Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychol., 03 June 2020
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Towards a Meaningful Instrumental Music Education. Methods, Perspectives, and Challenges View all 21 articles

Shall We Play the Same? Pedagogical Perspectives on Infants’ and Children’s Imitation of Musical Gestures

\r\nManuela Filippa,,*Manuela Filippa1,2,3*Maria Grazia MonaciMaria Grazia Monaci3Susan YoungSusan Young4Didier GrandjeanDidier Grandjean1Gianni NutiGianni Nuti3Jacqueline NadelJacqueline Nadel5
  • 1Neuroscience of Emotion and Affective Dynamics Laboratory, Department of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Swiss Center for Affective Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 2Division of Development and Growth, Department of Paediatrics, Gynaecology and Obstetrics, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland
  • 3Department of Social Sciences, University of Valle d’Aosta, Aosta, Italy
  • 4University of Roehampton London, London, United Kingdom
  • 5Sorbonne-University, CNRS UMR 7225, Paris, France

Imitation, both gestural and vocal, has been acknowledged to be at the origin of human communication (Donald, 1991). Music is often considered to be the first means of communication of emotion via both vocal and gestural synchronization (Malloch, 1999; Malloch and Trevarthen, 2009). Instrumental music is part of the human heritage for more than 35,000 years before our era (Aimé et al., 2020). However, very little is known about the acquisition of gestures that produce sounds (i.e., musical gestures) and their role in the development of music and musicality. In the present paper, we propose that studying early synchronous imitation of musical gestures is essential both for investigating the development of the early action–perception system and for outlining early music interventions during infancy. We designed double musical objects which can be used in preschool music education for prompting synchronic imitation of musical gestures between adult and child, and between dyads of infants. We conclude by proposing a novel pedagogical perspective in music education for the early years which links the privileged orientation of infants and children towards sound discoveries with the development of perception-action coupling via imitation of musical gestures.

Introduction

Imitative multimodal abilities have been acknowledged to be fundamental during infancy, for acquiring linguistic, social and emotional competences (Simpson et al., 2014; Kugiumutzakis and Trevarthen, 2015). If vocal imitation is correlated with speech development (Kuhl and Meltzoff, 1996), gestural imitation is also highly predictive of communicative development (Uzgiris, 1972, 1999). Through functional plasticity, early imitation is at the core of both learning and communication experience (Nadel, 2014).

From birth onwards, infants imitate gestures (Field et al., 1982; Meltzoff and Moore, 1983; Soussignan et al., 2011; Meltzoff et al., 2018), and sounds (Kugiumutzakis, 1998) that they have in their motor repertoire. They also imitate the rhythm of a vocal model (Papoušek and Papoušek, 1989), thus underlining that imitation is an interpersonal phenomenon. Within this framework, reciprocal imitations of early vocal interactions in mother-infant dyads are well documented (Kugiumutzakis, 2017). Musical acoustic techniques applied to proto- conversations show how a mother speaking to her infant produces musically organized utterances to which the infant responds synchronously by moving in rhythmic expressive ways (Trevarthen, 2002). Indeed, gestural synchronic imitation is the first format underlying music, very early in infancy (Zentner and Eerola, 2010), as shown by the widespread practice of infant-directed music production such as lullabies and nursery rhymes (Trehub et al., 1993; Trehub, 2000).

Though synchrony is needed to perform any social interaction, a privileged use of synchronic imitation is music: piano duets, orchestral playing, jam sessions, singing together, all require matching one’s rhythm to that of a partner. However, the evolution of young children’s motor imitation during musical play has been poorly investigated.

This perspective paper aims to underline the potentialities of early synchronic imitation of musical gestures; this particular category of gestures that produce sound. As a novel research field, it could first, give new insights into the origins and the development of human musical experience, with specific emphasis on children’s music production with objects and related emotional experience during such practice. Secondly, it can constitute an excellent context for identifying the development of perception-action coupling in music production. Finally, we suggest potential lines of research, both in music education and in music psychology. We propose designs and procedures enabling to elicit the imitation of musical gestures for music education and early therapeutic interventions in infancy.

