00343.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silvetti, M., Lasaponara, S., Lecce, F., and Cerebral, A. D. (2016). The response of the left ventral attentional system to invalid targets and its implication for the spatial neglect syndrome: a multivariate fMRI investigation. Cereb. Cortex 26, 4551–4562. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somers, D. C., and Sheremata, S. L. (2013). Attention maps in the brain. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 327–340. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutton, S. S., Braren, M. M., Zubin, J. J., and John, E. R. E. (1965). Evoked-potential correlates of stimulus uncertainty. Science 150, 1187–1188. doi: 10.1126/science.150.3700.1187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiel, C. M., Zilles, K., and Fink, G. R. (2004). Cerebral correlates of alerting, orienting and reorienting of visuospatial attention: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage 21, 318–328. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Todd, J. J., Fougnie, D., and Marois, R. (2005). Visual short-term memory load suppresses temporo-parietal junction activity and induces inattentional blindness. Psychol. Sci. 16, 965–972. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01645.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trautwein, F.-M., Singer, T., and Kanske, P. (2016). Stimulus-driven reorienting impairs executive control of attention: evidence for a common bottleneck in anterior insula. Cereb. Cortex 26, 4136–4147. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsao, D. Y., and Livingstone, M. S. (2008). Mechanisms of face perception. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 31, 411–437. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tyler, S. C., Dasgupta, S., Agosta, S., Battelli, L., and Grossman, E. D. (2015). Functional connectivity of parietal cortex during temporal selective attention. Cortex 65, 195–207. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uncapher, M. R., Hutchinson, J. B., and Wagner, A. D. (2011). Dissociable effects of top-down and bottom-up attention during episodic encoding. J. Neurosci. 31, 12613–12628. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0152-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Essen, D. C. (2005). A population-average, landmark- and surface-based (PALS) atlas of human cerebral cortex. Neuroimage 28, 635–662. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetter, P., Butterworth, B., and Bahrami, B. (2011). A candidate for the attentional bottleneck: set-size specific modulation of the right tpj during attentive enumeration. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 728–736. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossel, S., Thiel, C. M., and Fink, G. R. (2006). Cue validity modulates the neural correlates of covert endogenous orienting of attention in parietal and frontal cortex. Neuroimage 32, 1257–1264. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vossel, S., Weidner, R., Thiel, C. M., and Fink, G. R. (2009). What is “Odd” in Posner’s location-cueing paradigm? Neural responses to unexpected location and feature changes compared. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 30–41. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warbrick, T., Reske, M., and Shah, N. J. (2013). Do EEG paradigms work in fMRI? Varying task demands in the visual oddball paradigm: implications for task design and results interpretation. Neuroimage 77, 177–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weissman, D. H., and Prado, J. (2012). Heightened activity in a key region of the ventral attention network is linked to reduced activity in a key region of the dorsal attention network during unexpected shifts of covert visual spatial attention. Neuroimage 61, 798–804. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woldorff, M. G., Hazlett, C. J., Fichtenholtz, H. M., Weissman, D. H., Dale, A. M., and Song, A. W. (2004). Functional parcellation of attentional control regions of the brain. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 16, 149–165. doi: 10.1162/089892904322755638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, Q., Chang, C.-F., Xi, S., Huang, I.-W., Liu, Z., Juan, C.-H., et al. (2015). A critical role of temporoparietal junction in the integration of top-down and bottom-up attentional control. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4317–4333. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, L., and Saxe, R. (2008). The neural basis of belief encoding and integration in moral judgment. Neuroimage 40, 1912–1920. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.01.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, L., and Saxe, R. (2009). Innocent intentions: a correlation between forgiveness for accidental harm and neural activity. Neuropsychologia 47, 2065–2072. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention, temporal lobe, parietal lobe, magnetic resonance imaging, individual-subject approach, TPJ

Citation: Devaney KJ, Rosen ML, Levin EJ and Somers DC (2019) Identification of Visual Attentional Regions of the Temporoparietal Junction in Individual Subjects using a Vivid, Novel Oddball Paradigm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:424. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00424

Received: 13 November 2018; Accepted: 14 November 2019;
Published: 11 December 2019.

Edited by:

Michael A. Silver, University of California, Berkeley, United States

Reviewed by:

Taylor Whittington Webb, Princeton University, United States
Elisha P. Merriam, National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), United States

Copyright © 2019 Devaney, Rosen, Levin and Somers. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: David C. Somers, somers@bu.edu

ORCID: David C. Somers orcid.org/0000-0002-4169-5895

Download