Europe PMC

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


Over the first year, infants tune into the signals of their native language and begin to link them to meaning. Here, we ask whether infants, like adults, can also infer the communicative function of otherwise arbitrary signals (here, tone sequences) and link these to meaning as well. We examined 6-month-olds' object categorization in the context of sine-wave tones, a signal that fails to support categorization at any point during their first year. However, before the categorization task, we exposed infants to tones in one of two vignettes. In one, the tones were produced by an actor in a rich communicative exchange; in the other, infants heard the very same tones, but these were uncoupled from the actors' activity. Infants exposed to the communicative vignette successfully formed object categories in the subsequent test; those exposed to the non-communicative vignette failed, performing identically to infants with no prior exposure to this novel signal. This reveals in 6-month-old infants a remarkable flexibility in identifying which signals in the ambient environment are communicative and in linking these signals to core cognitive capacities including categorization.

Free full text 


Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
Cognition. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 Mar 13.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC5347446
NIHMSID: NIHMS845273
PMID: 26433024

What the [beep]? Six-month-olds Link Novel Communicative Signals to Meaning

Abstract

Over the first year, infants tune in to the signals of their native language and begin to link them to meaning. Here, we ask whether infants, like adults, can also infer the communicative function of otherwise arbitrary signals (here, tone sequences) and link these to meaning as well. We examined 6-month-olds’ object categorization in the context of sine-wave tones, a signal that fails to support categorization at any point during their first year. However, before the categorization task, we exposed infants to tones in one of two vignettes. In one, the tones were produced by an actor in a rich communicative exchange; in the other, infants heard the very same tones, but these were uncoupled from the actors’ activity. Infants exposed to the communicative vignette successfully formed object categories in the subsequent test; those exposed to the non-communicative vignette failed, performing identically to infants with no prior exposure to this novel signal. This reveals in 6-month-old infants a remarkable flexibility in identifying which signals in the ambient environment are communicative and in linking these signals to core cognitive capacities including categorization.

Keywords: infants, communication, language, categorization, social cognition

1. Introduction

Over the first year of life, infants tune in to the signals of their native language and begin to link them to meaning (Ferry, Hespos, & Waxman, 2010; 2013; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007; Gervain & Mehler, 2010; Saffran, Werker, & Werner, 2007; Vouloumanos, Hauser, Werker, & Martin, 2010; Waxman & Lidz, 2006). During this same period, they also become increasingly attuned to the communicative functions of other signals, including eye-gaze and pointing (Krehm, Onishi, & Vouloumanos, 2012; 2014; Liszkowski, 2008; Senju & Csibra, 2008). This early-emerging communicative competence flourishes over development. Indeed, a hallmark of being human is the flexibility with which we infuse otherwise arbitrary signals – from billows of smoke to Morse code tones – with communicative status. Our goal in the current experiment was to ask whether this capacity to infer communicative function in arbitrary signals is available to infants, or whether this flexibility requires the scaffolding of more fully developed social or linguistic capacities.

We take as our starting point recent evidence that listening to human language engages infants’ object categorization, a fundamental conceptual capacity (Ferry et al., 2010, 2013). Ferry and colleagues documented that, for infants as young as three months of age, listening to the vocalizations of either human or non-human primates promoted the formation of categories in a way that listening to well-matched sine-wave tone sequences did not. By six months, this facilitative effect on object categorization becomes tuned specifically to human vocalizations. Thus, well before infants begin to speak, they have already begun to link language and cognition, a link that will serve them well as they acquire the meanings of their first words (Brown, 1958; Medin & Rips, 2005; Murphy, 2004).