Imitation in Early Phases of Development

It is argued that neonatal imitation is a specific form of general imitation abilities that develop throughout the lifespan (Maratos, 1998). Neonatal imitation, although experience-dependent, adapts action to perception with high plasticity. The plasticity of early imitation is of paramount importance for further development as it allows vicarious processes to take place (Nadel, 2014). Interestingly for our specific perspective, newborns imitate not only facial gestures but also precise bodily movements such as finger movements and can match the specific dynamics of an action pattern (Nagy et al., 2005).

The development of imitation in infancy requires the acquisition of a wide range of procedures around the same motor patterns, including vocal patterns, the storage of corresponding sensorimotor feedbacks and motor imagery, and the recall of somatotopic and proprioceptive information related to movement and sound perception (Nadel, 2014). Indeed, imitation enriches an individual’s motor repertoire with gestures and actions that are acquired via watching or listening to other people. Moreover, besides the learning function, an essential function of imitation during infancy is non-verbal communication through synchronic sharing of motor patterns between partners (Nadel, 2014). The interactive emotional engagement during such shared actions producing sounds is likely to help the development of social and emotional abilities. During synchronic imitation, infants co-regulate their actions with an adult (Fogel, 1993) and, from two months on, they take conversational turns by alternating the roles of imitator and model, in a circle of reciprocity (Nadel, 2002). Many studies have demonstrated that infants immediately know if someone is synchronously engaged with them or not (Murray and Trevarthen, 1985; Nadel et al., 1999). In adults, electrophysiological studies reveal that synchrony emerges in the oscillation of brain waves during synchronic imitation (Tognoli et al., 2007; Dumas et al., 2010). When we do the same at the same time, we interact via sharing actions or representations. We share our motor patterns, the most intimate part of ourselves. We generate similar motor outputs that produce common visual and auditory outcomes (Nadel, 2014).

Within this framework, sound appears to play a crucial role in the development of infants’ and children’s ability to match others’ behaviors (Jones, 2009; Piaget, 2013). Although human beings need imitation abilities to synchronize with others during interactions, a privileged context requiring synchronous capacities is music production.

Music as a Privileged Context for Synchronic Imitation

Moving rhythmically to a musical beat and synchronizing to a proposed rhythm is observed in every human culture (Nettl, 2000) and very early in infancy (Zentner and Eerola, 2010), making it probably a universal musical experience. Synchronic imitation of movements, voices and sounds is one of the key characteristics of music-making during ritual experiences (Dissanayake, 2006). Human orientation and the ability to synchronize to proposed rhythm sequences could represent a cognitive adaptation for music-making (Patel and Iversen, 2006; Cross and Woodruff, 2009; Fitch and Popescu, 2019). Synchronous imitation of rhythms and gestures have positive effects on affiliation processes (Hove and Risen, 2009) and on the increase of prosocial behaviors in infancy (Kirschner and Tomasello, 2010; Cirelli et al., 2014). Peer imitation significantly influences expressive movements in response to music stimulus, in both preschool and kindergarten children (Flohr and Brown, 1979).

It is known that music-making with others is a multimodal activity that engages brain networks, including the mirror neuron system, particularly in the case of sensorimotor synchronization (Repp and Su, 2013). During music performance, where participants were asked to judge the degree of “groove” in the music presentation, sensorimotor coupling strength was significantly associated with higher levels of groove (Tomic and Janata, 2008).

In adults, areas of the brain associated with the processing of reward are active when individuals experience synchrony during joint music-making, and these reward signals are associated with an increase of prosocial behavior toward the music partner (Kokal et al., 2011). It has been shown that subjective entrainment, at both motor and visceral levels, with music tempi and rhythms, is correlated with emotional feelings emerging during music listening (Labbé and Grandjean, 2014). Furthermore, in Wiltermuth and Heath (2009) study, participants engaged in synchronous or asynchronous walking, singing, and hand movement activities. The participants who had synchronized with each other proved to be more cooperative than the others. Whether this is the case also for children, is still unknown. Double musical objects may offer a suitable methodology to document this question.

Even if synchronous imitation is commonly adopted in music learning, its pedagogical applications have just started to be of systematic interest (Buccino et al., 2004). Emerging research, for example, is starting to reflect on the role of mirror neurons in music teaching and learning abilities (e.g., Addessi and Volpe, 2011; Schiavio and Timmers, 2016).

