Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 08 August 2019
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Processes in Individual and Collective Creativity: A Cross-Cultural Perspective View all 22 articles

Acute Stress Shapes Creative Cognition in Trait Anxiety

Haijun Duan
Haijun Duan1*Xuewei WangXuewei Wang1Zijuan Wang,Zijuan Wang1,2Wenlong XueWenlong Xue1Yuecui KanYuecui Kan1Weiping Hu,
Weiping Hu1,3*Fengqing ZhangFengqing Zhang4
  • 1MOE Key Laboratory of Modern Teaching Technology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi’an, China
  • 2Jinyuan International School, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi’an, China
  • 3Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment Towards Basic Education Quality, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 4Department of Psychology, Drexel University, Philadelphia, PA, United States

This study examined the cognitive mechanism underlying acute stress in creative cognition among individuals with high and low trait anxiety. Specifically, cognitive inhibition was assessed using the flanker task during acute stress. Fifty-two participants (26 with high trait anxiety, 26 with low trait anxiety, with a mean age of 18.94 years) underwent stress induction via the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). They all completed the Alternative Uses Test (AUT) and the Remote Associates Test (RAT) before and after the TSST. Biochemical markers (salivary cortisol and salivary alpha amylase) were recorded at regular intervals. The results showed that cognitive inhibition was influenced by trait anxiety and acute stress. In low-trait anxious individuals after experiencing acute stress, there was a lack of cognitive inhibition and they performed better in AUT (fluency), compared to before experiencing acute stress, whereas high-trait anxious individuals showed a decreased interference effect and reduced performance in AUT (fluency, flexibility, and originality). In the RAT, there were shorter response times and increased accuracy after acute stress in both high- and low-trait anxiety groups. Thus, we suggest that cognitive control, which modulates changes in acute stress, influences creative cognition. These findings provide evidence that inhibition control mediates the effect of stress on the creativity of individuals with different trait anxiety.

Introduction

Creativity has long been of great interest in a wide range of fields. People have the ability to exert cognitive control over creativity (Beaty et al., 2014; Kenett et al., 2018). According to controlled-attention theory, creative production depends on individuals’ ability to exert control over their attention and cognition (Gilhooly et al., 2007; Beaty et al., 2016). Individuals high in creative thinking tend to be able to dynamically change their level of control according to the current task requirements (Gilhooly et al., 2007; Radel et al., 2010; Benedek et al., 2012). However, our ability to control creativity is not always consistent, especially in the face of suddenly occurring situations.

Stress is an unavoidable feature of modern life. Stress activates the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) and hypothalamus-pituitary adrenal (HPA) axis. In human saliva, the activity of the SNS and HPA can be measured by salivary alpha amylase (sAA) and salivary cortisol (sC), respectively (Kirschbaum and Hellhammer, 1989; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010). It is vital that individuals have the ability to exert cognitive control in the face of stress—not doing so may cause stress to impair functioning (Erskine et al., 2007), causing serious distress and mental impairment (McNally, 2006; Qureshi et al., 2011; Cisler and Olatunji, 2012).

Previous literature has presented inconsistent results regarding the association between stress and creativity. Some have found that stress leads to a decrease in creativity (Beversdorf et al., 1999; Probst et al., 2007; Byron et al., 2010; Lovelace and Hunter, 2013; Duan et al., 2019; Wang et al., 2019), while others found that it increased creativity (Baas et al., 2008; Ohly and Fritz, 2010). Still others found a U-shaped relationship between stress and creativity (Suedfeld and Vernon, 1965; Baer and Oldham, 2006; Yeh et al., 2015). Meanwhile, trait anxiety was usually considered as a stress-vulnerability factor (Eysenck and Derakshan, 2011; Berggren and Derakshan, 2013; Ward et al., 2017; Weger and Sandi, 2018).

Trait anxiety has been defined as the tendency of individuals to experience frequent and high-intensity anxiety and worry in the face of stressful situations (Spielberger, 1979). Hence, studies could focus on highly anxious individuals, who are more prone to anxiety in stressful situations. Additionally, these past studies did not explore the cognitive mechanisms underlying how stress affects creativity. As mentioned above, completing creative activities and facing stress both require cognitive control. Thus, the core mechanism underlying creative generation under stress may be an executive control process, including the ability to inhibit the influence of irrelevant information caused by stress. However, this fascinating possibility remains to be addressed.

According to the Attentional Control Theory (ACT; Eysenck et al., 2007), anxiety activates the stimulus-driven system and reduces the goal-driven system (the crux of the ACT is that we have two attentional systems: top-down, goal-driven processing and bottom-up, stimulus-driven processing). Individuals with high trait anxiety (HTA) appear to be more affected by stimulus-driven processing and find it difficult to suppress threatening stimuli from entering attention. Indeed, in the face of threat stimuli, individuals with HTA show more pronounced cognitive deficits, including weakened dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) activity (Clarke et al., 2014; Greening and Mitchell, 2015). Hence, individuals with HTA appear to show both behavioral alterations and cognitive deficits. Further research has revealed that anxiety affects the processing efficiency of executive functioning (shifting, updating and inhibition) when faced with threatening information (e.g., stress; Navarro et al., 2012; Edwards et al., 2015; Goette et al., 2015; Fogelman et al., 2016; Fonzo and Etkin, 2017). Thus, anxiety is related to a diminished ability to inhibit threat (Cisler and Koster, 2010).

A group of researchers suggested that acute stress may affect core executive functions (Hermans et al., 2014; Shields et al., 2016; Gu et al., 2018), particularly inhibition control (Shields et al., 2016). However, there are scant empirical studies examining the effect of acute stress on inhibition control in individuals with HTA and low trait anxiety (LTA). Some indirect evidence from studies on cortisol and individual differences in anxiety implied that anxiety-related personality traits modulate cognitive control processes under stress (Edwards et al., 2017). On the one hand, Grillon et al. (2017) asked participants to perform inhibition tasks while under threat of an electric shock, and found that increased anxiety promoted inhibition control. Individuals needed to allocate only a small amount of attentional resources to fully process the task-related information (Chajut and Algom, 2003). They explained the results using attention approach theory, positing that HTA individuals may have more limited attentional resources than LTA individuals, and thus their available attentional resources may become exhausted quickly under stressful conditions. On the other hand, according to the ACT (Eysenck et al., 2007), HTA individuals could display worse inhibition control under stress (Cisler and Koster, 2010; Navarro et al., 2012; Edwards et al., 2015; Goette et al., 2015; Fogelman et al., 2016; Fonzo and Etkin, 2017).