But is this link to cognition, once tuned specifically to human language, then reserved exclusively to human language? Or might a novel signal also promote categorization if 6-month-old infants could be convinced that it served a communicative function? Two emerging themes in the developmental literature support the latter possibility. First, by 6 months, infants have begun to appreciate the communicative function of speech as well as non-speech signals (Imafuku, Hakuno, Uchida-Ota, Yamamoto, & Minagawa, 2014; Lloyd-Fox, Széplaki-Köllőd, Yin, & Csibra, 2015; Parise & Csibra, 2013). For example, they expect that speech will be directed to people and not artefacts (Augusti, Melinder, & Gredebäck, 2010; Legerstee, Barna, & DiAdamo, 2000) and that speech can transmit information that non-communicative vocal sounds (e.g., coughing) cannot (Vouloumanos, Martin, & Onishi, 2014). By at least 8–9 months, infants also appreciate the communicative functions of eye gaze and pointing (Krehm et al., 2012; 2014; Senju & Csibra, 2008; Senju, Csibra, & Johnson, 2008). Second, at least for these older infants, these non-linguistic signals (e.g., eye gaze, pointing) may, like language, support infants’ learning about objects and object categories (Csibra & Gergely, 2009; Futó, Téglás, Csibra, & Gergely, 2010; Wu, Gopnik, Richardson, & Kirkham, 2011; Wu, Tummeltshammer, Gliga, & Kirkham, 2014; Yoon, Johnson, & Csibra, 2008).

But what remains unanswered is whether infants, like older children and adults, are flexible enough to identify a new communicative signal and relate it to meaning, as they do with language. To address this question, we examined 6-month-olds’ ability to form object categories while listening to a novel sound (a sine-wave tone sequence). We selected tone sequences because previous work documents that, unlike language, this sound fails to promote object categorization at any point within the first year (Balaban & Waxman, 1997; Ferry et al., 2010; Fulkerson & Haaf, 2006; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). This provided us with an opportunity to discover whether there were conditions under which infants would infuse this otherwise inert non-linguistic sound with communicative status and relate it to categorization.

Our design is straightforward: We first exposed infants to sine-wave tone sequences within the context of a brief videotaped vignette. Each vignette featured two female actors, engaged happily with one another in a joint social activity. Importantly, we developed two vignettes that differed in the way in which the tones were embedded in the actors’ interchange (see Figure 1). In the Communicative condition, the tones were embedded within a rich social communicative exchange in which one actor spoke and the other “beeped.” In the Non-communicative condition, infants heard the very same tone sequences, but these were uncoupled from the dialogue; they were no longer embedded in their communicative exchange. After this exposure period, all infants participated in the same object categorization task (Ferry et al., 2010; 2013; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms845273f1.jpg

A representation of the procedure. Infants were first exposed to the novel sound stimulus (sine-wave tones) in the context of either a Communicative or Non-communicative vignette. Next, they participated in an object categorization task while listening to tones.

This design permitted us to explore infants’ categorization in the context of a novel communicative signal in two ways. First, by varying the way in which tone sequences were embedded in the exposure vignette, we could identify whether communicative experience with the tones permitted infants to link them to object categories. Second, by exposing all infants to precisely the same set of sine-wave tone sequences, we could assess claims that signal familiarity alone can account for the influence of auditory stimuli on visual categorization (e.g., Robinson & Sloutsky, 2007a; 2007b; Sloutsky & Robinson, 2008).

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

Twenty-four healthy, full-term 6-month-olds (M=5.91 months, range=5.52–6.44, 13 F) participated. An additional 7 infants were tested but replaced due to looking less than 25% of the time (i.e., accumulating, on average, less than 5s of looking during either familiarization or test; N=6) or fussing out of the task before the test trials (N=1). One other infant, who was identified as an outlier (>2.5 MADs and >2 SDs from the condition mean), was replaced.

2.2. Apparatus

Infants sat on their caregivers’ laps approximately 110 cm from the centre of a white projector screen. The projected image was 125 cm (width) by 79 cm (height), although only the dialogue video used the full area of the projection. Auditory stimuli were played on two Audioengine A5 speakers placed beneath the screen 82cm apart. The speakers and other equipment were concealed with black fabric. Sessions were recorded with a videocamera through a 3 cm hole in the fabric beneath the screen.