The Origins of Music Production: the Musical Gesture

In recent years, embodied music cognition has started to reconsider the role and foundations of musical gestures in adults (Godøy and Leman, 2010). From this perspective, music cognition is not just a matter of perception, but involves the whole human body via acting (Varela et al., 2016). Likewise, in infancy, sound perception is linked to the motor action performed to create it. Active experience is critical to modulate motor activity during action perception early in development (Gerson et al., 2015). Among 10-month-old infants the researchers demonstrated a direct, causal effect of the active experience of sound-making gestures on the neural correlates of the same action perception (Gerson et al., 2015).

Similarly, musical gestures match their temporal profiles with the emotion expressed (Juslin and Laukka, 2004) and support universal expressions of emotion (Sievers et al., 2013), as physical and mental parts of “inner life” (Langer, 1957). Music, voice and movement are not only intimately related and share the same dynamic model for expressing emotions (Bänziger et al., 2009; Frühholz et al., 2016), but they are also the basis of human communicative musicality (Malloch, 1999). In the same way, musical gestures can represent a medium to create contact with a patient in therapeutic contexts (Benenzon, 2007).

As recently reviewed by Flilippa in a submitted paper, only a few studies that represents Figure 1 have investigated the origin and ontogenesis of musical gestures, and none of them has examined the role of perception-coupling via synchronic imitation in the development of joint music-making.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Gives the example of two specific tools which have been tested in kindergarten. Two examples of identical and shared sound objects. Adapted with permission from Nuti and Filippa (2016).

However, in seminal studies mainly conducted for pedagogical purposes, we can find qualitative descriptions of the acquisition of musical gestures. In Delalande’s descriptions (Delalande, 2015) when infants and young children create sounds by acting on an object like a bunch of keys or a more complex object like a zither or a cymbal, they produce repeated gestures, with simple variations. When a particular sound holds the child’s attention, they stand still for some seconds, and they often gaze at a caring listener, to share this new, significant discovery. This new sound discovery is defined as trouvaille (Delalande, 2015; Filippa, 2015). Attracted by this new sound, children concentrate on sound production and they continue to explore it, through repetitions and variations, a fundamental source of learning (Imberty, 2003; Ambrosini et al., 2013). Thus, they develop new musical gestures, and they enlarge their motor repertoire. Using our design with identical double objects, each young partner of a dyad would benefit from the other’s discovery, and would enlarge their motor repertoire by listening and playing the same.

Imitation With Double Identical Sound Objects

Given that synchrony is the primary vehicle of emotional communication, and that synchronic imitation offers an ideal way to learn and communicate at the same time, we propose to use the procedure designed by Nadel with sound objects. Nadel’s team developed a large series of experiments in order to test the existence of a non-verbal format of communication via synchronic imitation prior to language (see for instance Nadel and Badonnière, 1882; Nadel and Pezé, 1993; Nadel, 2002, 2014). To allow synchrony to spontaneously take place, identical objects were presented to dyads aged 15, 18, 21, 24, and 30 months and triads aged 30, 36, and 42 months. In one study comparing the same dyads aged 24 months when presented with 10 duplicate objects and 20 different objects, it was possible to show, while coding 31.875 s of protocols: that holds of similar objects lasted far longer than holds of different objects (M = 31 vs. 13 s); that attention to the partner’s action was far higher with similar objects in the hands (67 vs. 48%); that holds and discards were mostly simultaneous with similar objects (less than one second discard) while they were unrelated with different objects in the hands; and that the partners take turns with high degrees of symmetry between imitate and be imitated. Indeed, in this experimental condition, a genuine communicative system takes place that shows the three features of any communication format: turn-taking, joint attention, and synchrony (Nadel and Pezé, 1993). Since being imitated acts as a nebulization of oxytocin for affiliative behavior in persons with ASD (Delaveau et al., 2015), we can predict that synchronic imitation will impact on affiliative processes, during joint music-making with similar musical objects.

Moreover, we can assume that sound gestures and perception of sounds develop together from the first months of life and that the execution of the sound gesture provides a unique advantage, compared to pure observation, for the sensitivity to multisensory synchrony (Gerson et al., 2015). On this assumption we can predict that the imitation of sound gestures, early in infancy, may represent an essential source for learning and socio-emotional communicative skills. Inspired by the procedure and results, we have designed musical tools likely to elicit synchronic imitation of musical gestures (Filippa et al., 2020).

