Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 02 October 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology

MOCSE Centered on Students: Validation of Learning Demands and Teacher Support Scales

  • 1Developmental and Educational Psychology, Universitat Jaume I, Castellón de la Plana, Spain
  • 2Developmental and Educational Psychology, University of Valencia, Valencia, Spain

Based on The Educational Situation Quality Model (MOCSE, acronym in Spanish) framework, the primary objective of this study is to test the factorial validity and reliability of two MOCSE measure instruments referred to the preactional-decisional phase, specifically to learning demands and teacher supports perceived by students to overcome such demands in the classroom context. The participants were 357 Spanish undergraduate students. The data obtained by exploratory and confirmatory factor analyses revealed that the “Learning Demands Scale” (MOCSE-LDS) has a two-factor structure: perceived desirability and feasibility of demands. The data also revealed that the “Teacher Support Questionnaire” (MOCSE-TSQ) is composed of ten independent factors or subscales with good psychometric validity and reliability properties. Finally, the Student’s t-test generally indicated that the constructs considered in the instruments did not differ in gender terms. In short, the results obtained for the validity and reliability of the two tested instruments were good. Thus, the application of instruments MOCSE-LDS and MOCSE-TSQ is satisfactorily supported by empirical data. The resulting scales can be useful for researchers and teachers. On the one hand, this study provides researchers with two valid and reliable tools that may contribute to investigate students’ motivation in the university classroom context based on MOCSE postulates. On the other hand, the two tested instruments may provide teachers and school psychologists with important information to implement preventive or intervention actions to improve students’ intention to learn. Teachers may also use them to evaluate their own teaching and to research their own classrooms. The implications for education according to MOCSE postulates are discussed.

Introduction

The Educational Situation Quality Model (MOCSE, acronym in Spanish), devised by Doménech-Betoret (2006; 2013; 2018), is an instructional-motivational model that explains the functioning of an educational setting by organizing and relating the most important variables which, according to the literature, contribute to explain learning outcomes. Specifically, the model integrates motivational constructs and approaches from relevant psychological theories such as: Job Demands-Resources Model (JD-R) (Demerouti et al., 2001; Bakker and Demerouti, 2007, 2008), Expectancy-Value Theory (Eccles and Wigfield, 2002; Eccles, 2009), Achievement goal theory (Dweck and Leggett, 1988; Nicholls, 1989; Ames, 1992; Wigfield and Eccles, 2000), and Theory of Action Control (Heckhausen and Kuhl, 1985; Kuhl and Beckmann, 1985). It offers researchers a new tool to study how an educational setting operates and provides the teacher with a methodological procedure to guide and improve teacher practice. The model is made up of four sequential stages: (1) Student cognitive evaluation (learning demands and supports); (2) Intention to learn activation; (3) Action plan and teaching-learning process; (4) Learning outcomes. The stages are distributed into three broad phases: Preactional-decisional phase (Phase I), Actional phase (Phase II), Reflectional phase (Phase III).

The model starts from a basic premise, which claims that “learning” requires students’ intention to learn to be activated at the beginning of the educational process, and it has to remain active until the process ends. Students’ intention to learn is generated or activated on the first days of the teaching-learning process according to the information they receive from the environment in terms of the demands required and the supports/resources received. However as the course unfolds, students’ perceptions are continuously updated and changing as a result of the constant (re)appraisals made by them (Doménech-Betoret, 2018; Doménech-Betoret et al., 2019). The model centered on students is displayed in Figure 1. See Doménech-Betoret (2018) for a profounder understanding of the model.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. MOCSE diagram: Main course sequence actions for students. 1Connected with students’ interests, needs, and academic level (meaningful demands, Doménech-Betoret, 2018).

As seen in Figure 1, students’ perceptions of learning demands, and the supports they are provided with to overcome such demands (Stage 1), are assumed to predict intention to learn (Stage 2) which, in turn, affects the role adopted by students in classrooms (e.g., active-passive, engagement-disengagement, etc.) (Stage 3) which, in turn, finally has an effect on learning outcomes, such as academic achievement and course satisfaction (Stage 4). The whole model pivots around intention to learn (Stage 2), where the components from Stage I, such as demands and supports, are considered antecedents or predictive variables, whereas those from Stages 3 and 4 are taken as consequences or outcome variables. The temporal dimension of the MOCSE model is based on the Theory of Action Control (Heckhausen and Kuhl, 1985; Kuhl and Beckmann, 1985) as we explain below.

In the present study, we paid attention to Phase I (Preactional-decisional phase), specifically to the learning demands and supports (Stage 1) perceived by students, which are assumed to predict intention to learn (Stage 2) when motivational processes are involved. During the first process, initial wishes, desires and hopes are evaluated in terms of their chances of being fulfilled and transformed into personal goals (commitment) (Doménech-Betoret, 2018).