2.3. Procedure and Materials

The experiment included three phases: exposure, familiarization, and test (see Figure 1). In the exposure phase, infants viewed one of two 2-minute videotaped vignettes, each featuring two women sitting next to each other while engaged in joint activity. In the Communicative condition, they were engaged in a communicative exchange. The “speaker” spoke in infant-directed speech and the “beeper” responded in sine-wave tones that had been dubbed in and synchronized with her mouth movements. This “conversation” included several cues to communicative status of the tone sequences. For example, the speaker and beeper made eye contact, smiled, and waved to each other (and to the infant). In the Non-communicative condition, several cues from the Communicative condition were preserved: the women made frequent eye contact, and engaged one another (and the infant) by smiling and waving, and infants heard the very same auditory signals (both the tone-sequences and infant-directed utterances). Crucially, however, in the Non-communicative condition, the sound track was uncoupled entirely from the women’s activity. In this vignette, the women did not engage in a conversation, but instead cooperated silently in a shared task (mixing ingredients as if they were baking together) while the audio played in the background (as if they were listening to the radio or a CD). We designed the vignettes to insure that the amount and intensity of movement in which the actors were engaged were roughly comparable in the Communicative and Non-communicative vignettes. In both, the actors remained seated; they waved and smiled at one another and at the infant, laughed, turned their heads and nudged one another with their arms. In the Non-communicative vignette, each actor gently stirred liquid in a transparent bowl; they stirred continuously to align the amount of activity well with the continuous conversation in the Communicative condition. Finally, the order in which the beeper’s and speaker’s utterances were presented was shuffled (between-utterances) in the Non-communicative condition; the phrases from the Communicative condition remained intact but the shuffling eliminated the sense of conversational turn-taking.

After the exposure phase, infants in all condition were treated identically. They participated in a categorization task patterned closely after previous studies (Ferry et al., 2010; 2013; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). During familiarization, infants saw 8 line-drawn, colored images, all members of a single object category (either dinosaurs or fish, counterbalanced). Each image appeared for 20s and was paired with a single tone sequence (2.2s in duration), presented at image onset and repeated 10s later. This tone sequence differed in both rhythm and pitch (400hz) from those presented in the exposure phase. At test, we presented a spinning colorful wheel at the center of the screen to focus infants’ attention. Next, two new images appeared: a new member of the now-familiar category (e.g., another dinosaur) and a member of a novel category (e.g., a fish). Test images were presented side-by-side, in silence, for 20s. The left/right position of the novel image was counterbalanced across participants.

2.4. Analysis

Infants’ eye gaze directions were coded frame-by-frame by trained coders blind to the hypotheses, and 50% from each condition were recoded by a second coder to assess reliability (r(10)=.98, 95% CI [.93, .99], p<.001). As in previous work with this paradigm (e.g., Ferry et al., 2010; 2013; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007), a novelty preference score was calculated for each infant (accumulated time looking toward the novel test object/accumulated time looking toward both the novel and familiar test objects) based on infants’ first 10s of looking during test. (Analyses based on the entire test phase yielded the same pattern of results and p-values.) All statistical tests used arcsine root transformations. This transformation permits us to analyze bounded proportional data using linear models that assume an unbounded dependent measure. In describing the results in text, we report raw summary statistics for clarity. Because a preliminary ANOVA revealed no reliable effects of test image position, familiarization category, or participant gender (all p’s>.31), further analyses collapsed across these factors.