To conclude this perspective paper we propose that one research area that is particularly well suited for investigating the development of early imitation mechanisms is music, and, more specifically, the imitation of musical gestures by infants. In this context, the development of the early action–perception system as part of children’s learning and communication strategies, is specifically involved.

Interaction with pairs or with adults through the generation of sounds is also characterized by emotional aspects such as emotional resonance, shared feelings and affiliative processes that can help infants to better understand social interactions. It can also be argued that such shared emotional experiences can train infants’ abilities to use emotion for regulating social interactions.

Finally, we propose that eliciting the exploration of musical gestures in infancy can have a significant impact on early childhood music education, restoring the importance of bodily gestures as the origin of human musicality.

Author Contributions

MF and JN contributed to the conception and design of the manuscript. MF wrote the first draft of the manuscript. MM, SY, DG, and GN wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Addessi, A. R., and Volpe, G. (2011). “The MIROR project,” in Proceedings of the European Conference on Technology Enhanced Learning, (Berlin: Springer), 15–28.

Google Scholar

Aimé, C., Le Covec, M., Bovet, D., and Esseily, R. (2020). La musicalité est-elle un héritage de notre histoire biologique ? Enfance 1, 41–66.

Google Scholar

Ambrosini, E., Reddy, V., De Looper, A., Costantini, M., Lopez, B., and Sinigaglia, C. (2013). Looking ahead: anticipatory gaze and motor ability in infancy. PLoS One 8:e67916. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0067916

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bänziger, T., Grandjean, D., and Scherer, K. R. (2009). Emotion recognition from expressions in face, voice, and body: the Multimodal Emotion Recognition Test (MERT). Emotion 9:691. doi: 10.1037/a0017088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benenzon, R. O. (2007). The benenzon model. Nord. J. Music Ther. 16, 148–159. doi: 10.1080/08098130709478185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buccino, G., Vogt, S., Ritzl, A., Fink, G. R., Zilles, K., Freund, H. J., et al. (2004). Neural circuits underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study. Neuron 42, 323–334. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(04)00181-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., and Trainor, L. J. (2014). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Dev. Sci. 17, 1003–1011. doi: 10.1111/desc.12193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, I., and Woodruff, G. E. (2009). Music as a communicative medium. Prehist. Lang. 1, 113–144.

Google Scholar

Delalande, F. (2015). “Naissance de la Musique. Les Explorations Sonores de la Première Enfance. Rennes: Presses Universitaires de Rennes. Original Edition,” in La Nascita Della Musica: Esplorazioni sonore Nella Prima Infanzia, Ed. F. Delalande (Milano: Franco Angeli).

Google Scholar

Delaveau, P., Arzounian, D., Rotgé, J. Y., Nadel, J., and Fossati, P. (2015). Does imitation act as an oxytocin nebulizer in autism spectrum disorder? Brain 138, e360–e360. doi: 10.1093/brain/awv060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dissanayake, E. (2006). “Ritual and ritualization: musical means of conveying and shaping emotion in humans and other animals,” in Music and Manipulation: On the Social Uses and Social Control of Music, eds S. Brown and U. Voglsten (Oxford: Berghahn Books), 31–56.

Google Scholar

Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., and Garnero, L. (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS One 5:e12166. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Woodson, W., Greenberg, R., and Cohen, C. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science 218, 179–181. doi: 10.1126/science.7123230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Filippa, M. (2015). “Explorations prolongées d’une trouvaille,” in Naissance de la Musique. Les Explorations Sonores de la Première Enfance, ed. F. Delalande (Rennes: Presses Universitaires de Rennes).

Google Scholar

Filippa, M., Cornara, S., Monaci, M. G., Grandjean, D., Nuti, G., and Nadel, J. (2020). L’imitation sonore durant la période préverbale : enjeux théoriques et dispositifs. Enfance 1, 131–148.

Google Scholar

Fitch, W., and Popescu, T. (2019). The world in a song. Science 366, 944–945. doi: 10.1126/science.aay2214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flohr, J. W., and Brown, J. (1979). The influence of peer imitation on expressive movement to music. J. Res. Music Educ. 27, 143–148. doi: 10.2307/3344965

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fogel, A. (1993). “Two principles of communication: co-regulation and framing,” in New Perspectives in Communicative Development, eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (London: Routledge), 9–22. doi: 10.4324/9781315111322-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frühholz, S., Trost, W., and Kotz, S. A. (2016). The sound of emotions—Towards a unifying neural network perspective of affective sound processing. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 68, 96–110. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerson, S. A., Bekkering, H., and Hunnius, S. (2015). Short-term motor training, but not observational training, alters neurocognitive mechanisms of action processing in infancy. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 27, 1207–1214. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Godøy, R. I, and Leman, M. (eds) (2010). Musical Gestures: Sound, Movement, and Meaning. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Hove, M. J., and Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Soc. Cogn. 27, 949–960. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Imberty, M. (2003). Langage, musique et cognition: quelques remarques sur l’évolution nécessaire des problématiques psychologiques des vingt dernières années. Circuit Musiques Contemp. 13, 93–110. doi: 10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, S. S. (2009). The development of imitation in infancy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 364, 2325–2335.

Google Scholar

Juslin, P. N., and Laukka, P. (2004). Expression, perception, and induction of musical emotions: a review and a questionnaire study of everyday listening. J. New Music Res. 33, 217–238. doi: 10.1080/0929821042000317813

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschner, S., and Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evol. Hum. Behav. 31, 354–364. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokal, I., Engel, A., Kirschner, S., and Keysers, C. (2011). Synchronized drumming enhances activity in the caudate and facilitates prosocial commitment-if the rhythm comes easily. PLoS One 6:e27272. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kugiumutzakis, G. (1998). “Neonatal imitation in the intersubjective companion space,” in Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny, ed. S. Bräten (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 63–88.

Google Scholar

Kugiumutzakis, G. (2017). “Intersubjective vocal imitation in early mother-infant interaction,” in New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (Londres: Routledge), 23–47. doi: 10.4324/9781315111322-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kugiumutzakis, G., and Trevarthen, C. (2015). “Neonatal imitation,” in International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd Edn, Vol. 16, ed. J. D. Wright (Oxford: Elsevier), 481–488. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-08-097086-8.23160-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhl, P. K., and Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response to speech: vocal imitation and developmental change. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 100, 2425–2438. doi: 10.1121/1.417951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Labbé, C., and Grandjean, D. (2014). Musical emotions predicted by feelings of entrainment. Music Percept. 32, 170–185. doi: 10.1525/mp.2014.32.2.170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langer, S. K. K. (1957). Philosophy in a New Key: A Study in the Symbolism of Reason, Rite, and Art. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Malloch, S., and Trevarthen, C. (eds) (2009). Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Malloch, S. N. (1999). Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae Sci. 3, 29–57. doi: 10.1177/10298649000030s104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maratos, O. (1998). “Neonatal, early and later imitation: same order phenomena?,” in The Development of Sensory, Motor and Cognitive Capacities in Early Infancy: From Perception to Cognition, eds F. Simion and G. Butterworth (Hove: Psychology Press Ltd), 145–160.

Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N., and Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Dev. 54, 702–709. doi: 10.2307/1130058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzoff, A. N., Murray, L., Simpson, E., Heimann, M., Nagy, E., Nadel, J., et al. (2018). Re-examination of Oostenbroek et al.(2016): evidence for neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion. Dev. Sci. 21:e12609. doi: 10.1111/desc.12609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C. (1985). “Emotional regulation of interactions between 2-month-olds and their mothers,” in Social Perception in Infants, eds T. Field and N. Fox (Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishers), 177–197.

Google Scholar

Nadel, J. (2002). “Imitation and imitation recognition: their social use in healthy infants and children with autism,” in The Imitative Mind: Development, Evolution and Brain Bases, eds A. N. Meltzoff and W. Prinz (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 42–62. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511489969.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadel, J. (2014). How Imitation Boosts Development in Infancy and Autism Spectrum Disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Nadel, J., and Badonnière, P. M. (1882). The social function of reciprocal imitation in 2-year-old peers. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 5, 95–109. doi: 10.1177/016502548200500105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., and Réserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Dev. Sci. 1, 164–174.