The relationship between cognitive inhibition and creativity has been explored in relation to creativity. Researchers hold that there are two types of creative thinking: convergent and divergent thinking. Convergent thinking involves deriving a single correct solution, while divergent thinking involves thinking of as many potential solutions as possible. For convergent thinking, high inhibitory control is necessary to prevent irrelevant ideas from entering into working memory and helping individuals focus on identifying solutions that meet the required standards (Zhou et al., 2019). However, for divergent thinking, low inhibition (involving automatic association and a lack of filtering of seemingly irrelevant information) may actually facilitate generation of creative ideas (Chrysikou et al., 2014; Barr et al., 2015; Radel et al., 2015; Beaty et al., 2016; Hao et al., 2016; Kenett et al., 2018).

The present study drew on previous research topics involving the complex interaction between cognitive control, stress, and creativity, with a particular focus on individual differences in trait anxiety. To date, no study has considered all four of these variables together. The present study examined the relationships among trait anxiety, acute stress, and inhibitory control using a version of the flanker task. Trait anxiety was operationalized using the Chinese version of the trait anxiety portion of the State–Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; Spielberger et al., 1983; Shek, 1993). Acute stress was induced using the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST) (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Kudielka et al., 2007). This procedure allowed us to investigate whether trait anxiety under stress affects individuals’ ability to exert inhibitory control to influence creative performance outcomes. To summarize, we explore the difference in creativity between HTA and LTA individuals who underwent a stressful situation, and determined if inhibitory control mediated the effect of acute stress on creativity. To this end, two hypotheses were formulated: (1) if acute stress impairs inhibitory control processes, HTA individuals will present better divergent thinking performance and worse convergent thinking performance; (2) if acute stress does not impair inhibitory control processes, HTA individuals will present worse divergent thinking performance and better convergent thinking performance.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Initially, 713 undergraduate students from Shaanxi Normal University (pre-test) completed the Chinese version of the STAI. Based on their scores, we chose individuals for the HTA group (upper 27th percentile of the distribution) and LTA group (lower 27th percentile). In the present study, we invited 52 individuals, including 26 HTA individuals (Mage = 18.46 years, SD = 0.89) and 26 LTA individuals (Mage = 19.42 years, SD = 1.31), to participate. An independent-samples t-test revealed that the HTA group had higher trait anxiety scores (M = 55.42, SD = 5.85) than did the LTA group (M = 35.58, SD = 8.04) at pre-test, t(50) = −9.976, p < 0.001. We applied the following criteria when selecting these participants. We excluded those with a body mass index (BMI) below 18 or exceeding 27 kg/m2; those engaged in drug use; those who regularly consumed coffee or alcohol; and those with chronic or acute illnesses. Furthermore, participants were advised to refrain from physical exercise and consumption of food and drinks, except water, 3 h before the test sessions began (Kuhlmann et al., 2005; Kudielka et al., 2009). Female participants were not menstruating. The experiment was conducted from 2:00 pm to 5:00 pm owing to the circadian rhythms (Izawa et al., 2010). The study conformed to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (World Medical Association, 2013) and was approved by the Academic Committee of the Ministry of Education of Key Laboratory of Modern Teaching Technology, Shaanxi Normal University in China. All participants provided written informed consent after the procedures were fully explained, and were paid for their participation in the study.

Experiment Procedure

To control for individual differences, this study used a mixed design, with time of measurement (pre-test, post-test) as a within-subjects factor (McHugh et al., 2010) and group (HTA, LTA) as the between-subjects factor. The dependent variable of this experiment was performance on two creative thinking tasks: the Remote Association Test (RAT) and the Alternative Uses Test (AUT). The indicators selected for the RAT were response time and accuracy, and those for the AUT were fluency, flexibility, and originality. The overall procedure was as follows: the first salivary sample (S1) was collected on participants’ arrival at the laboratory. Subsequently, they completed a questionnaire on their demographic information, followed by the STAI. The subjects were allowed to relax for 15 min before the second salivary sample (S2). Participants in the HTA and LTA groups then completed the pre-test tasks (flanker and creative tasks) for 15 min, and the TSST for 10 min. After the TSST, the third salivary sample (S3) was taken. Then, all participants performed the post-test tasks (flanker and creative tasks). The order of the creative tasks was counterbalanced across participants. The AUT and RAT were administered via a computer using the E-Prime 2.0 software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, Pennsylvania, USA) (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the procedure. Saliva from the participants was collected at five time points (−15, 0, 15, 25, and 40 min in relation to the onset of the stressful task). After a rest phase, participants performed Flanker task and creative tasks (AUT and RAT) before and after the TSST.

Stress Task

According to the procedure of the TSST (Kirschbaum et al., 1993; Kudielka et al., 2007), participants were asked to create a 5-min interview speech for applying to college, which they would deliver to a panel of college counselors. They were given 3 min for preparation. The panel consisted of two experimenters. If a participant’s speech did not reach the full 5 min, they had to answer questions given by the experimenters until the full 5 min had passed. Finally, participants were asked to orally report answers to arithmetic problems (they had to subtract increments of 17 from 2023) as quickly and as accurately as possible for 5 min. When they made an error, the experimenter interrupted and instructed the participant to start over at 2023. The entire stress task was recorded with a digital video camera. Experimenters maintained a cold and reserved manner throughout.