3. Results

Infants’ categorization performance varied reliably as a function of the vignette to which they had been exposed, t(22)=2.15, p=.043, Cohen’s d=.88 (see Figure 2). Infants in the Communicative condition revealed a robust preference for the novel object, indicating that they had successfully formed an object category (M=.61, SD=.15; comparison to chance: t(11)=2.50, p=.029). In fact, this preference was statistically indistinguishable from their 6-month-old counterparts listening to speech (M=.63, SD=.19), t(42)=−.32, p=.75 (Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). In sharp contrast, infants in the Non-communicative condition who were exposed to the very same tone sequences, uncoupled from the communicative exchange, failed to form object categories (M=.48, SD=.15; t(11)=−.45, p=.66). Performance in this condition mirrored the chance level performance of their counterparts listening to tones in the absence of any prior exposure (M=.54, SD=.20), t(42)=−.87, p=.39 (Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007). Thus, merely exposing infants to tones did not affect their object categorization in this task. Instead, what was required was that the tones be introduced within a communicative context.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms845273f2.jpg

Infants’ proportion of looking to the novel object at test by condition. Nine of 12 infants in the Communicative condition preferred the novel object, as compared to 6 of 12 in the Non-communicative condition. Note: * denotes p < .05.

Importantly, this difference between the Communicative and Non-communicative conditions at test cannot be attributed to differences in infants’ attention during the exposure or familiarization phases. There were no differences between these conditions in the proportion of time that infants attended either to the exposure vignettes (Communicative: M=.85, SD=.20; Non-communicative: M=.83, SD=.11), t(22)=.73, p=.47, or to the familiarization objects (Communicative: M=.43, SD=.15; Non-communicative: M=.47, SD=.15), t(22)=−0.60, p=.55.

4. Discussion

This work provides new insights into infants’ early communicative capacity and its links to cognition. First, it offers the first evidence that infants as young as six months of age can successfully elevate novel, otherwise inert sounds to communicative status. This extends recent evidence that by six months, infants have begun to appreciate the distinct functions of several familiar communicative signals (Augusti, Melinder, & Gredebäck, 2010; Legerstee et al., 2000; Lloyd-Fox et al., 2015; Parise & Csibra, 2013; Senju & Csibra, 2008; Vouloumanos et al., 2014) and goes further to document that 6-month-olds also have the capacity to identify novel signals as communicative if those signals have been embedded in a rich communicative exchange. Second, we show that once a novel signal has been identified as communicative, it can then promote object categorization – mirroring the facilitative effects engendered by listening to language (Ferry et al., 2010; 2013; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007).

The current results challenge the view that appealing to signal familiarity alone can account for the influences of auditory input on infants’ categorization. Some researchers have argued that infants’ successful categorization in the context of hearing words (but not non-linguistic signals like tones) is attributable entirely to signal familiarity (e.g., Robinson & Sloutsky, 2007a; 2007b; Sloutsky & Robinson, 2008). The claim is that familiar sounds (like speech) interfere less with infants’ processing of visual materials than do unfamiliar sounds (like tone sequences). Notice however that this argument cannot account for the results reported here. After all, we held familiarity with sine-wave tones constant; all infants heard precisely the same tone sequences within a brief vignette. If infants’ success in forming object categories rested on their familiarity with the tone sequences alone, then infants in both conditions should have performed identically. Yet this was not the case. Instead, infants’ performance varied as a function of the communicative context in which the tones were introduced. Thus, the current results underscore the inadequacy of appealing to signal familiarity alone to account for the facilitative effect of language on categorization (see also Althaus & Plunkett, 2015; Booth & Waxman, 2009; Ferry et al., 2013; Gliga, Volein, & Csibra, 2010; Noles & Gelman, 2012; Plunkett, 2008).