Contribution of the Job Demands and Resources Model (JD-R) and the Expectancy-Value Theory to Explain the Preactional-Decisional Phase of MOCSE

According to the MOCSE Model, before making the decision to assume and commit to achieve (or not) the learning objectives (goals) planned in a specific subject matter, students follow several cognitive processes that can be explained by the JD-R Model (Evaluation Process) and Expectancy-Value Theory (Anticipatory cognitive process). Accordingly, and based on the MOCSE postulates, this phase predetermines, from the beginning of the course, the degree of student involvement in the teaching-learning process.

According to the Job Demands-Resources (JD-R) Model (Demerouti et al., 2001; Bakker and Demerouti, 2007, 2008), burnout and work engagement are two psychological states that play a key role in the workplace, and job demands and job resources/supports evoke two underlying psychological processes: (a) an energetic process during which high job demands lead to burnout and, in turn, they affect health; (b) a motivational process during which job resources promote engagement and, in turn, organizational commitment, but burnout also increases when job resources are lacking (Schaufeli and Bakker, 2004). The JD-R Model was traditionally utilized in the job context. The MOCSE model applies this theory to the school context.

Intention to learn (Stage 2) is a complex construct in which multiple variables are involved. For us, intention to learn has the same meaning as motivation to learn. Intention is considered the immediate antecedent of action (Doménech-Betoret, 2018). Intention to learn is basically made up of the anticipatory cognitive motivators proposed by the Expectancy-Value Theory. Authors from this tradition, such as Pintrich and de Groot (1990), distinguish three general categories of motivational constructs that are relevant for motivation in educational contexts: (a) individual perceptions and beliefs about the ability to perform a task/subject (e.g., expectancy of success, expectancy of control, etc.), Will I be able to pass this subject?; (b) the reasons or intentions to get involved in a task/subject (e.g., goals, value of the subject, etc.), Why am I going to get involved in this subject?; (c) affective reactions to a task/subject (e.g., expectations of enjoyment, feeling stressed, etc.). How will I feel in this subject during the course?

In the Preactional-decisional phase (I), MOCSE attempts to bridge both theories (JD-R and Expectancy-Value Theory) to explain how students make decisions to be involved in the teaching-learning process of a specific subject. Accordingly, and based on prior research (Patrick et al., 2007; Wentzel, 2009; Lin-Siegler et al., 2016), the model starts with a basic premise: “the perception that each student forms of; first, the scheduled learning demands that they must complete to pass a specific subject; second, the support that they perceive as being provided, mainly from the teacher and family, to face these demands, is crucial to activate students’ intention to learn” (Doménech-Betoret et al., 2019, p. 3). Moreover, MOCSE, in line with the JD-R Model, defends that demands are probably more related to affective reactions, whereas supports (from teacher, peers, family, etc.) are probably more related to expectancy beliefs (expectancy of success, expectancy of control, etc.).

Regarding learning demands, the “Model of Action Phases” (Heckhausen and Gollwitzer, 1987), based on the Theory of Action Control (Heckhausen and Kuhl, 1985; Kuhl and Beckmann, 1985), postulates that the person’s decision to set a goal intention is commonly assumed to depend on both the desirability and feasibility (Fishbein and Ajzen, 2010) of demands. Using the same reasoning, in the classroom context it also depends on both the desirability and feasibility of the academic-learning demands planned for a specific subject. In the classroom context, students’ subjective evaluation of both desirability and feasibility will be better insofar as demands connect with students’ interests, needs and academic level (meaningful demands, see Doménech-Betoret, 2018).

Regarding teaching support, prior research has shown that positive students’ perceptions of emotional/affective and instrumental/academic teacher support are positively related to students’ intrinsic motivation and positive emotional responses (Katz et al., 2009; Roorda et al., 2011; Federici and Skaalvik, 2014a). In the same vein, a supportive student-teacher relationship is particularly relevant for student motivation (Roorda et al., 2011; Dietrich et al., 2015). Finally, based on the Social Cognitive Theory (Bandura, 1986), Federici and Skaalvik (2014b) argue that teachers who provide both emotional/affective and instructional/academic support likely persuade students to believe in their ability and, as a consequence, to exert more effort to complete and master their learning activities.

In short, the above empirical findings seem to indicate a clear positive association between JD-R Model (demands and supports) and Intention to learn in the classroom context in accordance with the MOCSE postulates.

Demands and Supports for Students in the Classroom Context

In the JD-R Model context, job demands are defined as “physical, psychological, social or organizational aspects of work that require physical and/or psychological effort (cognitive or emotional), and are associated with a certain physiological and/or psychological cost” (Bakker and Demerouti, 2007, p. 312). Job resources/supports refer “to the physical, psychological, social, or organizational aspects of the job that may reduce job demands and the associated physiological and psychological cost, are functional for achieving work goals, and stimulate personal growth, learning and development” (Hakanen et al., 2006, p. 497).

Applying this theory to the school context first requires a thorough analysis of what kind of learning demands related to a specific subject matter must students assume and, second, what kind of support (from teacher, family, etc.) should students be provided with during the curse to complete these demands. According to Lin-Siegler et al. (2016), students’ beliefs about themselves and their environment influence their motivation. Previous research based on the Job Demands-Resources Model (JD-R) (Demerouti et al., 2001) suggests that students’ perceptions of the resources/supports they are provided with (by teacher, family, etc.) to complete learning demands may have strongly influenced the variables related to intention to learn (motivational processes), such as expectancy-value believes (Abellán-Roselló, 2016) and goal pursuit (Wentzel et al., 2010; King and McInerney, 2014).