Flanker Task

The flanker task measures individuals’ ability to selectively attend to a target and ignore distractors (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974). According to Friedman and Miyake (2004), inhibitory control has at least two components: inhibition of the dominant response and prevention of distracting interference. Compared to other inhibition tasks, the flanker task is considered to best reflect an individual’s ability to engage in inhibition control (Redick and Engle, 2006; Shields et al., 2016). In the flanker task, a central arrow (1.48° × 0.82°) was flanked by two distractor arrows, which were kept at a distance of 0.16°. The distractor arrows were pointed either in the same (i.e., congruent trial) or opposite directions (i.e., incongruent trial) as the central target arrow. A fixation cross was displayed for 1,200 ms. After presenting a black screen for 500–1,000 ms, the arrow flanker task was presented for 1,500 ms or until a response was obtained. After presenting a black screen for 1,000 ms, the next flanker task began. Overall, participants completed 100 flanker trials. The flanker-interference effect (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974) was defined as the difference in reaction times under the incompatible and compatible conditions (a greater difference indicates more interference).

Creative Task

The AUT was selected to measure divergent thinking. Participants were given 2 min per object to verbalize as many uses as they could. Two lists of objects were used for each experimental session (pre-test: bucket, shoe, newspaper; post-test: umbrella, can, paperclip) (Radel et al., 2015). The order of the lists was randomized. According to Guilford (1950), the test is scored in terms of fluency, flexibility, and originality. The fluency score was calculated as the number of responses; the flexibility score as the number of categories of responses; and originality as the frequency of occurrence of a given response among the participants. A response frequency percentage of less than 1% was given a score of 2; a frequency between 1 and 5% was given a score of 1; and a frequency of more than 5% was given a score of 0 (Radel et al., 2015). Two experienced creative field coders rated participants’ responses. They had satisfactory inter-rater reliability (Cronbach’s alphas: 1 for fluency, 1 for originality, 0.872 for flexibility).

For convergent thinking, we used the updated Chinese Compound RAT, compiled by Xu et al. (2015) (cf. Bowden and Jung-Beeman, 2003). Each problem in this test consists of three words chosen from the Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary (1986). High-frequency words (mean frequency: 4,981.6 per million) were used to ensure that participants would understand them. Based on a preliminary test, 135 items were selected. From these, we selected 40 items for which the solution rates ranged from 40 to 65%. Twenty items were used for the pre-test and 20 for the post-test. This selection of Chinese Compound RAT problems had satisfactory internal consistency (Cronbach’s α = 0.897) and criterion validity. The solution rate was 66%, and the average response time was 3.79 s. The experiment was compiled using the E-prime program. Five items were used in a practice experiment. At the beginning of each trial, a 500-ms fixation is displayed at the center of the screen. Participants must press the space bar and immediately enter the RAT item in the next screen; then, they must think of an answer and say it aloud immediately after pressing the space bar. The screen automatically records the answer spoken by the participant and presents the correct answer. Finally, participants are asked to judge the answer displayed on the screen. If the correct answer is consistent with the answer spoken, they should press Q; if not, they should press P. If after 15 s, the participant is still not able to determine the answer, the program skips to the next question. A random interval of 100–500 ms is presented between two items.

Physiological Measures

The participants deposited salivary samples at −15, 0, 15 min (T2), 25 min (T3), and 40 min (T4) after the stress task. To control for stress induced by lab environment in subjects, the lower of the two samples between the first sample and the second sample was chosen as the baseline (T1) (McHugh et al., 2010). Saliva was collected using Salivettes® (Sarstedt 51.1534.500, Germany). All saliva samples were stored at −22°C and then thawed and centrifuged at 3000 rpm. The cortisol concentration and salivary alpha amylase were determined by enzyme immunoassay (Jianglai, China).

Results

The physiological data, flanker task, and creativity scores were analyzed using univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group (HTA, LTA) as the between-subjects factor and time (measurement time points) as within-subject factor. The ANOVA tested for the main effects of trait anxiety and presence/absence of stressor and their interaction. The Greenhouse-Geisser correction for non-sphericity was performed wherever appropriate. Bonferroni corrections were used to control for multiple comparisons. Partial-eta2 ( η p 2 ) is reported as a measure of effect size. Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis were conducted using SPSS.

Physiological Results

For salivary cortisol, a repeated measurement ANOVA with the within-subject factor of time (T1, T2, T3, and T4) and between-subject factor of group (HTA and LTA) was computed with the salivary cortisol data to examine the effect of stress on salivary cortisol with high- and low-trait anxiety individual. ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of time, F(3,150) = 123.55, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.712, and a significant main effect of group, F(1,50) = 7.75, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.134. The HTA group (M = 7.45, SD = 2.23) was lower than the LTA (M = 8.36, SD = 1.77) group in salivary cortisol. The results also revealed a time × group interaction, F(3,48) = 6.88, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.301. Bonferroni-corrected simple-effects tests at each time point revealed that the two groups showed significantly lower cortisol at T1 than at each of the subsequent times, (p < 0.001). Otherwise, compared with group difference in every time, the HTA group was significantly lower than the LTA group at T1 (p < 0.001) and T2 (p < 0.05) (see Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean salivary cortisol (nmol/L) as a function of time (minutes following intervention onset) for the stress. Error bars represent standard errors of the means.

For salivary alpha amylase, a repeated measurement ANOVA with the within-subject factor of time (T1, T2, T3, and T4) and the between-subject factor of group (HTA and LTA) was computed for the salivary alpha amylase data to examine the effect of stress on salivary alpha amylase with high- and low-trait anxiety individuals. ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of time, F(3,150) = 114.16, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.695, and a significant main effect of group, F(1,50) = 63.50, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.599. The HTA group (M = 133.34, SD = 42.77) was lower than the LTA group (M = 164.27, SD = 26.61) in salivary alpha amylase. The results also revealed a time × group interaction, F(3,150) = 22.568, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.311. Bonferroni-corrected simple-effects tests at each time point revealed that the two groups showed significantly lower cortisol at T1 than at each of the subsequent times (p < 0.001). Furthermore, compared with group difference in every time, the HTA group was significantly lower than the LTA group at T1, T2, and T3 (p < 0.001) (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean salivary alpha amylase (U/ml) as a function of time (minutes following intervention onset) for the stress. Error bars represent standard errors of the means.

Flanker Interference Effect Results

Descriptions of mean response times and error rates in the flanker task in the pre-test and post-test for the LTA and HTA groups are shown in Table 1. When analyzing the response times and error rate, extreme values of three standard deviations were excluded. Repeated measures ANOVAs with the within-subject factor measurement (pre-test and post-test) and the between-subject factor of group (HTA and LTA) were computed for the interference effect on response time and error rate. For interference effect in RTs, ANOVA yielded a significant interaction effect of group and measurement, F(1,50) = 20.38, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.290. However, the main effect on group and measurement was insignificant (p > 0.05). Bonferroni-corrected simple-effects tests revealed that the HTA group (M = 81.13, SD = 40.21) was significantly slower than the LTA group (M = 54.19, SD = 32.25) in pre-test, p = 0.010. Nevertheless, the HTA group (M = 53.41, SD = 32.24) was significantly faster than the LTA group (M = 88.49, SD = 43.35) in post-test, p = 0.002. The HTA group pre-test (M = 81.13, SD = 40.21) was significantly slower than post-test (M = 53.41, SD = 32.24), p = 0.006, while the LTA group’s pre-test (M = 54.19, SD = 32.25) was significantly faster than the post-test (M = 88.49, SD = 43.35), p = 0.001 (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean and standard deviations of response time (ms) and error rate (%) for flanker tasks pre and post stress for the LTA and HTA.

For the interference effect in error rate, ANOVA only yielded a significant main effect of measurement (pre-test and post-test); F(1,50) = 5.65, p = 0.021, η p 2 = 0.102. Post-test (M = 0.020, SD = 0.013) showed significantly lower score than the pre-test (M = 0.079, SD = 0.036), while the main effect on group and the interaction effect between group and measurement were insignificant (p > 0.05) (see Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Interference effects (i.e., incongruent-congruent) on response time (a) pre- and post-TSST, for the HTA and LTA groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. ** p < 0.01.

Creativity Results

Repeated measures ANOVAs for the within-subject factor measurement (pre-test and post-test) and between-subject factor of group (HTA and LTA) was computed for the AUT (fluency, flexibility, and originality) and RAT (response time and accuracy) to examine the effect of stress on creativity with high- and low-trait anxiety individuals.

For fluency of AUT, the results only revealed a significant time × group interaction, F(1,50) = 16.29, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.245. Bonferroni-corrected simple-effects tests revealed that the HTA group (M = 20.69, SD = 8.13) showed significantly more fluency than the LTA group (M = 13.69, SD = 4.86) only in post-test, p < 0.001. Interestingly, compared with group difference in pre- and post-test, the HTA group’s (M = 13.69, SD = 4.86) pre-test result was significantly lower than that of the post-test (M = 16.89, SD = 5.78), p = 0.005, whereas the LTA group scored significantly higher in pre-test (M = 20.692, SD = 8.13) than post-test (M = 17.65, SD = 6.56), p = 0.008. For AUT flexibility, the results revealed a significant time × group interaction, F(1,50) = 17.70, p < 0.001, η p 2 = 0.261. Bonferroni-corrected simple-effects tests revealed that the HTA group (M = 9.50, SD = 3.05) showed significantly lower flexibility than the LTA group (M = 13.88, SD = 4.43) in the post-test, p < 0.001. Compared with group difference in pre- and post-test, HTA group’s pre-test score (M = 11.65, SD = 3.87) was significantly larger than that of their post-test (M = 9.50, SD = 3.05), p = 0.006, whereas the LTA group scored (M = 11.62, SD = 5.12) significantly lower in pre-test than post-test (M = 13.89, SD = 4.43), p = 0.004. For AUT originality, the results also revealed a significant time × group interaction, F(1,50) = 6.36, p = 0.015, η p 2 = 0.113. Bonferroni-corrected simple-effects tests revealed that the HTA group (M = 10.88, SD = 5.55) showed significantly less originality than the LTA group (M = 18.46, SD = 10.39) in post-test, p = 0.002. Compared with group difference in pre- and post-test, HTA group’s score in pre-test (M = 14.04, SD = 7.61) was significantly higher than in post-test (M = 10.88, SD = 5.55), p = 0.025 (see Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Mean and standard deviation for Alternative Uses Test scores for the HTA and LTA groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.

For the accuracy of the RAT, there was a significant main effect of time (pre-test and post-test), F(1,50) = 7.02, p = 0.011, η p 2 = 0.123. The pre-test accuracy score (M = 51.54, SD = 14.30) was significantly lower than the post-test accuracy score (M = 57.60, SD = 15.03). The results showed a significant group effect, F(1,50) = 11.11, p = 0.002, η p 2 = 0.182. The HTA group scored (M = 49.42, SD = 14.27) significantly higher than the LTA group (M = 59.71, SD = 13.84). There was no interaction effect between measurement and group. For the RAT response time, the results showed a main effect of time (pretest and posttest), F(1,50) = 5.00, p = 0.030, η p 2 = 0.091. The pre-test score (M = 6174.26, SD = 1513.66) was significantly lower than the post-test score (M = 5610.49, SD = 1435.86) (see Figure 6).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Mean accuracy (A) and response time (B) for the Remote Associates Test or for the HTA and LTA groups. Error bars represent standard errors of the means. * p < 0.05.

To further verify whether there was a mediating mechanism in the process of stress affecting creativity using the bootstrapping method using PROCESS (Hayes, 2013) among flanker performances (model 4) (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). The 95% bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) was examined based on 1,000 bootstrap samples. The area under the curve with respect to increase (AUCi) was calculated using the trapezoidal method for HTA and LTA groups. Pruessner et al. (2003) pointed out that the method represented time-related changes and overall intensity of said changes in salivary cortisol and salivary alpha amylase levels. We performed z-transformed AUCi in sC and sAA data because of the individual differences in biological markers (sC and sAA), which were considered independent variables (sAAAUCi; sCAUCi). The changes in creative task performances were considered dependent variables (fluency, flexibility, and originality of the AUT; response time and accuracy of the RAT) and changes in flanker task performances were considered mediator variables (reaction time (FRT) and error rate interference effect (FEI) of flanker); meanwhile, the baseline of flanker and creative task performances were considered control variables. Table 2 presents the correlations among all variables.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlation coefficients between biological, Flanker and creative performance.

Results only showed that cognitive inhibition (reaction time interference effect of flanker) mediated the effect of stress (AUCi for sAA) on creativity (fluency of AUT), with an estimate of 0.59 and a 95% bootstrap CI of 0.0061–1.4137 (see Table 3, Figure 7). Based on this result, we claimed that cognitive inhibition was related to pre-post creativity performance (divergent thinking) in both HTA and LTA groups. Furthermore, this result supported the above results that the increase in inhibition control was associated with significantly decreased divergent thinking performance of the HTA group, while decrease in inhibition control was associated with significantly increased divergent thinking performance of the LTA group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mediation results presented based on 1,000 bootstrap resamples.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Mediation analyses of cognitive inhibition on the relationship between stress and creativity. Learning stress served as independent variable. Path a is the effect of stress on the proposed mediator, while path b represents the effect of the mediator on creativity. Path c’ shows stress’s direct effect on creativity. Stress using the indictor of sAAAUCi; inhibition, FRT; creativity, fluency. Result presented here is based on 1,000 bootstrap samples. *p < 0.05.

Discussion

The present study examined the mechanism underlying the effect of acute stress on creative thinking, and to what extent this mechanism is influenced by individual differences in trait anxiety. We found higher levels of salivary cortisol and salivary alpha amylase after the TSST, indicating that participants experienced robust activation of the HPA and SNS. These results were in keeping with previous research, indicating that these two effects are likely due to different neural mechanisms, including the slowly increasing and persistent sC responses during HPA activation and the sAA responses during SNS system activation (Kirschbaum and Hellhammer, 1989; Foley and Kirschbaum, 2010). It is understandable that cognitive tasks (Flanker and creative task) also induced the increase in biological indicators in that cognitive stressors could produce subjective and objective stress effects (MacLeod, 1991; Renaud and Blondin, 1997). In present study, it is important to note that individuals were exposed to robust and reliable stress situation whatever the stress was induced by stress task or by the creative task itself.

One interesting finding was that the HTA group had lower salivary cortisol and salivary alpha amylase than the LTA group. This was consistent with earlier observations, which showed that HTA individuals (who have a relatively low baseline compared with the LTA group) often experience stress in daily social life. This was in line with another study showing that individuals with social anxiety disorder had a decreased cortisol baseline to stress (Elzinga et al., 2010). One possible explanation was that HTA individuals might initially present increased adrenocortical activity, leading to activation of chronicity compensatory mechanisms, gradually resulting in the attenuation in cortisol (Steudte et al., 2011). The repeated exposure to stress then results in habituation reactions, thereby reducing the individual HPA axis response level (Schommer et al., 2003; Shirotsuki et al., 2009). LTA individuals (who have a relatively high baseline compared with the LTA group), on the other hand, might be more effective in responding to environmental threats owing to their higher cortisol secretion (Villada et al., 2016).

As for the RT interference effect, compared with the LTA group, the HTA group showed worse inhibition control ability before stress, but significantly better inhibition control after. This was consistent with the ACT, which explains that HTA reduces inhibitory control compared with LTA (Eysenck et al., 2007). More importantly, under a stress manipulation, we saw a reduction in interference, whereas LTA individuals showed an increase due to induced anxiety. Combined with the theory of social promotion (Baron, 1986), individuals under stress could narrow the scope of attention and pay closer attention to the target information without distractors because these individuals had no remaining resources to deal with irrelevant information (Chajut and Algom, 2003).

For creative thinking performance, the mediation analysis showed that the increase in inhibition control was associated with significantly decreased divergent thinking performance in the HTA group, while a decrease in inhibition control was associated with significantly increased divergent thinking performance in the LTA group. These results partially supported the hypothesis and were consistent with previous observations, which indicated that anxiety traits modulate biological responses related to cognitive control and representation of cognitive improvement in individuals with HTA under stress (Sehlmeyer et al., 2010). However, the mediation analysis indicated no significant effects of inhibition control on convergent thinking.

According to the ACT, HTA might not decrease effectivity under certain conditions, thereby enabling HTA individuals to recruit additional processing resources to match those of LTA individuals (Eysenck and Derakshan, 2011; Berggren and Derakshan, 2013; Ward et al., 2017). In our study, HTA enhanced top-down processing and thereby hampered divergent thinking. For LTA, the decreased influence of bottom-up automatic processes was more helpful in associating remote ideas during the divergent thinking task (Chrysikou et al., 2014; Barr et al., 2015; Beaty et al., 2016; Hao et al., 2016; Kenett et al., 2018). The finding indicated that the trait anxiety moderates the effect of inhibition control on creativity under stress. The effect of inhibition on creativity also differed with the type of creativity. In other words, a low-inhibition state would enable individuals to obtain potentially useful information in a semantic network through a free association for divergent thinking; however, such a state would cause more distraction, hampering convergent thinking (Eysenck, 1995; Radel et al., 2015).

However, it was surprising that the HTA and LTA groups did not significantly differ in their RAT performance. Our study revealed that there was a shorter response time and a higher accuracy rate after stress in both trait anxiety groups. In the RAT, which is a measure of convergent thinking, stress induction was associated with higher accuracy and shorter reaction times. One possible explanation is that acute stress could increase individual dopamine levels (Robbins and Arnsten, 2009), which might help to facilitate creative problem-solving, such as the RAT (Cristofori et al., 2018). Besides, the problem solution for RAT could involve analytical strategies and insight strategies (Kounios and Beeman, 2014). In negative affect state, people are inclined to use analytical strategies and perform higher accuracy in high negative affect compared to relatively low negative emotions (Shen et al., 2019). Stress is usually accompanied by a relatively higher negative affect which prompts individuals to apply more analytical strategies to facilitate convergent thinking.

Future Directions

Creative cognition involves recruitment of working memory (Chuderski and Jastrzębski, 2017, 2018), inhibition (Radel et al., 2015; Teng et al., 2018), and cognitive flexibility (Müller et al., 2016). Our findings indicated that acute stress impaired inhibitory control in LTA individuals but increased inhibitory control in HTA individuals. However, the mechanism underlying the effect of stress was no doubt very complex. Numerous studies showed that activation of the HPA axis was considered to have a significant impact on executive function (working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility). Increased cortisol also could impair working memory (Shields et al., 2015), reduce cognitive inhibition, and increase response inhibition (Shields et al., 2016). In terms of cognitive flexibility, the general conclusion was that stress impaired cognitive flexibility (Alexander et al., 2007; Shields et al., 2016). Recent research has shown that HPA axis activation reduces switching flexibility but increases individual flexibility (Goldfarb et al., 2017). To better understand the role of cognitive control in creative thinking, we need a deeper understanding of the relationship between executive control components and acute stress.

We also found individual differences in creativity under stress. Future research should focus next on individuals with high and low creativity (Beaty et al., 2018), which would enable the assessment of brain functional connectivity as a predictor of individual creative ability under acute stress.

Conclusion

The results showed that cognitive inhibition was influenced by trait anxiety and acute stress. Compared to before experiencing acute stress, there was a lack of cognitive inhibition in LTA individuals and they performed better on the AUT (fluency) after acute stress. HTA individuals, on the other hand, showed a decreased interference effect and reduced performance in the AUT (fluency, flexibility, and originality). In the RAT, there were shorter response times and increased accuracy after acute stress in both trait anxiety groups.

Thus, the findings suggest that cognitive control, which is modulated by changes in acute stress, influences creative cognition. The findings also indicated that acute stress can be influenced by anxiety, thus highlighting the crucial relation between creative cognition, acute stress, and individualdifferences.

Author Contributions

HD and WH designed the experiments. XW and ZW carried out the experiments. FZ analyzed the experimental results. WX and YK assisted with performing the experiments. HD wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the Humanity and Social Science Youth Foundation of Ministry of Education of China grant (16YJC190004), National Natural Science Foundation of China grant (31871118, 31700976), Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (GK201902011, GK201901006), Key project for Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment Towards Basic Education Quality at Beijing Normal University (2018-05-009-BZPK01), and Social Science Foundation of Shaanxi Province (13N151).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alexander, J. K., Hillier, A., Smith, R. M., Tivarus, M. E., and Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Beta-adrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 468–478. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.468

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K., and Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychol. Bull. 134:779. doi: 10.1037/a0012815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, M., and Oldham, G. R. (2006). The curvilinear relation between experienced creative time pressure and creativity: moderating effects of openness to experience and support for creativity. J. Appl. Psychol. 91:963. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.91.4.963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction–conflict theory: progress and problems. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 19, 1–40.

Google Scholar

Barr, N., Pennycook, G., Stolz, J. A., and Fugelsang, J. A. (2015). Reasoned connections: a dual-process perspective on creative thought. Think. Reason. 21, 61–75. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2014.895915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Benedek, M., Silvia, P. J., and Schacter, D. L. (2016). Creative cognition and brain network dynamics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 20, 87–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Kenett, Y. N., Christensen, A. P., Rosenberg, M. D., Benedek, M., Chen, Q., et al. (2018). Robust prediction of individual creative ability from brain functional connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 115, 1087–1092. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1713532115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaty, R. E., Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Jauk, E., and Benedek, M. (2014). The roles of associative and executive processes in creative cognition. Mem. Cogn. 42, 1186–1197. doi: 10.3758/s13421-014-0428-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Franz, F., Heene, M., and Neubauer, A. C. (2012). Differential effects of cognitive inhibition and intelligence on creativity. Pers. Individ. Dif. 53, 480–485. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berggren, N., and Derakshan, N. (2013). Attentional control deficits in trait anxiety: why you see them and why you don’t. Biol. Psychol. 92, 440–446. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beversdorf, D. Q., Hughes, J. D., Steinberg, B. A., Lewis, L. D., and Heilman, K. M. (1999). Noradrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility in problem solving. Neuroreport 10, 2763–2767. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199909090-00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bowden, E. M., and Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 35:634. doi: 10.3758/BF03195543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byron, K., Khazanchi, S., and Nazarian, D. (2010). The relationship between stressors and creativity: a meta-analysis examining competing theoretical models. J. Appl. Psychol. 95:201. doi: 10.1037/a0017868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chajut, E., and Algom, D. (2003). Selective attention improves under stress: implications for theories of social cognition. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 85, 231–248. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chrysikou, E. G., Weber, M. J., and Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). A matched filter hypothesis for cognitive control. Neuropsychologia 62, 341–355. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuderski, A., and Jastrzębski, J. (2017). Working memory facilitates insight instead of hindering it: comment on decaro, van stockum, and wieth (2016). J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 43, 1993–2004. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chuderski, A., and Jastrzębski, J. (2018). Much ado about aha!: insight problem solving is strongly related to working memory capacity and reasoning ability. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 147:257. doi: 10.1037/xge0000378

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., and Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Mechanisms of attentional biases towards threat in the anxiety disorders: an integrative review. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30:203. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cisler, J. M., and Olatunji, B. O. (2012). Emotion regulation and anxiety disorders. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 14, 182–187. doi: 10.1007/s11920-012-0262-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, P. J., Browning, M., Hammond, G., Notebaert, L., and MacLeod, C. (2014). The causal role of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in the modification of attentional bias: evidence from transcranial direct current stimulation. Biol. Psychiatry 76, 946–952. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cristofori, I., Salvi, C., Beeman, M., and Grafman, J. (2018). The effects of expected reward on creative problem solving. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 18, 925–931. doi: 10.3758/s13415-018-0613-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duan, H., Wang, X., Hu, W., and Kounios, J. (2019). Effects of acute stress on divergent and convergent problem-solving. Think Reason. 1–19. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2019.1572539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, E. J., Edwards, M. S., and Lyvers, M. (2015). Cognitive trait anxiety, situational stress and mental effort predict shifting efficiency: implications for attentional control theory. Emotion 15, 350–359. doi: 10.1037/emo0000051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edwards, M. S., Edwards, E. J., and Lyvers, M. (2017). Cognitive trait anxiety, stress and effort interact to predict inhibitory control. Cognit. Emot. 31, 671–686. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1152232