These intriguing findings also open several paths for future research. First, it will be important to identify which features of the vignette (separately or in unison) permit 6-month-olds to link an otherwise non-communicative signal to a cognitive process like categorization. Several features, present in the Communicative but absent in the other condition, are likely candidates. For example, the cooperative, turn-taking between tones and speech may be instrumental (Johnson, 2003; Watson, 1972). This would be consistent with evidence that if an entirely novel object “converses” by beeping, then 9- to 12-month-old infants expect that that object is a communicative agent; in contrast, they have no such expectations for novel objects that beep, but do so inconsistently (Beier & Carey, 2013; Johnson, Slaughter, & Carey, 1998). It will also be interesting to ascertain whether turn-taking itself is sufficient, or whether a novel signal must take its conversational turns with speech (Vouloumanos et al., 2014; see also Csibra, 2010). By manipulating these features and others (e.g., audio-visual synchrony, human versus non-human actors) in the exposure paradigm that we have designed here, it will be possible to specify more clearly what is required for infants to identify a novel signal as communicative and to relate it to the objects and events in their world.

A second challenge will be to identify the mechanism underlying the cognitive consequences of listening to communicative signals (e.g., “communicative” tones, language). For example, it could be that communicative signals direct infants’ attention specifically toward information that is category-relevant. This proposal, which is consistent with the theory of natural pedagogy (Csibra & Gergely, 2006; 2009), predicts the advantageous effect of communicative signals promote categorization, a fundamental processes underlying generalization (Ferry et al., 2010; 2013; Fulkerson & Waxman, 2007; see also Futó et al., 2010; Hernik & Csibra, 2015; Yoon et al., 2008). Alternatively, communicative signals might have a more general cognitive effect, heightening infants’ attention more broadly to the objects and events that surround them (Ferry et al., 2010; Kuhl, 2007). This proposal predicts that communicative signals will also enhance cognitive and attentional capacities beyond object categorization, a possibility that is consistent with several recent observations (Kuhl, Tsao, & Liu, 2003; Marcus, Fernandes, & Johnson, 2007; Thiessen, Hill, & Saffran, 2005; Vouloumanos & Waxman, 2014; Wu et al., 2011; 2014). To further adjudicate between these alternatives, it will be important to identify which other cognitive capacities (if any) are supported in the context of novel communicative signals (e.g., Ferguson & Lew-Williams, 2014; see also Vouloumanos & Waxman, 2014), and whether the underlying mechanisms that link linguistic signals (e.g., speech, manual gestures) to meaning differ from those linking non-linguistic signals (e.g., communicative tones).

In closing, we have shown that six-month-old infants are remarkably flexible in identifying which signals in the ambient environment are communicative and in linking these signals to core cognitive capacities including categorization. This outcome sets the stage for asking whether infants learn, in their first months, that speech is communicative or whether they are endowed at birth with this expectation (cf. Ferry et al., 2013). Pursuing this question will bring us closer to identifying the developmental origin of infants’ expectations about the communicative power of language and its links to cognition.

Highlights

  • Listening to language has been shown to facilitate infants’ object categorization throughout the first year of life.

  • We ask whether there are conditions under which six-month-olds will link a novel signal to categorization in the same way.

  • Our findings reveal that a novel signal can indeed facilitate infants’ object categorization, but only if it is presented as part of a communicative exchange.

  • This effect cannot be reduced to signal familiarity, in contrast to a competing account of the influence of auditory stimuli on categorization.

  • Links between language and concepts may emerge from a broad initial link between communicative signals and cognition.