Google Scholar

Nadel, J., and Pezé, A. (1993). “What makes immediate imitation communicative in toddlers and autistic children?,” in New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (London, NY: Routledge), 139–156. doi: 10.4324/9781315111322-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nagy, E., Compagne, H., Orvos, H., Pal, A., Molnar, P., Jansky, I., et al. (2005). Index finger movement imitation by human neonates. Pediatr. Res. 8, 749–753. doi: 10.1203/01.PDR.0000180570.28111.D9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nettl, B. (2000). “An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sound and musical culture,” in The Origins of Music, eds N. L. Wallin, B. Merker, and S. Brown (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 463–472.

Google Scholar

Nuti, G., and Filippa, M. (2016). un nido di suoni. Firenze: ed. Polistampa.

Google Scholar

Papoušek, M., and Papoušek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matchingin interactions between mothers and their pre-canonical infants. First Lang. 9, 137–158.

Google Scholar

Patel, A. D., and Iversen, J. R. (2006). “A non-human animal can drum a steady beat on a musical instrument,” in Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Music Perception & Cognition (ICMPC9), eds M. Baroni, A. R. Addessi, R. Caterina, and M. Costa (Bologna: The Society for Music Perception & Cognition and European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music), 477.

Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (2013). Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood. Abingdon: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Repp, B. H., and Su, Y. H. (2013). Sensorimotor synchronization: a review of recent research (2006–2012). Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 20, 403–452. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0371-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schiavio, A., and Timmers, R. (2016). Motor and audiovisual learning consolidate auditory memory of tonally ambiguous melodies. Music Percept. 34, 21–32. doi: 10.1525/mp.2016.34.1.21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sievers, B., Polansky, L., Casey, M., and Wheatley, T. (2013). Music and movement share a dynamic structure that supports universal expressions of emotion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 110, 70–75. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1209023110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, E. A., Murray, L., Paukner, A., and Ferrari, P. F. (2014). The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 369:1644. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soussignan, R., Courtial, A., Canet, P., Danon-Apter, G., and Nadel, J. (2011). Human newborns match tongue protrusion of disembodied human and robotic mouths. Dev. Sci. 14, 385–394. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00984.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tognoli, E., Lagarde, J., DeGuzman, G. C., and Kelso, J. S. (2007). The phi complex as a neuromarker of human social coordination. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci U.S.A. 104, 8190–8195. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0611453104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomic, S. T., and Janata, P. (2008). Beyond the beat: modeling metric structure in music and performance. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 124, 4024–4041. doi: 10.1121/1.3006382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trehub, S. E. (2000). “Human processing predispositions and musical universals,” In The Origins of Music, eds N. L. Wallin and B. Merker, & S. Brown (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 427–448.

Google Scholar

Trehub, S. E., Unyk, A. M., and Trainor, L. J. (1993). Maternal singing in cross-cultural perspective. Infant Behav. Dev. 16, 285–295. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(93)80036-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2002). “Origins of musical identity: evidence from infancy for musical social awareness,” in Musical Identities, (R. A. R. Macdonald, D. J. Hargreaves, and D. Miell), 21–38.

Google Scholar

Uzgiris, I. (1972). “Patterns of vocal and gestural imitation in infants,” in Determinants of Behavioral Development, eds F. J. Monks, W. Hartup, and J. de Witt (New York, NY: Academic Press), 467–471. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-504750-0.50041-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uzgiris, I. (1999). “Imitation as activity: its developmental aspect,” in Imitation in Infancy, eds J. Nadel and G. Butterworth (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 186–206.

Google Scholar

Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., and Rosch, E. (2016). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT press.

Google Scholar

Wiltermuth, S. S., and Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zentner, M., and Eerola, T. (2010). Rhythmic engagement with music in infancy. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 107, 5768–5773. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000121107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: imitation, gesture, action-perception coupling, children, music, sound exploration

Citation: Filippa M, Monaci MG, Young S, Grandjean D, Nuti G and Nadel J (2020) Shall We Play the Same? Pedagogical Perspectives on Infants’ and Children’s Imitation of Musical Gestures. Front. Psychol. 11:1087. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01087

Received: 07 December 2019; Accepted: 28 April 2020;
Published: 03 June 2020.

Edited by:

Luc Nijs, Ghent University, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Robert Hamilton, Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute, United States
Colwyn Trevarthen, University of Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Filippa, Monaci, Young, Grandjean, Nuti and Nadel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Manuela Filippa, manuela.filippa@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.