Learning Demands

Learning demands refer to a specific subject in the classroom context, and basically to the tasks that students have to complete (procedural demands) and the contents they have to study (conceptual demands) to fulfill the programmed objectives and pass the subject (Doménech-Betoret et al., 2019). Students are expected to assume and pursue the learning objectives/goals planned by the teacher to fulfill them.

The concept of intention is central to theorizing on human goal striving. “In traditional theories on goal striving, the intention to achieve a certain goal is seen as an immediate determinant of goal achievement” (Brandstätter et al., 2001, p. 946). Accordingly, for decades, research was centered to identify the factors that determine the formation of strong intentions (e.g., Atkinson, 1957; Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975; Ajzen and Fishbein, 1980; Heckhausen et al., 1985). The Theory of Action Control (Heckhausen and Kuhl, 1985; Kuhl and Beckmann, 1985) extended this concern to the gap between intention and action. Integrating motivational and volitional (Executive motivation) aspects have contributed to a more comprehensive representation of the learning process. This theory promotes a new approach centered on the temporal dimension of motivation.

Based on the Theory of Action Control (Heckhausen and Kuhl, 1985; Kuhl and Beckmann, 1985), the “Model of Action Phases” (Heckhausen and Gollwitzer, 1987) applies psychological theories about cognition and motivation to investigate the processes that occur during goal pursuit, from setting a goal to fulfilling it. It offers a time perspective on goal striving and thus takes an integrative view by examining both goal setting and goal implementation within a single conceptual framework. According to this model, goal pursuit is carried out in four successive action phases: the predecisional, preactional, actional and postactional phases (for a summary, see Gollwitzer, 1990). The first stage of the motivated behavioral process (predecisional) is to choose among competing wishes and turning them into binding goals (goal intentions). Usually, the variables related to expectancy-value theories are employed to explain the forming of a goal intention. Forming a goal intention is the first step to fulfill a certain desired outcome. During this process, people deliberate (deliberative mindset) and carry out an analysis of a goal’s feasibility and desirability (Gollwitzer, 1990; Gollwitzer and Bayer, 1999). In short, the decision to set a goal intention is commonly assumed to depend on both desirability and feasibility (Fishbein and Ajzen, 2010). That is, goals are most likely to be set when the anticipated endstate is subjectively evaluated as both desirable (I want X!) and feasible (Is X affordable for me?). From a psychological perspective, “a strong desire to attain a goal is not sufficient for the formation of a goal intention; in addition, one must be confident that the chances of attaining the goal are high” (Doménech-Betoret et al., 2019, p. 4). In the classroom context, students’ subjective evaluation of both desirability and feasibility will be better insofar as the teacher adapts learning demands to students’ personal characteristics; that is, to what extent demands connect with students’ interests, needs and academic level (meaningful demands, see Doménech-Betoret, 2018).

Teacher-External Support Resources to Complete the Required Learning Demands

According to the self-determination view, teacher support occurs when students perceive cognitive, emotional or autonomy-oriented support from a teacher during their learning process. According to the broad perspective of the social support model, based on Tardy’s (1985) teacher support is defined as a teacher giving informational, instrumental, emotional, or appraisal support to students in any environment. As we can see, there is lack of consistency in the definition and terminology used with the supports provided by teachers, but most authors usually distinguish between instructional-instrumental and affective-social supports (Doménech-Betoret, 2018). Instructional-instrumental support provided by teachers aims to help students’ content domain and to achieve learning demands. The affective-social support provided by teachers aims to meet students’ psychological needs and wishes in the classroom context. Instructional-instrumental support is related to academic dimension, whereas affective-social support is related to intrapersonal or interpersonal dimensions. Teacher affective-social support enhances a teacher’s relationship with students. Teachers who show care and concern for their students receive the same treatment from students by, for instance, adhering to classroom norms (Chiu and Chow, 2011). In the tested instrument, we considered both instructional-instrumental and affective-social supports (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Characteristics of the considered teacher support variables.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedure

The sample consisted of 357 Spanish undergraduate students, of whom 56 were male (15.7%), and 301 were female (84.3%). They were aged between 19 and 50 years (M = 22.17, SD = 4.22). The participants studied the Educational Psychology and Education degree at two Universities located in east Spain. The participants completed an online version of the two measure instruments during a class session. Participation in the study was completely voluntary. Confidentiality and personal data protection were guaranteed in accordance with current Spanish laws.

Measures

The last versions of two self-report measure instruments tested for validation in the current study are described below. Both instruments were originally constructed in accordance with the MOCSE principles and theoretical directions. They have been periodically revised and refined over time based on previous research conducted in the university context (Doménech-Betoret, 2006, 2012; Doménech-Betoret et al., 2014; Doménech-Betoret, 2018). Students completed an online version of both instruments in class.