Supplementary Material

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Doctoral Fellowship awarded to B.F. and a National Science Foundation grant (BCS-1023300) to S.R.W. We gratefully acknowledge the infants and parents who participated in these experiments. We also thank Sheena Desai and Tracy Smith for acting in the vignettes, Ilana Clift and Jane Hohman for data collection, and Jacob Dink for comments on a previous version of this manuscript. Portions of these data were presented at the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society in Berlin and the 38th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • Althaus N, Plunkett K. Timing matters: The impact of label synchrony on infant categorisation. Cognition. 2015;139:1–9. 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.02.004. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Augusti E-M, Melinder A, Gredebäck G. Look who’s talking: pre-verbal infants’ perception of face-to-face and back-to-back social interactions. Frontiers in Psychology. 2010;1(161) 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00161. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Balaban MT, Waxman SR. Do words facilitate object categorization in 9-month-old infants? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 1997;64(1):3–26. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Beier JS, Carey S. Contingency is not enough: Social context guides third-party attributions of intentional agency. Developmental Psychology. 2013;50(3):889–902. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Brown R. Words and things. Glencoe, IL: Free Press; 1958. [Google Scholar]
  • Csibra G. Recognizing communicative intentions in infancy. Mind & Language. 2010;25(2):141–168. [Google Scholar]
  • Csibra G, Gergely G. Social learning and social cognition: The case for pedagogy. In: Munakata Y, Johnson MH, editors. Processes of change in brain and cognitive development. Attention and performance XXI. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2006. pp. 249–274. [Google Scholar]
  • Csibra G, Gergely G. Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13(4):148–153. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferguson B, Lew-Williams C. Communicative signals promote abstract rule learning by 7-month-old infants. In: Bello P, Guarini M, McShane M, Scassellati B, editors. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society; 2014. pp. 2192–2198. [Google Scholar]
  • Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: An advantage of words over tones. Child Development. 2010;81(2):472–479. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Ferry AL, Hespos SJ, Waxman SR. Nonhuman primate vocalizations support categorization in very young human infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110(38):15231–15235. 10.1073/pnas.1221166110. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fulkerson AL, Haaf RA. Does object naming aid 12-month-olds’ formation of novel object categories? First Language. 2006;26(4):347–361. 10.1177/0142723706059217. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fulkerson AL, Waxman SR. Words (but not tones) facilitate object categorization: Evidence from 6-and 12-month-olds. Cognition. 2007;105(1):218–228. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Futó J, Téglás E, Csibra G, Gergely G. Communicative function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants. Cognition. 2010;117(1):1–8. 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.06.003. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gervain J, Mehler J. Speech perception and language acquisition in the first year of life. Annual Review of Psychology. 2010;61:191–218. 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.100408. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gliga T, Volein A, Csibra G. Verbal labels modulate perceptual object processing in 1-year-old children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22(12):2781–2789. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Hernik M, Csibra G. Infants learn enduring functions of novel tools from action demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2015;130(C):176–192. 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.10.004. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Imafuku M, Hakuno Y, Uchida-Ota M, Yamamoto JI, Minagawa Y. “Mom called me!” Behavioral and prefrontal responses of infants to self-names spoken by their mothers. NeuroImage. 2014;103:476–484. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.034. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson SC. Detecting agents. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2003;358(1431):549–559. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1237. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson SC, Slaughter V, Carey S. Whose gaze will infants follow? The elicitation of gaze-following in 12-month-olds. Developmental Science. 1998;1(2):233–238. [Google Scholar]
  • Krehm M, Onishi KH, Vouloumanos A. I See Your Point: Infants Under 12 Months Understand That Pointing Is Communicative. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2014;15(4):527–538. 10.1080/15248372.2012.736112. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kuhl PK. Is speech learning “gated” by the social brain? Developmental Science. 2007;10(1):110–120. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Kuhl PK, Tsao FM, Liu HM. Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2003;100(15):9096–9101. 10.1073/pnas.1532872100. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Legerstee M, Barna J, DiAdamo C. Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: Understanding people and their actions. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36(5):627–634. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Liszkowski U. Before L1: A differentiated perspective on infant gestures. Gesture. 2008;8(2):180–196. 10.1075/gest.8.2.04lis. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lloyd-Fox S, Széplaki-Köllőd B, Yin J, Csibra G. Cortex. 2015:1–37. 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.005. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Marcus GF, Fernandes KJ, Johnson SP. Infant Rule Learning Facilitated by Speech. Psychological Science. 2007;18(5):387–391. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01910.x. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Medin DL, Rips LJ. Concepts and categories: Memory, meaning, and metaphysics. The Cambridge Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning. 2005:37–72. [Google Scholar]