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elzinga, B. M., Spinhoven, P., Berretty, E. D., de Jong, P., and Roelofs, K. (2010). The role of childhood abuse in HPA-axis reactivity in social anxiety disorder: a pilot study. Biol. Psychol. 83, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.3758/BF03203267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erskine, J. A., Kvavilashvili, L., and Kornbrot, D. E. (2007). The predictors of thought suppression in young and old adults: effects of rumination, anxiety, and other variables. Pers. Individ. Dif. 42, 1047–1057. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity (Vol. 12). Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., and Derakshan, N. (2011). New perspectives in attentional control theory. Pers. Individ. Dif. 50, 955–960. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.08.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7, 336–353. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogelman, N., Mikhailik, A., Mueller-Alcazar, A., Bernard, K., and Canli, T. (2016). Stressing over anxiety: a novel interaction of 5-HTTPLR genotype and anxiety-related phenotypes in older adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 71, 36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foley, P., and Kirschbaum, C. (2010). Human hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis responses to acute psychosocial stress in laboratory settings. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 35, 91–96. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fonzo, G. A., and Etkin, A. (2017). Affective neuroimaging in generalized anxiety disorder: an integrated review. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 19, 169–179.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Friedman, N. P., and Miyake, A. (2004). The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 133:101. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilhooly, K. J., Fioratou, E., Anthony, S. H., and Wynn, V. (2007). Divergent thinking: strategies and executive involvement in generating novel uses for familiar objects. Br. J. Psychol. 98, 611–625. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.2007.tb00467.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goette, L., Bendahan, S., Thoresen, J., Hollis, F., and Sandi, C. (2015). Stress pulls us apart: anxiety leads to differences in competitive confidence under stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology 54, 115–123. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldfarb, E. V., Froböse, M. I., Cools, R., and Phelps, E. A. (2017). Stress and cognitive flexibility: cortisol increases are associated with enhanced updating but impaired switching. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 14–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greening, S. G., and Mitchell, D. G. (2015). A network of amygdala connections predict individual differences in trait anxiety. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4819–4830. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grillon, C., Robinson, O. J., Oconnell, K., Davis, A., Alvarez, G., Pine, D. S., et al. (2017). Clinical anxiety promotes excessive response inhibition. Psychol. Med. 47, 1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, S., Gao, M., Yan, Y., Wang, F., Tang, Y., and Huang, J. H. (2018). The neural mechanism underlying cognitive and emotional processes in creativity. Front. Psychol. 9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01924

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity research: past, present and future. Am. Psychol. 5, 444–454. doi: 10.1037/h0063487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hao, N., Ku, Y., Liu, M., Hu, Y., Bodner, M., Grabner, R. H., et al. (2016). Reflection enhances creativity: beneficial effects of idea evaluation on idea generation. Brain Cogn. 103, 30–37. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Ethn. Health 51, 335–337. doi: 10.1080/13557858.2017.1315056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermans, E. J., Henckens, M. J., Joëls, M., and Fernández, G. (2014). Dynamic adaptation of large-scale brain networks in response to acute stressors. Trends Neurosci. 37, 304–314. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2014.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izawa, S., Sugaya, N., Yamamoto, R., Ogawa, N., and Nomura, S. (2010). The cortisol awakening response and autonomic nervous system activity during nocturnal and early morning periods. Neuro Endocrinol. Lett. 31:685.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kenett, Y. N., Medaglia, J. D., Beaty, R. E., Chen, Q., Betzel, R. F., Thompson-Schill, S. L., et al. (2018). Driving the brain towards creativity and intelligence: a network control theory analysis. Neuropsychologia. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschbaum, C., and Hellhammer, D. H. (1989). Salivary cortisol in psychobiological research: an overview. Neuropsychobiology 22, 150–169. doi: 10.1159/000118611

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K. M., and Hellhammer, D. H. (1993). The ‘Trier Social Stress Test’–a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kounios, J., and Beeman, M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 65, 71–93. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115154

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., Kirschbaum, C., Harmon-Jones, E., and Winkielman, P. (2007). Ten years of research with the Trier Social Stress Test—revisited. Soc. Neurosci. 56:83.

Google Scholar

Kudielka, B. M., Hellhammer, D. H., and Wüst, S. (2009). Why do we respond so differently? Reviewing determinants of human salivary cortisol responses to challenge. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 2–18. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhlmann, S., Kirschbaum, C., and Wolf, O. T. (2005). Effects of oral cortisol treatment in healthy young women on memory retrieval of negative and neutral words. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 83, 158–162. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2004.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovelace, J. B., and Hunter, S. T. (2013). Charismatic, ideological, and pragmatic leaders' influence on subordinate creative performance across the creative process. Creat. Res. J. 25, 59–74. doi: 10.1080/10400419.2013.752228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol. Bull. 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McHugh, R. K., Behar, E., Gutner, C. A., Geem, D., and Otto, M. W. (2010). Cortisol, stress, and attentional bias toward threat. Anxiety Stress Coping 23, 529–545. doi: 10.1080/10615801003596969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McNally, R. J. (2006). Cognitive abnormalities in post-traumatic stress disorder. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 271–277. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary (1986). Institute of Language Teaching and Research. Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Press.

Google Scholar

Müller, B. C., Gerasimova, A., and Ritter, S. M. (2016). Concentrative meditation influences creativity by increasing cognitive flexibility. Psychol. Aesthet. Creat. Arts 10, 278–286. doi: 10.1037/a0040335

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarro, M., Miyamoto, N., van der Kamp, J., Morya, E., Ranvaud, R., and Savelsbergh, G. J. (2012). The effects of high pressure on the point of no return in simulated penalty kicks. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 34, 83–101. doi: 10.1123/jsep.34.1.83

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohly, S., and Fritz, C. (2010). Work characteristics, challenge appraisal, creativity, and proactive behavior: a multi-level study. J. Organ. Behav. 31, 543–565. doi: 10.1002/job.633

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). “Contemporary approaches to assessing mediation in communication research” in The Sage source book of advanced data analysis methods for communication research. 13–54.

Google Scholar

Probst, T. M., Stewart, S. M., Gruys, M. L., and Tierney, B. W. (2007). Productivity, counterproductivity and creativity: the ups and downs of job insecurity. J. Occup. Organ. Psychol. 80, 479–497. doi: 10.1348/096317906X159103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pruessner, J. C., Kirschbaum, C., Meinlschmid, G., and Hellhammer, D. H. (2003). Two formulas for computation of the area under the curve represent measures of total hormone concentration versus time-dependent change. Psychoneuroendocrinology 28, 916–931. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(02)00108-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Qureshi, S. U., Long, M. E., Bradshaw, M. R., Pyne, J. M., Magruder, K. M., Kimbrell, T., et al. (2011). Does PTSD impair cognition beyond the effect of trauma? J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 23, 16–28. doi: 10.1176/jnp.23.1.jnp16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radel, R., Davranche, K., Fournier, M., and Dietrich, A. (2015). The role of (dis) inhibition in creativity: decreased inhibition improves idea generation. Cognition 134, 110–120. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radel, R., Sarrazin, P., Legrain, P., and Wild, T. C. (2010). Social contagion of motivation between teacher and student: analyzing underlying processes. J. Educ. Psychol. 102, 577–587. doi: 10.1037/a0019051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Redick, T. S., and Engle, R. W. (2006). Working memory capacity and attention network test performance. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 713–721. doi: 10.1002/acp.1224

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Renaud, P., and Blondin, J. (1997). The stress of Stroop performance: physiological and emotional responses to color-word interference, task pacing, and pacing speed. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 27, 87–97. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(97)00049-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robbins, T. W., and Arnsten, A. F. (2009). The neuropsychopharmacology of fronto-executive function: monoaminergic modulation. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 267–287. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135535

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schommer, N. C., Hellhammer, D. H., and Kirschbaum, C. (2003). Dissociation between reactivity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis and the sympathetic-adrenal-medullary system to repeated psychosocial stress. Psychosom. Med. 65, 450–460. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000035721.12441.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sehlmeyer, C., Konrad, C., Zwitserlood, P., Arolt, V., Falkenstein, M., and Beste, C. (2010). Erp indices for response inhibition are related to anxiety-related personality traits. Neuropsychologia 48, 2488–2495. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, D. T. (1993). The Chinese version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: its relationship to different measures of psychological well-being. J. Clin. Psychol. 49, 349–358. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199305)49:3<349::AID-JCLP2270490308>3.0.CO;2-J

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shen, W., Zhao, Y., Hommel, B., Yuan, Y., Zhang, Y., Liu, Z., et al. (2019). The impact of spontaneous and induced mood states on problem solving and memory. Think. Skills Creat 32, 66–71. doi: 10.1016/j.tsc.2019.03.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, G. S., Bonner, J. C., and Moons, W. G. (2015). Does cortisol influence core executive functions? A meta-analysis of acute cortisol administration effects on working memory, inhibition, and set-shifting. Psychoneuroendocrinology 58, 91–103. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A., and Yonelinas, A. P. (2016). The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: a meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 651–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shirotsuki, K., Izawa, S., Sugaya, N., Yamada, K. C., Ogawa, N., Ouchi, Y., et al. (2009). Salivary cortisol and DHEA reactivity to psychosocial stress in socially anxious males. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 72, 198–203. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D. (1979). Understanding stress and anxiety. New York, NY: Harper & Rowe.

Google Scholar

Spielberger, C. D., Gorsuch, R. L., Lushene, R., Vagg, P. R., and Jacobs, G. A. (1983). Manual for the State-Trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Steudte, S., Stalder, T., Dettenborn, L., Klumbies, E., Foley, P., Beesdo-Baum, K., et al. (2011). Decreased hair cortisol concentrations in generalised anxiety disorder. Psychiatry Res. 186, 310–314. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Suedfeld, P., and Vernon, J. (1965). Stress and verbal originality in sensory deprivation. Psychol. Rec. 15, 567–570. doi: 10.1007/BF03393631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Teng, J., Shen, W., and Hao, N. (2018). The role of cognitive control in divergent thinking. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 26, 411–422. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00411

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villada, C., Hidalgo, V., Almela, M., and Salvador, A. (2016). Individual differences in the psychobiological response to psychosocial stress (Trier Social Stress Test): the relevance of trait anxiety and coping styles. Stress. Health 32, 90–99. doi: 10.1002/smi.2582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, R. T., Smith, S. L., Kraus, B. T., Allen, A. V., Moses, M. A., and Simondack, S. L. (2017). Alpha band frequency differences between low-trait and high-trait anxious individuals. Neuroreport 29, 79–83. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0000000000000915

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., Duan, H., Kan, Y., Wang, B., Qi, S., and Hu, W. (2019). The creative thinking cognitive process influenced by acute stress in humans: an electroencephalography study. Stress 22, 472–481. doi: 10.1080/10253890.2019.1604665

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weger, M., and Sandi, C. (2018). High anxiety trait: a vulnerable phenotype for stress-induced depression. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 87:27. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Medical Association (WMA) (2013). World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. Available from: www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/b3.htm

Google Scholar

Xu, S., Duan, H., Qi, S., Gao, J., and Hu, W. (2015). Development of Chinese compound remote association test. Paper presented at the meeting of the 18th national conference of Chinese Psychological Association; Tianjin, China.

Google Scholar

Yeh, Y. C., Lai, G. J., Lin, C. F., Lin, C. W., and Sun, H. C. (2015). How stress influences creativity in game-based situations: analysis of stress hormones, negative emotions, and working memory. Comput. Educ. 81, 143–153. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2014.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, Z., Hu, L., Sun, C., Li, M., Guo, F., and Zhao, Q. (2019). The effect of Zhongyong thinking on remote association thinking: an EEG study. Front. Psychol. 10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: acute stress, creative cognition, Trier social stress test, alternative uses test, remote associates test

Citation: Duan H, Wang X, Wang Z, Xue W, Kan Y, Hu W and Zhang F (2019) Acute Stress Shapes Creative Cognition in Trait Anxiety. Front. Psychol. 10:1517. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01517

Received: 28 September 2018; Accepted: 17 June 2019;
Published: 08 August 2019.

Edited by:

Wangbing Shen, Hohai University, China

Reviewed by:

Nick Berggren, Birkbeck, University of London, United Kingdom
Xiaoyang Yang, Sichuan Normal University, China
Xiang Zhou, Nankai University, China

Copyright © 2019 Duan, Wang, Wang, Xue, Kan, Hu and Zhang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Haijun Duan, duanhj@126.com; Weiping Hu, weipinghu@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.