Learning Demands Scale (MOCSE-LDS)

This scale comprises 17 items and was devised to measure students’ perception of learning demands in a specific subject matter in desirability and feasibility terms. Students indicated their level of agreement on a Likert response scale ranging from 1 (totally agree) to 6 (totally disagree).

External Support: Teacher Support Questionnaire (MOCSE-TSQ)

This self-report questionnaire comprises 72 items distributed on 10 scales. It was devised to measure students’ perception of teaching support in a specific subject matter to help students to achieve learning demands. Students indicated their level of agreement on a Likert response scale ranging from 1 (totally agree) to 6 (totally disagree).

Finally, an additional shortened questionnaire to measure intention to learn components was used to explore the relations between demands-supports variables (from Stage I) and intention to learn components (from Stage 2). See Doménech-Betoret et al. (2019) to get the complete questionnaire.

– The Intention to Learn Questionnaire (MOCSE-ILQ). It is made up of two basic dimensions:

(I) Expectancy Beliefs. This scale measures students’ anticipatory cognitive responses and emotional reactions. It is composed of two factors: 1.1. Expectancy of Success and 1.2. Process Expectancy. The first factor (Expectancy of Success) is made up of 10 items (α = 0.92). Response scale: completely disagree (1) to completely agree (6). This construct comprises both self-efficacy expectancy and outcome expectancy (Liem et al., 2008). The second factor (Process Expectancy) is made up of 10 items (α = 0.96). Response scale: quite unlikely (1) to quite likely (6). This construct refers to the affective reactions that students expect to experience in their interaction with the teacher and subject during the course.

(II) Achievement Goals. It measures the achievement goal setting by students according to the degree of commitment that they are willing to make regarding learning demands. Response scale: Completely disagree (1) to Completely agree (6). It is composed of two factors: 2.1. Mastery goals and 2.2. Avoidance goals. The first factor (Mastery goals) is made up of five items (α = 0.96). Students who adopt a mastery goal focus on improving their competence and progress in an academic task/subject. The second factor (Avoidance goals) is made up of four items (α = 0.91). Students who adopt avoidance goals make the minimum effort, or even try to avoid learning, and work avoidance represent the absence of an achievement goal (Elliot, 1999).

Data Analysis

First, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted on each instrument (previous version) to examine the latent factor structure using SPSS package 25.00 (IBM Corp, 2018). An observed measure was obtained by averaging the items included in each factor or subscale. Second, a Confirmatory Factor analysis (CFA) was performed to confirm the factor structure obtained with the EFA. The goodness-of-fit statistics of the hypothesized model was computed using the EQS program (Bentler, 2006). Raw data were used as the data matrix. As the chi-square test is sensitive to sample size, using relative fit indices like CFI, the NNFI and RMSEA is highly recommended (Bentler, 1990). Values up to 0.08 for RMSEA indicate an acceptable fit, whereas values above 0.08 indicate a poor fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1993). For NNFI and CFI, values above 0.90 (Hoyle, 1995), or even 0.95 (Hu and Bentler, 1999), were fixed as the cut-off point. Third, Cronbach’ α for examining the instruments’ reliability was calculated. Finally, gender differences were examined with the Student’s t-test for independent samples.

Results

Learning Demands Scale (MOCSE-LDS)

Exploratory Factor Analysis

An EFA (principal component method with varimax rotation) was conducted on the 17-item scale to examine the latent factor structure. Two factors, regarding the desirability and feasibility concepts with learning goals, were extracted (eigenvalue > 1). They accounted for 67.374% of total variance. Factor 1 (Desirability) was made up of 11 items and accounted for 42.19% of variance. Factor 2 (Feasibility) was made up of six items and accounted for 25.18% of variance.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The two-factor structure scale obtained with the EFA was tested to carry out the CFA. The scale factor structure was optimized when six items were removed following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test in the EQS program. Then the 11-item scale was tested again. As multivariate kurtosis (Mardias’s coefficient = 29.5452, normalized estimate = 16.4816) indicated that normal distribution was violated, the ML robust method of estimation was used. The obtained fit indices (χ2 = 104.662; p = 0.0000, d.f. = 43; NNFI = 0.971; CFI = 0.977; RMSEA = 0.064) showed a good data fit for the 11-item two-factor structure scale. See Table 2 for more details.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. MOCSE-LDS factor structure, items’ standardized coefficients and their contribution to the corresponding factor (R-squared).

Reliability Analysis Findings as a Result of the CFA

The Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency coefficient for the total questionnaire and each scale in the instrument was calculated. For the total questionnaire (MOCSE-LDS), the alpha coefficient was α = 0.772 Cronbach’s alpha values for these two scales are: α = 0.939 for Factor 1 (Desirability) and α = 0.916 for Factor 2 (Feasibility).

Teacher Support Questionnaire (MOCSE-TSQ)

Exploratory Factor Analysis

An EFA (principal component method with varimax rotation) was conducted on the total questionnaire composed of 72 items to examine the latent factor structure. A cross loading problem (>0.40, according to Stevens, 2002) was observed for 12 items, so they were removed and the EFA was repeated again with the remaining 60 items. Ten factors were extracted, with an eigenvalue higher than 1, which accounted for 74.04% of total variance. Factor 1 (Content comprehension support) was made up of 10 items, and accounted for 13.48% of variance; Factor 2 (Autonomy support) was made up of seven items and accounted for 8.10% of variance; Factor 3 (Relatedness support) was made up of 10 items and accounted for 8.03% of variance; Factor 4 (Peer support) was made of five items and accounted for 7.40% of variance; Factor 5 (Awakening interest in the subject) was made of five items and accounted for 6.89% of variance; Factor 6 (Acknowledging the student’s effort) was made of five items and accounted for 6.77% of variance; Factor 7 (Self-competency support) was made of five items and accounted for 6.58% of variance; Factor 8 (Guiding students in their learning) was made of five items and accounted for 6.54% of variance; Factor 9 (Providing didactic resources) was made of four items and accounted for 5.48% of variance; Factor 10 (Providing feedback) was made of four items and accounted for 4.73% of variance.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis

The 10-factor structure scale obtained with the EFA was tested to carry out a CFA. The CFA provides a regression coefficient and an error number showing the degree of relation between each item and its corresponding latent variable or factor. The scale factor structure was optimized when five items were removed following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test in the EQS program. Then the whole scale made up of 55 items was tested again. As multivariate kurtosis (Mardias’s coefficient = 760.176, normalized estimate = 90.568) indicated that normal distribution was violated, the ML robust method of estimation was used. The obtained fit indices (χ2 = 2,265.183; p = 0.0000, d.f. = 1385; NNFI = 0.929; CFI = 0.934; RMSEA = 0.042) indicated that the questionnaire factor structure, composed of 10 scales, satisfactorily fitted the data. See Table 3 for details.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. MOCSE-TSQ factor structure, items standardized coefficients and their contribution to the corresponding factor (R-squared).

Reliability Analysis Findings as a Result of the CFA

Cronbach’s alpha internal consistency coefficient for each factor/scale in the instrument (MOCSE-TSQ), was calculated. The Cronbach’s alpha values ranged between 0.95 (maximum) and 0.86 (minimum). See Table 2 for more details.

Student’s t-Test for Gender Differences

Gender differences were examined with the Student’s t-test for independent samples (male = 1; female = 2). As noted in Table 3, no remarkable gender differences were generally observed for the construct considered in both measures. The most important difference referred to “Peer support” (t = 2.800, sig. = 0.005). All the results are displayed in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Student’s t-test for gender differences.

Pearson’s Bivariate Correlations

Finally, an additional analysis was carried out. A Pearson’s bivariate correlational analysis was performed as an approach to explore the relations between the variables from Stage 1 (demands and supports) and those from Stage 2 (expectancy beliefs and goals adopted by students). The scales from Stage 1 were administered after the first month of class, whereas the scales from Stage 2 were administered 1 month later. Regarding teacher support, positive and significant correlations were generally obtained among teacher support and expectancy of success, process expectancy (the highest values found) and the mastery goals adopted by students, Conversely, negative and significant correlations were obtained between teacher support and avoidance goals. Regarding learning demands, on the one hand, positive and significant correlations were obtained among desirability and expectancy of success, process expectancy (emotional reactions), where the highest value was found (r = 0.717, p > 0.01), and mastery goals. On the other hand, negative and significant correlations were observed among feasibility and expectancy of success, process expectancy (emotional reactions) and mastery goals. See Table 5 for more details.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlation between demands-supports and anticipatory cognitive motivators.

Discussion

The primary purpose of the present study was to examine the psychometric properties of the “Learning Demands Scale” (MOCSE-LDS) and “Teacher Support Questionnaire” (MOCSE-TSQ) to determine their factorial validity and internal consistency.

The Maximum Likelihood (ML) Robust method of estimation, developed by Satorra and Bentler (1988, 1994), appears to be a good approach when the multivariate normality assumption is violated and/or the sample size is small (Hu et al., 1992; Curran et al., 1996). This study suffers from both problems, which is frequently the case in Social Sciences.

Studies centered on examining the role of learning demands and teacher support in the university context are scarce, but important for improving both teacher practice and students’ motivation, issues traditionally ignored in Higher Education. The results of a meta-analysis conducted by Lei et al. (2018) revealed that teacher support correlated significantly with students’ academic emotions in both no university and the university classroom context.

The EFA conducted of the Learning Demands Scale (MOCSE-LDS) proved the existence of a two-factor structure referring to the constructs of desirability and feasibility as regards demands. According to the literature based on the Theory of Action Control (Heckhausen and Kuhl, 1985), both constructs are considered crucial in the decision to set a goal intention (Fishbein and Ajzen, 2010). The EFA performed of the Teacher Support Questionnaire (MOCSE-TSQ) revealed the existence of 10 factors or subscales with good psychometric validity and reliability properties.

The CFA conducted on both instruments, using the EQS program, suggested that some minor changes should be made to the initial structure following the recommendations of the Wald and Lagrange test for introducing into or removing parameters. Consequently, the factor structure of both instruments was optimized and fitted the data, which means that the resulting theoretical structure proposed for both the MOCSE-LDS and MOCSE-TSQ was adequate. Moreover, the scales from both instruments showed good internal consistencies, and they all meet the standard of 0.70 recommended by Nunnally and Bernstein (1994). In short, the results confirmed the validity and reliability of both instruments. The data indicated a bidimensional structure of the MOCSE-LDS instrument made up of two factors/subscales named desirability and feasibility, and a multidimensional structure of the MOCSE-TSQ instrument made up of 10 factors/subscales (Content comprehension support, Teacher accessibility and closeness, Autonomy support, Peer support, Awaken interest in the subject, Acknowledging the student’s effort, Guiding students in their learning, Providing didactic resources, Providing feed-back). Most of the research in the existing literature on this topic have focused on the teaching supports related to the three basic psychological needs (autonomy, competence, relatedness) proposed in the Self-Determination Theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2002). In the present questionnaire, we extended this traditional approach by taking into account additional teacher supports related to instructional and affective-social student needs that previous research has demonstrated as being important for student motivation in Secondary Education (Ahmed et al., 2010; Wentzel et al., 2010; Federici and Skaalvik, 2014a; Federici et al., 2016; Han et al., 2019). We think that such additional supports can also be interesting if they are taken into account in the university context.

To check for gender differences, Student’s t-tests for independent samples were performed. Overall, no remarkable gender differences were observed for the construct/dimensions considered in both measures; that is, demands and supports. The most important significant difference referred to the factor “Peer support,” which indicated that males’ perception was much better than females’ perception of this construct. Consequently, it should be taken into account when this construct is measured in the university context.

Finally, regarding the correlational analysis, two things should be highlighted. First, values generally go in the expected direction (e.g., teaching supports showed a positive relation with mastery goals and a negative relation with avoidance goals). This seems to indicate that the two tested instruments (MOCSE-LDS and MOCSE-TSQ) properly measure the constructs considered in the present study. Second, the correlation analysis reveals important associations between the variables from Stage 1 (learning demands and teacher support) and those from Stage 2 (students’ expectancy and goals). Accordingly, an in-depth analysis, following the MOCSE postulates, is suggested in the future at different levels of education and with distinct curricular contents.

In conclusion, the factor validity of both instruments was examined with EFA and CFA based on the MOCSE postulates. The results confirmed the validity and reliability of both instruments which teachers can use to evaluate how their students perceive demands and supports. Likewise, we wish to highlight the importance of having valid assessment instruments like the “Learning Demands Scale” (MOCSE-LDS) and the “Teacher Support Questionnaire” (MOCSE-TSQ) for both applied and research purposes.

Practical Implications

Both instruments can be useful for researcher and teachers. On the one hand, this study provides researchers with two valid and reliable tools that can contribute to investigate students’ motivation; that is, why a student decides to strive to achieve academic demands/goals or not, and to investigate other related constructs, such as students’ anticipatory cognitive motivators and achievement goals. They were all in accordance with the MOCSE postulates. On the other hand, the two tested instruments can provide teachers and school psychologist with important information to implement preventive or intervention actions that improve students’ intention to learn. Teachers can also use them to evaluate their own teaching and to research their own classrooms. Briefly, the results obtained for the validity and reliability of the two tested instruments are good. Therefore, the application of the instruments MOCSE-LDS and MOCSE-TSQ was satisfactorily supported by the empirical data.

Limitations and Future Directions

In the present study, the participants were students from Spanish university classes. Although the results obtained in this study are satisfactory, some limitations and suggestions for future research are pointed out. Perhaps the number of the participants in this study is somewhat limited for validating the second instrument (MOCSE-TSQ) composed of 72 items. Further research is needed to investigate whether the validity of the two instruments presented herein is stable for a larger sample, and for other levels of education, and socio-cultural contexts. A computer-based version of both forms is highly recommended to increase accessibility as students can fill in forms outside class. In future studies, more in-depth analyses, like multiple-group structural analyses, could be considered to test the invariance for gender and grade levels. MOCSE-TSQ and MOCSE-LDS will provide important information to understand students’ perceptions of not only the required learning demands in terms of desirability and feasibility, but also the supports they are expected to be provided with by the teacher during the course for a specific subject matter.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FD-B wrote the first draft of the manuscript and conducted the statistical analyses. AG-A and LA-R reviewed the whole manuscript, checked the references, and made significant contributions. ER-B reviewed the manuscript and contributed to the discussion. All the authors collected the data, reread the manuscript and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abellán-Roselló, L. (2016). Motivación y Aprendizaje Escolar en la Educación Secundaria Obligatoria: Un Estudio Empírico Desde el Modelo de Calidad de Situación Educativa [Scholar Learning and Motivation: Un Empirical Study from the Quality Educational Setting Model]. Ph. D. Dissertation, University of Valencia, Valencia.

Google Scholar

Ahmed, W., Minnaert, A., Van der Werf, G., and Kuyper, H. (2010). Perceived social support and early adolescents’ achievement: the mediational roles of motivational beliefs and emotions. J. Youth Adolesc. 39, 36–46. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9367-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: goals, structures, and student motivation. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 261–271. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.3.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychol. Rev. 64(Pt 1), 359–372. doi: 10.1037/h0043445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakker, A. B., and Demerouti, E. (2007). The job demands resources model: state of the art. J. Manag. Psychol. 22, 309–328. doi: 10.1108/02683940710733115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakker, A. B., and Demerouti, E. (2008). Towards a model of work engagement. Career Dev. Int. 13, 209–223. doi: 10.1108/13620430810870476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1986). Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Fit indexes, langrage multipliers, constraint changes and incomplete data in structural models. Multivariate Behav. Res. 25, 163–172. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr2502_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentler, P. M. (2006). EQS 6 Structural Equations Program Manual. Ecino, CA: Multivarite Sotware Inc.

Google Scholar

Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 946–960. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.946

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1993). “Alternative ways of assessing model fit,” in Testing Structural Equation Models, eds K. A. Bollen and J. S. Long (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage), 136–162.

Google Scholar

Chiu, M. M., and Chow, B. W.-Y. (2011). Classroomdiscipline across 41 countries: school, economic, and cultural differences. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 42, 516–533. doi: 10.1177/0022022110381115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curran, P. J., West, S. G., and Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychol. Methods 1, 16–29. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.1.1.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuit: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inq. 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demerouti, E., Bakker, A. B., Nachreiner, F., and Schaufeli, W. B. (2001). The job demands resources model of burnout. J. Appl. Psychol. 86, 499–512. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dietrich, J., Dicke, A.-L., Kracke, B., and Noack, P. (2015). Teacher support and its influence on students’ intrinsic value and effort: dimensional comparison effects across subjects. Learn. Instr. 39, 45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2015.05.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doménech-Betoret, F. (2012). Psicología Educativa: Su Aplicación al Contexto de la Clase [Educational Psychology: its Application in the Classroom Context], Vol. 13. Castellón: Publicaciones de la Universitat Jaume I. Col⋅lecció Psique.

Google Scholar

Doménech-Betoret, F. (2006). Testing an instructional model in a university educational setting from the student’s perspective. Learn. Instr. 16, 450–466. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doménech-Betoret, F. (2013). An instructional model for guiding reflection and research in the classroom: the educational situation quality model. Electr. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 11, 239–260.

Google Scholar

Doménech-Betoret, F. (2018). The educational situation quality model: recent advances. Front. Psychol. 9:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00328

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doménech-Betoret, F., Gómez-Artiga, A., and Abellán-Roselló, L. (2019). The educational situation quality model: a new tool to explain and improve academic achievement and course satisfaction. Front. Psychol. 10:1692. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Doménech-Betoret, F., Gómez-Artiga, A., and Lloret-Segura, S. (2014). Personal variables, motivation and avoidance learning strategies in undergraduate students. Learn. Indiv. Differ. 35, 122–129. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2014.06.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S., and Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychol. Rev. 95, 256–273. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.95.2.256

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. (2009). Who am I and what am I going to do with my life? Personal and collective identities as motivators of action. Educ. Psychol. 44, 78–89. doi: 10.1080/00461520902832368

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eccles, J. S., and Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 53, 109–132. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elliot, A. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educ. Psychol. 34, 169–189. doi: 10.1207/s15326985ep3403_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Federici, R. A., and Skaalvik, E. M. (2014a). Students’ perceptions of emotional and instrumental teacher support: relations with motivational and emotional responses. Int. Educ. Stud. 7, 21–36.

Google Scholar

Federici, R. A., Caspersen, J., and Wendelborg, C. H. (2016). Students’ perceptions of teacher support, numeracy, and assessment for learning: relations with motivational responses and mastery experiences. Int. Educ. Stud. 9:1. doi: 10.5539/ies.v9n10p1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Federici, R. A., and Skaalvik, E. M. (2014b). Students’ perception of instrumental support and effort in mathematics: the mediating role of subjective task values. Soc. Psychol. Educ. 17, 527–540. doi: 10.1007/s11218-014-9264-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Google Scholar

Fishbein, M., and Ajzen, I. (2010). Predicting and Changing Behavior: The Reasoned Action Approach. New York, NY: Psychology Press.

Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). “Action phases and mind-sets,” in Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior, Vol. 2, eds E. T. Higgins and R. M. Sorrentino (New York, NY: The Guilford Press), 53–92.

Google Scholar

Gollwitzer, P. M., and Bayer, U. (1999). “Deliberative versus implemental mindsets in the control of action,” in Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology, eds S. Chaiken and Y. Trope (New York, NY: Guilford Press), 403–422.

Google Scholar

Hakanen, J. J., Bakker, A. B., and Schaufeli, W. B. (2006). Burnout and work engagement among teachers. J. Sch. Psychol. 43, 495–513. doi: 10.1016/j.jsp.2005.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Han, H., Bong, M., Kim, S.-I., and Kwon, S. K. (2019). Utility value and emotional support of teachers as predictors of student utility value and achievement. Educ. Psychol. doi: 10.1080/01443410.2019.1693509 [Epub ahead of print],

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heckhausen, H., and Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional states of mind. Motiv. Emot. 11, 101–120. doi: 10.1007/bf00992338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heckhausen, H., and Kuhl, J. (1985). “From wishes to action: the dead ends and shortcuts on the long way to action,” in Goal Directed Behavior: Psychological Theory and Research on Action, eds M. Frese and J. Sarini (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), 134–159.

Google Scholar

Heckhausen, H., Schmalt, H., and Schneider, K. (1985). Achievement Motivation in Perspective. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press.

Google Scholar

Hoyle, R. H. (1995). Structural Equation Modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: coventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., Bentler, P. M., and Kano, Y. (1992). Can test statistics in covariance structure analysis be trusted? Psychol. Bull. 112, 351–362. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

IBM Corp (2018). IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22,0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp.

Google Scholar

Katz, I., Kaplan, A., and Gueta, G. (2009). Students’ needs, teachers’ support, and motivation for doing homework: a cross-sectional study. J. Exp. Educ. 78, 246–267. doi: 10.1080/00220970903292868

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, R. B., and McInerney, D. M. (2014). The work avoidance goal construct: Examining its structure, antecedents, and consequences. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 39, 42–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhl, J., and Beckmann, J. (1985). Action Control: From Cognition to Behavior. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Lei, H., Cui, Y., and Chiu, M. M. (2018). The relationship between teacher support and students’ academic emotions: a meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:2288. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liem, A. D., Lau, S., and Nie, Y. (2008). The role of self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals in predicting learning strategies, task disengagement, peer relationship, and achievement outcome. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 33, 486–512. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2007.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin-Siegler, X., Dweck, C. S., and Cohen, G. L. (2016). Instructional interventions that motivate classroom learning. J. Educ. Psychol. 108, 295–299. doi: 10.1037/edu0000124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholls, J. (1989). The Competitive Ethos and Democratic Education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Nunnally, J. C., and Bernstein, I. H. (1994). The assessment of reliability. Psychom. Theory 3, 248–292.

Google Scholar

Patrick, H., Ryan, A. M., and Kaplan, A. (2007). adolescents’ perceptions of the classroom social environment, motivational beliefs, and engagement. J. Educ. Psychol. 99, 83–98.

Google Scholar

Pintrich, P. R., and de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning components of classroom academic performance. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 33–40.

Google Scholar

Roorda, D. L., Koomen, H. M. Y., Spilt, J. L., and Oort, F. J. (2011). The influence of affective teacher-student relationships on students’ school engagement and achievement: a meta-analytic approach. Rev. Educ. Res. 81, 493–529. doi: 10.3102/0034654311421793

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2002). “An overview of self-determination theory: an organismic-dialectical perspective,” in Handbook of Self-Determination Research, eds E. L. Deci and R. M. Ryan (Rochester, NY: The University of Rochester Press), 3–33.

Google Scholar

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (1988). “Scaling corrections for chi-square statistics in covariance structure analysis,” in ASA 1988 Proceedings of the Business and Economic Statistics, Section, (Alexandria, VA: American Statistical Association), 308–313.

Google Scholar

Satorra, A., and Bentler, P. M. (1994). “Corrections to test statistics and standard errors in covariance structure analysis,” in Latent variables analysis: Applications for Developmental Research, eds A. von Eye and C. C. Clogg (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 399–419.

Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., and Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources and their relationship with burnout and engagement: a multi-sample study. J. Organ. Behav. 25, 293–315. doi: 10.1002/job.248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, J. P. (2002). Applied Multivariate Statistics for the Social Sciences, 4th Edn. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Google Scholar

Tardy, C. H. (1985). Social support measurement. Am. J. Community Psychol. 13, 187–202. doi: 10.1007/BF00905728

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wentzel, K. R. (2009). “Students’ relationships with teachers as motivational contexts,” in Educational Psychology Handbook Series. Handbook of Motivation at School, eds K. R. Wenzel and A. Wigfield (Abingdon: Routledge), 301–322.

Google Scholar

Wentzel, K. R., Battle, A., Russell, S., and Looney, L. (2010). Social supports from teachers and peers as predictors of academic and social motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 35, 193–202. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigfield, A., and Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 25, 68–81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: educational model, teacher support, learning demands, student motivation, questionnaire validation

Citation: Doménech-Betoret F, Gómez-Artiga A, Abellán-Roselló L and Rocabert-Beút E (2020) MOCSE Centered on Students: Validation of Learning Demands and Teacher Support Scales. Front. Psychol. 11:582926. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.582926

Received: 13 July 2020; Accepted: 08 September 2020;
Published: 02 October 2020.

Edited by:

Raquel Gilar-Corbi, University of Alicante, Spain

Reviewed by:

Maria Eugenia Martin Palacio, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain
Maria Clelia Zurlo, University of Naples Federico II, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Doménech-Betoret, Gómez-Artiga, Abellán-Roselló and Rocabert-Beút. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Fernando Doménech-Betoret, betoret@uji.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.