  • Murphy GL. The Big Book of Concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2004. [Google Scholar]
  • Noles NS, Gelman SA. Effects of categorical labels on similarity judgments: A critical analysis of similarity-based approaches. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48(3):890–896. 10.1037/a0026075. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Parise E, Csibra G. Neural Responses to Multimodal Ostensive Signals in 5-Month-Old Infants. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(8):7. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Plunkett K. Labels can override perceptual categories in early infancy. Cognition. 2008;106(2):665–681. 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.003. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM. Linguistic labels and categorization in Infancy: Do labels facilitate or hinder? Infancy. 2007a;11(3):233–253. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Robinson CW, Sloutsky VM. Visual processing speed: effects of auditory input on visual processing. Developmental Science. 2007b;10(6):734–740. 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00627.x. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Saffran JR, Werker JF, Werner LA. The infant’s auditory world: Hearing, speech, and the beginnings of language. Handbook of Child Psychology 2007 [Google Scholar]
  • Senju A, Csibra G. Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals. Current Biology. 2008;18(9):668–671. 10.1016/j.cub.2008.03.059. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Senju A, Csibra G, Johnson MH. Understanding the referential nature of looking: Infants’ preference for object-directed gaze. Cognition. 2008;108(2):303–319. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.02.009. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sloutsky VM, Robinson CW. The Role of Words and Sounds in Infants’ Visual Processing: From Overshadowing to Attentional Tuning. Cognitive Science. 2008;32(2):342–365. 10.1080/03640210701863495. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Thiessen ED, Hill EA, Saffran JR. Infant-directed speech facilitates word segmentation. Infancy. 2005;7(1):53–71. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Vouloumanos A, Waxman SR. Listen up! Speech is for thinking during infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18(12):642–646. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.10.001. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Vouloumanos A, Hauser MD, Werker JF, Martin A. The tuning of human neonates’ preference for speech. Child Development. 2010;81(2):517–527. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Vouloumanos A, Martin A, Onishi KH. Do 6-month-olds understand that speech can communicate? Developmental Science. 2014;17(6):872–879. 10.1111/desc.12170. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Watson JS. Smiling, cooing, and “the game” Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development. 1972:323–339. [Google Scholar]
  • Waxman SR, Lidz J. Early word learning. In: Kuhn D, Siegler R, editors. Handbook of Child Psychology. 6. Vol. 2. Hoboken, NJ, USA: Wiley; 2006. pp. 299–335. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Waxman SR, Lidz J, Braun IE, Lavin T. Twenty four-month-old infants’ interpretations of novel verbs and nouns in dynamic scenes. Infant Pathways to Language:, Methods, Models and Research Directions. 2009;59(1):67–95. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2009.02.001. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wu R, Gopnik A, Richardson DC, Kirkham NZ. Infants learn about objects from statistics and people. Developmental Psychology. 2011;47(5):1220–1229. 10.1037/a0024023. [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wu R, Tummeltshammer KS, Gliga T, Kirkham NZ. Ostensive signals support learning from novel attention cues during infancy. Frontiers in Psychology. 2014;5(251) 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00251. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yoon JMD, Johnson MH, Csibra G. Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2008;105(36):13690–13695. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Alternative metrics

Altmetric item for https://www.altmetric.com/details/4572190
Altmetric
Discover the attention surrounding your research
https://www.altmetric.com/details/4572190

Smart citations by scite.ai
Smart citations by scite.ai include citation statements extracted from the full text of the citing article. The number of the statements may be higher than the number of citations provided by EuropePMC if one paper cites another multiple times or lower if scite has not yet processed some of the citing articles.
Explore citation contexts and check if this article has been supported or disputed.
https://scite.ai/reports/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.09.020

Supporting
Mentioning
Contrasting
2
45
0

Article citations


Go to all (25) article citations

Data 


Data behind the article

This data has been text mined from the article, or deposited into data resources.

Similar Articles 


To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.


Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

NICHD NIH HHS (1)

National Science Foundation (1)

Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada