Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 20 January 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive and Affective Factors in Relation to Learning View all 14 articles

Working Memory and Its Mediating Role on the Relationship of Math Anxiety and Math Performance: A Meta-Analysis

  • 1Department of Applied Educational Science, Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden
  • 2Faculty of Education and Welfare Studies, Åbo Akademi University, Vaasa, Finland

It is well established that math anxiety has a negative relationship with math performance (MP). A few theories have provided explanations for this relationship. One of them, the Attentional Control Theory (ACT), suggests that anxiety can negatively impact the attentional control system and increase one's attention to threat-related stimuli. Within the ACT framework, the math anxiety (MA)—working memory (WM) relationship is argued to be critical for math performance. The present meta-analyses provides insights into the mechanisms of the MA—MP relation and the mediating role of WM. Through database searches with pre-determined search strings, 1,346 unique articles were identified. After excluding non-relevant studies, data from 57 studies and 150 effect sizes were used for investigating the MA—MP correlation using a random-effects model. This resulted in a mean correlation of r = −0.168. The database search of WM as a mediator for the MA—MP relation revealed 15 effects sizes leading to a descriptive rather than a generalizable statistic, with a mean indirect effect size of −0.092. Overall, the results confirm the ACT theory, WM does play a significant role in the MA—MP relationship.

Introduction

It is well established that there is a negative relationship between math anxiety (MA) and math performance (MP; Namkung et al., 2019). There are a few theories that explain how MA affects MP. One that has gained a steady foothold in the literature is the Attentional Control Theory (ACT; Eysenck et al., 2007), which stipulates that anxiety can deplete cognitive resources, which is vital for computing math-related problems. Unexpectedly, there are no systematic literature studies that have explicitly looked at (I) the relationship between MA and WM, and (II) the mediating effect of working memory (WM) on the relationship of MA predicting MP, despite the ACT being the dominant theory. In line with the ACT, the literature points out that MA can deprive WM resources that are needed for complex math computation (see Ashcraft and Krause, 2007 for an overview). This systematic literature study and meta-analysis address these research gaps by synthesizing research that has studied the MA—WM link and WM's mediating effect on the MA—MP relationship.

Math Anxiety, Working Memory and Performance

MA, which has been of concern for social science researchers since at least the 1950's (Dreger and Aiken, 1957), is commonly defined as feelings of fear, apprehension and tension that interfere when performing math-related activities (Ashcraft, 2002). Math is the single most strenuous subject in the school curriculum which can cause emotions comparable to phobia (Ashcraft and Ridley, 2005), and has been shown to correlate with other types of anxieties, such as test anxiety (Kazelskis et al., 2000; Ashcraft, 2002). Although the two constructs have in some cases shown to correlate strongly with each other, it has been possible to distinguish one from the other through confirmatory factor analysis (Kazelskis et al., 2000).

Ramirez et al. (2018) suggested three potential explanations for MA. The first is framed as the deficit theory in which poor math skills explain MA. Ma and Xu (2004) conducted a longitudinal study and found that lower math achievement predicted higher MA. The second is framed as a genetic predisposition. Wang et al. (2014) studied a sample of 514 twin-siblings (m = 12.25 years), and their results suggested that ~40% of the variation in MA is due to genetic predispositions, the variation left is caused by environmental factors specific to the child. The third is framed as socio-environmental factors, arguing that children in the lower grades can inherit some of their parents MA, though only if their MA parents reported to frequently support their child (Maloney et al., 2015). On the other hand, Vukovic et al. (2013b) reported that parental involvement in their child's learning significantly reduced MA, which led to an increase in MP (the phenomena was observed in algebraic and word problems, not whole arithmetic problems).

Ashcraft and Moore (2009) propose a risk-factor model of MA which consists of (I) deficits in MP (deficit theory), (II) lack of motivation and (III) weak WM. WM becomes a more relevant factor once the child faces more complex math than single digit arithmetical tasks. Although the developmental aspect of MA is still in need of more research, especially in younger participants to better understand the onset and progression of MA, some studies have found early indications of MA. For example, Mononen et al. (2021) found that MA growth was negatively related to growth in MP in participants as young as 6-year-olds. Another perspective is to what extent MA can arise as soon as formal school is introduced (Maloney and Beilock, 2012). Ashcraft and Moore's (Ashcraft and Moore, 2009) review suggests that MA strengthens in middle school, and peaks around grade 9 or 10.

Concerning the MA—MP link, two diametrically opposite models have been researched in attempts to identify the causality between the two variables (Carey et al., 2016). (I) The Deficit Theory, assuming that poor MP causes MA, was supported in Passolunghi's (2011) study, where children with math learning disabilities exhibited higher MA. In a longitudinal study of Finnish students in grade 3–5, Sorvo et al. (2019) found that arithmetic achievement predicted MA one year later, MA on the other hand, didn't predict later math achievement. The opposing theory, (II) the Debilitating Anxiety Model implies that MA leads to poorer MP. These models combined suggest a reciprocal model (Carey et al., 2016). Other longitudinal studies have provided evidence of reciprocal effects, (Ma and Xu, 2004; Gunderson et al., 2018). Although the relationship between MA and MP may be of a reciprocal nature, in the current study we approached this relationship from the Debilitating Anxiety model by adopting the ACT. Further research is necessary to better understand the MA—MP relation, potentially by addressing influences of individual differences. For instance, Chang and Beilock (2016) discuss the reciprocal relationship between MA and MP and whether individual differences can explain the MA—MP relationship, such as (a) variations in individual cognitive, affective, and motivational factors and (b) environmental factors that consist of teachers and parental MA, and student's perceived classroom environment. In the present study, we focus on WM, a subconstruct of cognition. We use our WM to retrieve the information needed to solve math tasks, keep relevant information about the salient problem, and inhibit irrelevant information.

According to Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) definition of WM, the construct comprises an attentional control system (the central executive), a modality-free processor able to monitor, plan, manipulate information, and select strategies to complete the tasks at hand. This control system is accompanied by two sub-systems: the visuospatial sketchpad and the phonological loop. The WM model has proven to be long-lasting and is referenced in a wide range of research areas (Baddeley, 2010). An additional component was subsequently proposed, namely the episodic buffer, which supposedly is a passive, multimodal storage system that integrates with the subsidiary systems as well as the long-term memory in holding episodic information (Baddeley, 2000). Working memory has consistently been shown to predict math performance (Swanson and Kim, 2007; De Smedt et al., 2009; Wiklund-Hörnqvist et al., 2016). If the math task induces anxiety, the ability of our working memory to maintain information online, and store and retrieve information from long-term memory will be reduced (Ashcraft and Kirk, 2001; Ramirez et al., 2013). Hence, the cognitive processing associated with the to-be-solved mathematical task in combination with math anxiety can overload the working memory system which, consequently, will reduce one's MP. This fits well with the ACT proposition, though one important detail that is vital to the ACT is that anxiety affects processing efficiency (effort spent on a task in relation to performance effectiveness) to a further extent than performance effectiveness (quality of performance; Eysenck et al., 2007). According to the ACT, anxiety impairs attentional processes relevant to the WM, and thereafter redistributing attentional resources on either internal or external stimuli. Internal consisting of worrying thoughts, external of irrelevant distractors threatening to the on-going task.

When reviewing the literature of the MA—WM relationship, the findings varied considerably. Whereas some studies found the MA—WM correlation to be 0.079 and 0.081, respectively (Ching, 2017; Pappas et al., 2019), others found the correlation to be −0.43 and −0.4, respectively (Witt, 2012; Soltanlou et al., 2019). Moreover, in Ashcraft and Kirk's (2001) experiments, the authors found that their subjects WM-scores significantly declined as MA increased, though only in WM-tests containing numerical information, not in language-based tests (rletter−span = −0.2; rcomputation−span = −0.4). This is certainly interesting in respect of the utilization of measurement instruments for analyzing the MA—WM link. These differences in effect sizes may also vary as a function of age.

There have been gender differences reported, though to what degree gender can moderate relationships of MA—MP or MA—WM is unclear. Females have displayed higher MA than their male counterparts (Hembree, 1990; Hopko, 2003). In longitudinal studies, Geary et al. (2019) found that MP predicted future MA in females but not males, and in contrast Ma and Xu (2004) found that MP predicted higher MA in males compared to females. Regarding WM, there have been reports of males scoring better at visuospatial measures but not verbal (Robert and Savoie, 2006). Further, Maloney et al. (2012) found that spatial ability mediated the gender—MA relationship.

Previous Meta-Analyses

Hembree (1990) conducted one of the first, if not the first, meta-analysis on the correlation between MA and MP (high school students r = −0.34; college students r = −0.31). Later, Ma (1999) synthesized correlations from 26 studies focusing on samples from elementary and high schools (r = −0.27). More recently, Namkung et al. (2019), Zhang et al. (2019) and Barroso et al. (2021) pooled correlations of MA and math performance from 223, 131 and 49 studies and found moderate negative correlations (r = −0.28; r = −0.34; r = −0.3). Altogether research has established a robust negative relationship between MA and MP. This is especially of concern as Fan et al. (2019) found, through latent profile analysis on PISA-data, that 22% of US and Korean students and 10% of Finnish students belonged to a high MA profile. Peng et al. (2016) conducted a meta-analysis on the WM—MP relationship from 110 studies and found a moderate correlation (r = 0.35). However, the correlation for typically math performing participants was r = 0.34, while participants with math difficulties and other cognitive disorders displayed a correlation of r = 0.52. Regarding the WM domains, the use of composite measures of WM had a larger correlation with MP (r = 0.38) than isolated WM-domains alone (verbal WM: r = 30; visuospatial WM: r = 0.31), most likely because these tasks include computational requirements which are predictive of MP, both in the visuospatial and the phonological domains (Swanson and Kim, 2007). Moreover Liang et al. (2021) showed that children in the first grade depended more on visuospatial ability than verbal WM, while fifth graders relied on both WM domains in MP situations. These results highlight the progressive and domain specific aspects of WM's influence on MP.

The Current Study

Though many studies have researched the MA—MP relation, there are still gaps that need to be investigated to better understand the relationship between the two variables. Following the tenets of the ACT, the available executive function resources are depleted as a consequence of MA, and fewer resources are left to the designated task. Lowered WM capacity and diminished executive control following MA will affect performance. For anxious individuals, a worrying stimulus acts as a distractor by reducing the cognitive resources required for successful performance. Conversely, higher WM capacity can act as a protective factor. Although these theories have support from empirical studies, there would be a lot to gain from synthesizing the existing research in order to present a mean correlation. With the ACT setting as the basic framework, we posed two research questions (RQ), assessing the strength of the association between MA and WM and potential mediating effect of WM: (RQ1) what is the mean correlation between MA and WM? (RQ2) what is the mean indirect effect between MA and MP while accounting for WM as a mediator?

Methods

Literature Search

The following databases were employed for our search task: Web of Science, Google Scholar, APAPsycNet, Scopus, ProQuest, and the meta-database EBSCO(host)1. No restrictions were applied on the dates for when the research-articles were published (earliest possible date until 25th October 2020). A search string was developed by looking up synonyms with various thesauruses. After running pilot-searches the final search-string resulted in the following:

(“math* achievement” OR “math* performance” OR “math* success” OR “math* score” OR “arithmet*” OR “calculation” OR “math* ability”)

AND

(“math* anxiety” OR “math* worry”)

AND

(“working memory” OR “short term memory” OR “spatial” OR “phonological loop” OR “memory span” OR “digit span” OR “cognit*”)

The same search string was used in all the aforementioned databases except for Google Scholar as the character limit was exceeded2. The search resulted in 1,901 articles which were imported to the reference management tool EndNote x9. Hand-searched references, which gained an additional 8 articles to the collection, were also imported to EndNote. The initial search accumulated 1,909 articles. After duplicates were removed 1,346 unique articles were reviewed on a title and abstract level.

Study Criteria

After an initial screening of abstracts, the articles were assessed in full-text. To be included in the full-text analysis, the article had to be peer-reviewed, published, written in English and comprise all three variables of interest: MA, WM, and MP. Further, the results had to include either (or both) a correlation between MA and WM, or a mediation analysis where WM was set to mediate the MA—MP relationship. See Figure 1 for a flowchart of the literature search procedure.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart of literature search. Based on PRISMA from Moher et al. (2009).

MA is usually conceptualized as two-dimensional, having one affective and one cognitive component (Ho et al., 2000). Regarding MA and the affective dimension, instruments such as MARS (Richardson and Suinn, 1972), different varieties of the MARS, and the AMAS (Hopko et al., 2003) were included in the study. These measurements are designed to capture feelings of nervousness when facing mathematically challenging situations. Regarding MA and the cognitive worry dimension, only the Faces Pain Scale instruments (Bieri et al., 1990) have been used in studies of MA (e.g., Trezise and Reeve, 2014a,b). Other measures of anxieties, such as test anxiety, were not considered to meet the MA-criteria and were consequently excluded from the literature review.

Following the Baddeley and Hitch (1974) WM model, the study included measures of the central executive (e.g., operation span), and the sub-systems: phonological loop (e.g., digit span) and visuospatial sketch pad (e.g., Corsi block span). See Table 1 for examples of these measures. Moreover, by extension a categorization into one of the four categories: (1) visuospatial, (2) phonological, (3) central executive + phonological, or (4) central executive + visuospatial was conducted. To qualify into categories one and two, the test must measure verbal or visual memory without applying strains on the central executive. To qualify for categories three or four, the test should require the participants to utilize either spatial or phonological abilities, and simultaneously manipulate information or steer their attention to a second, concurrent task.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Examples of the most common instruments measuring working memory.

MP was essentially measured in every article that measured MA and WM, these tests differed somewhat. The majority of the studies used standardized math tests, such as the quantitative reasoning ability from the Woodcock-Johnson III-battery (e.g., Miller and Bichsel, 2004). Other tests were researcher's self-designed math test (e.g., Novak and Tassell, 2017) or ordinary class exams (e.g., Alamolhodaei, 2009). Working memory has been shown to predict a broad range of math outcomes, even when other cognitive factors are controlled for (see Raghubar et al., 2010 for an overview), However task differences have been observed. When comparing types of math skill with WM, Peng et al.'s (2016) meta-analysis revealed that word-problem solving and whole-number calculations correlated the strongest with WM (r = 0.37; r = 0.35), while geometry differed significantly from the aforementioned, displaying a weaker correlation with WM (r = 0.23).

Coding and Interrater Reliability

To measure the reliability of the screening process, the first author read all abstracts and the second author read 40% of the abstracts, this allowed us to calculate a reliability statistic. The Cohen's kappa, an interrater reliability measure, was calculated with the following formula k = (PoPe)/(1−Pe) and resulted in an adequate reliability k = 0.83 (Cooper et al., 2019). Any disagreements were revisited and discussed by both authors until consensus was achieved. From the abstract screening process, 1,249 articles were excluded leaving 97 articles eligible for full-text assessment.

The first and second author assessed all 97 articles in full text, if articles didn't present relevant results they were excluded. The reliability coefficient for the second comparison was k = 0.79. Similar to the first comparison, both authors revisited the articles that were subject for disagreements, in order to discuss and resolve any ambiguities. The first and second authors extracted and independently double coded all of the study's variables, this was in accordance with Cooper et al.'s (2019) recommendations. The study information included variables, such as sample size, sample characteristics, effect sizes, measurement instruments and country, which were all coded into an Excel workbook. Sample size was used to calculate study weights and variances (within study variance) for the effect sizes. Sample size was also needed to calculate the tau-squared (τ2), which functioned as a between study variance-measure (Borenstein et al., 2009). All the coded information was compared between the two authors, and any inconsistencies were revisited and discussed. If an inconsistency remained uncertain it was brought up under meetings and assessed by all five authors.

Analysis

The correlation measure Pearson's r for MA and WM was used for the meta-analysis in RQ1. The correlations were transformed into Fisher's 𝓏 effect sizes and variances. As for studies that reported multiple effect sizes based on the same sample, a mean effect size and variance was calculated for the dependent effect sizes. The dependent effects were correlated and estimated with the robust variance estimation (RVE) method with the R-package Robumeta (Fisher and Tipton, 2015). A problem with treating dependent effect sizes that are positively correlated with each other as independent effects, is that the analysis can overestimate the precision and underestimate the error of the mean effect (Borenstein et al., 2009). With this in mind, a mean effect size for dependent effects was computed. The variance for each mean effect size considered the within-study correlation between the outcomes (ρ). Depending on how ρ is specified it can affect the estimation of the between-study variance (τ2), the mean error and the actual effect size. The ρ was specified to 0.8 as it was expected that the effect sizes would be correlated with each other. A sensitivity analysis revealed that the analysis was robust over the whole range of estimates of ρ (0–1).

Sub-group analyses (on age, school level, WM domain, verbal- and numerical tests) were performed with the Meta-package in R (Schwarzer, 2007) in accordance with Harrer et al.'s (2021) guide. In the sub-group analysis, moderator variables with multiple categories were transformed into dummy variables, dichotomous variables didn't require transformation. In the sub-group analysis, each effect size was treated as an independent effect.

Regarding the second aim for this study, whether working memory mediates the relationship between math anxiety and math performance, standardized regression coefficients from such models were extracted if reported. In some cases, only unstandardized coefficients were reported. We then used reported descriptive information of means and standard deviations to transform the statistics into standardized coefficients. The main analysis was to compute a summary effect with confidence intervals of the indirect effect, which was calculated by multiplying path a by path b (see Figure 2). Some studies answering RQ2 reported multiple effect sizes. As in RQ1, the RVE method was employed to balance the weights of the studies containing multiple effects. ρ was specified to 0.8. A sensitivity analysis showed that the estimated mean effect and the τ2 was slightly affected over the span of 0–1, though within reasonable limits.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediation model.

The RVE used employed a random-effects model. The rationales for using random-effects model rather than fixed-effect model for answering RQ1 were: (I) the samples originated from different populations as samples varied by age, country, and other factors. (II) A test of heterogeneity was significant (p < 0.001) and the I2-index (I2 = total heterogeneity/total observed variability), a statistic that can offer an indication of the degree of heterogeneity, resulted in 75.6% heterogeneity, which can be considered a high value (Higgins et al., 2003). Regarding the analysis for RQ2, there was evidence of some heterogeneity (I2 = 47 %) in addition to the varied samples. This also suggested that a random-effects model would be appropriate.

Potential bias in our data was analyzed by fitting a funnel plot (Figure 4) to the data for subsequent visual inspection. Egger's regression test was carried out for assessing asymmetry in the funnel plot. The inverse of the standard errors was applied to the vertical axis as it's usually the recommended practice for bias-detection in meta-analysis (Sterne and Egger, 2001). This brings studies with larger sample sizes to the top of the funnel and smaller closer to the bottom.

Sub-Group Analysis

The fairly high level of heterogeneity gave reason to investigate whether certain variables affected the MA—WM relationship. The sub-groups that were chosen were based on sample characteristics and the large amount and diversity of the WM measures. These consisted of age, school-level, WM-domain, and type of WM-test. Age was categorized into participants (I) <12 years, (II) ≥12 years, (III) samples consisting of participants around the age 12. The decision for our cut-off value of 12-years for the age-variable, was based partly on earlier research that has found MA to increase in the early teens (Ashcraft and Moore, 2009), and partly on a related earlier meta-analysis' age grouping (Caviola et al., 2017). School level consisted of (I) primary school, (II) high school, (III) university. WM-domains consisted of: (I) visuospatial, (II) phonological, (III) visuospatial + central executive and (IV) phonological + central executive. WM-tests were divided into (I) verbally based and (II) tests without verbal elements. WM-tests were also divided based on their numerical attributes into either (I) numerical tests, or (II) non-numerical tests.

Results

Mean Correlation Working Memory—Math Anxiety

To answer RQ1 effect sizes were pooled from a total of 57 articles, 66 unique samples comprising of 16,589 participants and 150 correlation coefficients. The mean correlation between MA and WM was −0.168 with confidence intervals on the 95% level ranging from −0.203 to −0.133 (see Table 2). The sub-group analysis of age, school level, and numerical based WM-tests were all significant, p < 0.001, p < 0.01, and p < 0.05, respectively.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean correlation and sub-group analysis on the MA—WM relationship.

Participant Age

Age was one of the clearest moderating variables. Participants of under the age of 12 had a weak MA—WM correlation (r = −0.101, 95 % C.I. [−0.148; −0.053]). Participants around the age of 12 (samples that ranged from under 12 to over 12), had a slightly stronger MA—WM relationship (r = −0.126, 95 % C.I. [−0.161; −0.090]). Participants over the age of 12 exhibited the strongest MA—WM relationship (r = −0.219, 95 % C.I. [−0.251; −0.188]) which was significantly stronger than the two other age groups (p < 0.001). A meta-regression analysis was carried out with participant mean age as the predictor of the relationship. This resulted in a small, significant effect (β = −0.005, std. error = 0.002, p < 0.01). Further, the MA—WM correlation increased in strength from primary to high-school (r = −0.113; r = −0.205). The correlation didn't significantly differ from high school to university (r = −0.205; r = −0.202).

Regarding the gender aspect, there was not enough available data for investigating differences between males and females (number of effect sizes: males = 2; females = 12) in the MA—WM relation. This would be of interest for future research as previous research has shown that females experience higher levels of MA compared to males (Hembree, 1990).

Working Memory Category and Tests

A between-group analysis revealed a marginal main effect of WM categories (p = 0.055). Descriptively, this effect can be seen in Table 2, represented by almost non-overlapping confidence intervals. Hence, the phonological (r = −0.125, 95 % C.I. [−0.162; −0.087]) and phonological + central executive domain's (r = −0.205, 95 % C.I. [−0.247; −0.161]) confidence intervals (95%) overlapped with only one thousandths decimal point. A meta-regression analysis revealed that phonological domain predicted the MA—WM correlation (β = 0.064, std. error = 0.036, p < 0.05). No other WM category revealed significant effects, neither sub-group analysis nor meta-regression analysis. The analysis of verbal WM tests revealed no main effect while the analysis of numerical tests revealed a main effect (p < 0.05), with a stronger MA—WM correlation for numerical tests (r = −0.212) compared to non-numerical tests (r = −0.152). See Table 2 for details.

Mean Correlation of Indirect Effect

As for RQ2, 10 studies reportedly measured the mediation model of interest (see Figure 2). However, one study didn't report the necessary statistics for the analysis, as their aim was more focused on the gender aspect, though they did mention that MA failed to show an indirect or a direct effect on MP in a model accounting for WM, other parallel mediators, and covariates. A second study was dropped because the reported statistics were out of proportion and couldn't be transformed into standardized values. Though in the same study, the authors reported a significant indirect effect of MA predicting MP while accounting for WM. A sample size of 1,824 participants from eight studies with a total of 15 effect sizes of the indirect effects of MA predicting MP while accounting for WM as a mediator, were synthesized with the RVE method and resulted in a significant negative indirect effect (r = −0.092, p < 0.05) between MA predicting MP while accounting for WM. See Table 3 for details.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean correlation of the indirect effect.

Funnel Plot Analysis

A funnel plot with study effect sizes for RQ1 was analyzed (see Figure 3). Visual inspection of the effect sizes in relation to the funnel suggested the data wasn't normally distributed as effect sizes occurred outside the funnel. Egger's regression test for asymmetry was significant (p < 0.001) confirming the asymmetric data. It's possible that the data suffers from small-study effects as stronger effects are seen in smaller studies with larger standard errors (Rücker et al., 2011). A bias-corrected analysis in the form of a trim-and-fill method can function as a sensitivity analysis (Peters et al., 2007). Adding 27 effect sizes, to mirror the extremes, into the funnel plot (n = 173) lowered the average correlation to −0.136, while remaining significant (95 % C.I. −0.164 to −0.108, p < 0.001). A meta-regression was performed on publication year which suggested that the effect slightly decreases with more recent publications (β = 0.008, SE = 0.003, p < 0.001).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Funnel plot of the math anxiety—working memory effects.

Discussion

This meta-analysis investigated the relationship between MA and WM, and the mediating effect of WM on the relationship of MA predicting MP. The results based on 66 unique samples showed that MA had a significant negative correlation with WM (r = −0.168), which according to Hemphill (2003), is interpreted as a small effect size. This relationship varied significantly as a function of age, school level, WM category and whether WM tests were numerically based or not. Furthermore, a significant indirect effect (−0.092) of MA on MP through WM was found based on eight unique samples. There has been plenty of research reporting the MA—WM statistic, though these effect sizes have varied from study to study. A synthesized correlation of MA—WM has been reported in Namkung et al. (2019) which contrasts our study as they found a non-significant correlation between the two variables MA and WM (r = −0.08), though their study-focus was on the MA—MP correlation. The current study presents a mean correlation on the MA—WM link, based on synthesized effect sizes from published, peer reviewed research.

Participant Characteristics

There was a clear age effect in the MA—WM relationship, as older participants displayed a stronger negative MA—WM relationship. This was also mirrored in the school-level analyses. From primary school to high school the MA—WM relationship grew stronger, leveling off in high school and remained static throughout university. These results are in line with Ashcraft and Moore's (Ashcraft and Moore, 2009) suggestion that MA peaks around grade 10, and levels off shortly after. Moreover, the age-effect in the MA—WM correlation in the present study and the MA—MP correlation in Zhang et al.'s (2019) meta-analysis is up to high school level very similar. With the exception that University students displayed a lower MA—MP correlation than high school students, in their study.

Working Memory

The WM measures selected were based on Baddeley and Hitch's (1974) model and thus were categorized into phonological, visuospatial or combinations of the central executive and the phonological or visuospatial sub-systems. Between-group analysis, between phonological—MA and phonological + centra executive—MA, revealed a marginal significant main effect (p = 0.055; Table 2), as indicated by almost separated confidence intervals (95%). This in combination with the meta-regression analysis showing that the phonological domain predicted the relationship of MA—WM, indicate that the phonological domain might differ from the rest. The phonological + central executive measures correlated more strongly with MA (r = −0.205) compared to tests measuring solely the phonological loop (r = −0.125). These results are interesting from the perspective of the ACT, which theorizes that anxiety functions as a distractor that depletes cognitive resources required for cognitively demanding problems. Just like its predecessor, the processing efficiency theory (Eysenck and Calvo, 1992), the ACT emphasizes that anxiety has a greater impact on the executive component of working memory. The interpretation of our results can to some extent support the idea, that anxiety affects short term memory storage and attentional processes to a greater extent compared to short term memory storage alone. This is however not applied to our results of the visuospatial domain as most of the C.I. in visuospatial and visuospatial + central executive overlapped, indicating that the correlation was fairly similar.

Working Memory as a Mediator

The debilitating anxiety model suggests that anxiety can cause deficiencies in math performance. Deficits in MP caused by MA can fully or at least partly be mediated by cognitive processes. Indeed, our results showing a significant negative indirect effect of MA on MP via WM, support the ACT. When one experiences worrying thoughts, working memory resources are spent on irrelevant stimuli, thus, limiting the processing capacity required for performing the math problems at hand. Regarding RQ1 our sub-group analysis on numerical-based WM-tests supports Ashcraft and Kirk's (2001) findings that MA correlates more strongly with WM if the WM-measures are numerical. This implies that numbers per se can trigger anxiety to large extent and thus deplete cognitive processing resources. But why is higher MA significantly associated with lower WM, when looking at non-numerical WM measures? An explanation could potentially be found in the lack of construct independence. Although MA is defined as a feeling of nervousness in a math-related context (Ashcraft, 2002), its relative high correlation with test anxiety (Kazelskis et al., 2000), indicates associations to other forms of anxieties. Indeed, studies have found that MA relates moderately to strongly with state anxiety, (r = 0.30, Hopko, 2003), general anxiety (r = 0.5, Llabre and Suarez, 1985) and test anxiety (r = 0.687, O'Leary et al., 2017).

Limitation

The results from the funnel plot (Figure 4) with some studies outside the funnel in combination with Egger's regression test for asymmetry and the trim-and-fill method that lowered the average correlation indicated a publication bias. Especially as the current study only collected peer-reviewed, published studies, which can be seen as a limitation as some research (e.g., theses) will be left out. However, the effect sizes collected for our meta-analysis were seldom the main aim in the included literature. These effect sizes were most often found in sections where descriptive information or background variables were presented, typically not being an object of publication bias- providing a counterargument for publication bias. Moreover, a meta-regression revealed that publication date predicted the MA—WM relationship, which partly could explain the asymmetry in the funnel plot. With respect to the heterogeneity that characterizes the data (Higgins et al., 2003) we opted for implementing a random-effects model for RQ1. For instance, the data involves a vast and diverse measurement for both MA and WM, and the results show evidence of how the relationship increases in strength as the population grows older (Table 2). Imputing new data for bias-reasons may not be the answer in this case. As the τ2 was significant, it is not always good practice to correct for this type of bias as the chances of unreliable results increase (Peters et al., 2007).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Funnel plot of indirect effects.

Regarding RQ2, an evident problem had to be addressed, namely the small number of studies answering the research question. This challenged the external validity and is considered a limitation. The reader should regard the results for RQ2 as descriptive and not prescriptive.

Conclusion

This study presents a robust significant negative relationship between MA—WM confirming the relevance of the ACT. A significant indirect effect from MA predicting MP while accounting for WM is also shown, though this relationship should be interpreted with caution as there was a limited number of studies answering this question. Researchers should be aware of what type of WM-measure is used and whether the instrument is of numerical nature or not, which could impact on the MA participants. Further, our results suggest that WM-measures of both the phonological and executive components have a stronger association with MA than phonological storage processes alone, which also is stated in the ACT. We recognize that there is still a shortage of data for determining the precision and certainty of the indirect effect that MA has on MP. This question still requires further investigation. Lastly, there is the age aspect to the MA—WM relationship. Our results indicate that somewhere between primary and high school the relationship develops in strength and levels off in university. This protracted development may be connected to younger children experiencing less MA than older (Wigfield and Meece, 1988; Ashcraft and Moore, 2009). If that is the case, the findings of this study highlight the importance of early interventions to suppress anxieties that can have detrimental effects on math performance and be pertinent to general cognitive processing abilities.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

JK and BJ came up with the idea for the study. JF, ES, JK, HE, and BJ jointly contributed to the study's conceptualization and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content. JF performed the statistical analysis and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the manuscript and read and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Funding was received from the Swedish Research Council, VR, Grant (2019–03928).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^EBSCO(host) included the following databases: Academic Search Premier, APA Psycinfo, Business Source Premier, CINAHL, ERIC, MEDLINE, SocINDEX and SPORTDiscus.

2. ^The following were excluded in Google Scholar: “calculation,” “math* ability,” “memory span” and “digit span”.

References

*Alamolhodaei, H. (2009). A working memory model applied to mathematical word problem solving. Asia Pacific Educ. Rev. 10, 183–192. doi: 10.1007/s12564-009-9023-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Alikamar, M. A., Alamolhodaei, H., and Radmehr, F. (2013). The role of metacognition on effect of working memory capacity on students' mathematical problem solving. Eur. J. Child Dev. Educ. Psychopathol. 1, 125–139. doi: 10.30552/ejpad.v1i3.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Allen, M., and Vallee-Tourangeau, F. (2016). Interactivity defuses the impact of mathematics anxiety in primary school children. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 14, 1553–1566. doi: 10.1007/s10763-015-9659-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H. (2002). Math anxiety: personal, educational, and cognitive consequences. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 11, 181–185. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ashcraft, M. H., and Kirk, E. P. (2001). The relationships among working memory, math anxiety, and performance. J. Exp. Psychol. 130:224. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.2.224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Krause, J. A. (2007). Working memory, math performance, and math anxiety. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 14, 243–248. doi: 10.3758/BF03194059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Moore, A. M. (2009). Mathematics anxiety and the affective drop in performance. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 27, 197–205. doi: 10.1177/0734282908330580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ashcraft, M. H., and Ridley, K. S. (2005). “Math anxiety and its cognitive consequences,” in Handbook of Mathematical Cognition, ed J. I. Campbell (New York, NY: Psychology Press), 315–327.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

*Ashkenazi, S., and Danan, Y. (2017). The role of mathematical anxiety and working memory on the performance of different types of arithmetic tasks. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 7, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2017.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2000). The episodic buffer: a new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci. 4, 417–423. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01538-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (2010). Working memory. Curr. Biol. 20, R136–R140. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. D., and Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 8, 47–89. doi: 10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Barroso, C., Ganley, C. M., Hart, S. A., Rogers, N., and Clendinning, J. P. (2019). The relative importance of math- and music-related cognitive and affective factors in predicting undergraduate music theory achievement. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 771–783. doi: 10.1002/acp.3518

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barroso, C., Ganley, C. M., McGraw, A. L., Geer, E. A., Hart, S. A., and Daucourt, M. C. (2021). A meta-analysis of the relation between math anxiety and math achievement. Psychol. Bull. 147:134. doi: 10.1037/bul0000307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bieri, D., Reeve, R. A., Champion, G. D., Addicoat, L., and Ziegler, J. B. (1990). The faces pain scale for the self-assessment of the severity of pain experienced by children: development, initial validation, and preliminary investigation for ratio scale properties. Pain 41, 139–150. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(90)90018-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P., and Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to Meta-Analysis. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1002/9780470743386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Braham, E. J., and Libertus, M. E. (2018). When approximate number acuity predicts math performance: the moderating role of math anxiety. PLoS ONE 13:e0195696. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Buelow, M. T., and Frakey, L. L. (2013). Math anxiety differentially affects WAIS-IV arithmetic performance in undergraduates. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 28, 356–362. doi: 10.1093/arclin/act006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carey, E., Hill, F., Devine, A., and Szücs, D. (2016). The chicken or the egg? The direction of the relationship between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance. Front. Psychol. 6:1987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Cargnelutti, E., Tomasetto, C., and Passolunghi, M. C. (2017). The interplay between affective and cognitive factors in shaping early proficiency in mathematics. Trends Neurosci. Educ. 8–9, 28–36. doi: 10.1016/j.tine.2017.10.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Casey, M. B., Nuttall, R. L., and Pezaris, E. (1997). Mediators of gender differences in mathematics college entrance test scores: a comparison of spatial skills with internalized beliefs and anxieties. Dev. Psychol. 33, 669–680. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.33.4.669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caviola, S., Carey, E., Mammarella, I. C., and Szucs, D. (2017). Stress, time pressure, strategy selection and math anxiety in mathematics: a review of the literature. Front. Psychol. 8:1488. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, H., and Beilock, S. L. (2016). The math anxiety-math performance link and its relation to individual and environmental factors: a review of current behavioral and psychophysiological research. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10, 33–38. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*and Ching, B. H.-H. (2017). Mathematics anxiety and working memory: longitudinal associations with mathematical performance in Chinese children. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 51, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, H., Hedges, L. V., and Valentine, J. C. (2019). The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-Analysis. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. doi: 10.7758/9781610448864

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Daneshamooz, S., Radmehr, F., Alamolhodaei, H., and Mohajer, M. (2011). The effects of sleep duration on predictive factors of mathematical performance. ARPN J. Sci. Technol. 2, 283–291.

Google Scholar

De Smedt, B., Janssen, R., Bouwens, K., Verschaffel, L., Boets, B., and Ghesquière, P. (2009). Working memory and individual differences in mathematics achievement: a longitudinal study from first grade to second grade. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 103, 186–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Delgado, A. R., and Prieto, G. (2008). Stereotype threat as validity threat: the anxiety-sex-threat interaction. Intelligence 36, 635–640. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Douglas, H. P., and LeFevre, J. A. (2017). Exploring the influence of basic cognitive skills on the relation between math performance and math anxiety. J. Numerical Cogn. 3, 642–666. doi: 10.5964/jnc.v3i3.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreger, R. M., and Aiken, L. R. Jr (1957). The identification of number anxiety in a college population. J. Educ. Psychol. 48:344. doi: 10.1037/h0045894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., and Calvo, M. G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: the processing efficiency theory. Cogn. Emot. 6, 409–434. doi: 10.1080/02699939208409696

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, M. W., Derakshan, N., Santos, R., and Calvo, M. G. (2007). Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion 7:336. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, X., Hambleton, R. K., and Zhang, M. (2019). Profiles of mathematics anxiety among 15-year-old students: a cross-cultural study using multi-group latent profile analysis. Front. Psychol. 10:1217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ferguson, A. M., Maloney, E. A., Fugelsang, J., and Risko, E. F. (2015). On the relation between math and spatial ability: the case of math anxiety. Learn. Individ. Dif. 39, 1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2015.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, Z., and Tipton, E. (2015). Robumeta: An R-package for robust variance estimation in meta-analysis. arxiv [preprint].arxiv:1503.02220.

Google Scholar

*Ganley, C. M., and Vasilyeva, M. (2014). The role of anxiety and working memory in gender differences in mathematics. J. Educ. Psychol. 106:105. doi: 10.1037/a0034099

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., Chu, F., Scofield, J. E., and Ferguson Hibbard, D. (2019). Sex differences in mathematics anxiety and attitudes: concurrent and longitudinal relations to mathematical competence. J. Educ. Psychol. 111, 1447–1461. doi: 10.1037/edu0000355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., and Scofield, J. E. (2020). In-class attention, spatial ability, and mathematics anxiety predict across-grade gains in adolescents' mathematics achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. doi: 10.1037/edu0000487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Georges, C., Hoffmann, D., and Schiltz, C. (2016). How math anxiety relates to number-space associations. Front. Psychol. 7:1401. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunderson, E. A., Park, D., Maloney, E. A., Beilock, S. L., and Levine, S. C. (2018). Reciprocal relations among motivational frameworks, math anxiety, and math achievement in early elementary school. J. Cogn. Dev. 19, 21–46. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2017.1421538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Guthrie, L. G., and Vallee-Tourangeau, F. (2018). Numbers in action: individual differences and interactivity in mental arithmetic. Cogn. Process. 19, 317–326. doi: 10.1007/s10339-018-0856-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrer, M., Cuijpers, P., Furukawa, T. A., and Ebert, D. D. (2021). Doing Meta-Analysis With R: A Hands-On Guide. Boca Raton, FL; London: Chapmann; Hall/CRC Press. doi: 10.1201/9781003107347

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hart, S., Daucourt, M., and Ganley, C. (2017). Individual differences related to college students' course performance in calculus II. J. Learn. Anal. 4, 129–153-129–153. doi: 10.18608/jla.2017.42.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Hartwright, C. E., Chung Yen, L., Sella, F., Inuggi, A., Santos, F. H., González-Salinas, C., et al. (2018). The neurocognitive architecture of individual differences in math anxiety in typical children. Sci. Rep. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-26912-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hembree, R. (1990). The nature, effects, and relief of mathematics anxiety. J. Res. Math. Educ. 21, 33–46. doi: 10.2307/749455

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hemphill, J. F. (2003). Interpreting the magnitudes of correlation coefficients. Am. Psychol. 58, 78–80. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.1.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., and Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ 327, 557–560. doi: 10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ho, H. Z., Senturk, D., Lam, A. G., Zimmer, J. M., Hong, S., Okamoto, Y., et al. (2000). The affective and cognitive dimensions of math anxiety: A cross-national study. J. Res. Math. Educ. 31, 362–379. doi: 10.2307/749811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*and Hoffman, B. (2010). 'I think I can, but I'm afraid to try': the role of self-efficacy beliefs and mathematics anxiety in mathematics problem-solving efficiency. Learn. Individ. Dif. 20, 276–283. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.02.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopko, D. R. (2003). Confirmatory factor analysis of the math anxiety rating scale–revised. Educ. Psychol. Measure. 63, 336–351. doi: 10.1177/0013164402251041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopko, D. R., Mahadevan, R., Bare, R. L., and Hunt, M. K. (2003). The abbreviated math anxiety scale (AMAS) construction, validity, and reliability. Assessment 10, 178–182. doi: 10.1177/1073191103010002008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Imbo, I., and Vandierendonck, A. (2007). The development of strategy use in elementary school children: working memory and individual differences. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 96, 284–309. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Juniati, D., and Budayasa, I. K. (2020). Working memory capacity and mathematics anxiety of mathematics undergraduate students and its effect on mathematics achievement. J. Educ. Gifted Young Sci. 8, 279–291. doi: 10.17478/jegys.653518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Justicia-Galiano, M. J., Martin-Puga, M. E., Linares, R., and Pelegrina, S. (2017). Math anxiety and math performance in children: the mediating roles of working memory and math self-concept. British J. Educ. Psychol. 87, 573–589. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kazelskis, R., Reeves, C., Kersh, M. E., Bailey, G., Cole, K., Larmon, M., et al. (2000). Mathematics anxiety and test anxiety: separate constructs? J. Exp. Educ. 68, 137–146. doi: 10.1080/00220970009598499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kucian, K., Zuber, I., Kohn, J., Poltz, N., Wyschkon, A., Esser, G., et al. (2018). Relation between mathematical performance, math anxiety, and affective priming in children with and without developmental dyscalculia. Front. Psychol. 9:263. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Kyttala, M., and Bjorn, P. M. (2014). The role of literacy skills in adolescents' mathematics word problem performance: controlling for visuo-spatial ability and mathematics anxiety. Learn. Individ. Dif. 29, 59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2013.10.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Lauer, J. E., Esposito, A. G., and Bauer, P. J. (2018). Domain-specific anxiety relates to children's math and spatial performance. Dev. Psychol. 54, 2126–2138. doi: 10.1037/dev0000605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liang, Z., Dong, P., Zhou, Y., Feng, S., and Zhang, Q. (2021). Whether verbal and visuospatial working memory play different roles in pupil's mathematical abilities. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. e12454. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Likhanov, M., Zakharov, I., Kuzmina, Y., Budakova, A., Vasin, G., Malykh, S., et al. (2017). “Maths anxiety does not moderate the link between spatial and maths ability,” in Icpe 2017: International Conference on Psychology and Education (Moscow). doi: 10.15405/epsbs.2017.12.21

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llabre, M. M., and Suarez, E. (1985). Predicting math anxiety and course performance in college women and men. J. Counsel. Psychol. 32:283. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.32.2.283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X. (1999). A meta-analysis of the relationship between anxiety toward mathematics and achievement in mathematics. J. Res. Math. Educ. 30, 520–540. doi: 10.2307/749772

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, X., and Xu, J. (2004). The causal ordering of mathematics anxiety and mathematics achievement: a longitudinal panel analysis. J. Adolesc. 27, 165–179. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Maldonado Moscoso, P. A., Anobile, G., Primi, C., and Arrighi, R. (2020). Math anxiety mediates the link between number sense and math achievements in high math anxiety young adults. Front. Psychol. 11:1095. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. A., and Beilock, S. L. (2012). Math anxiety: who has it, why it develops, and how to guard against it. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 404–406. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. A., Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E. A., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2015). Intergenerational effects of parents' math anxiety on children's math achievement and anxiety. Psychol. Sci. 26, 1480–1488. doi: 10.1177/0956797615592630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maloney, E. A., Waechter, S., Risko, E. F., and Fugelsang, J. A. (2012). Reducing the sex difference in math anxiety: the role of spatial processing ability. Learn. Individ. Dif. 22, 380–384. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Miller, H., and Bichsel, J. (2004). Anxiety, working memory, gender, and math performance. Pers. Individ. Dif. 37, 591–606. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., and Prisma Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mononen, R., Niemivirta, M., Korhonen, J., Lindskog, M., and Tapola, A. (2021). Developmental relations between mathematics anxiety, symbolic numerical magnitude processing, and arithmetic skills from first to second grade. Cogn. Emot. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2021.2015296. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Namkung, J. M., Peng, P., and Lin, X. (2019). The relation between mathematics anxiety and mathematics performance among school-aged students: a meta-analysis. Rev. Educ. Res. 89, 459–496. doi: 10.3102/0034654319843494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Necka, E. A., Sokolowski, H. M., and Lyons, I. M. (2015). The role of self-math overlap in understanding math anxiety and the relation between math anxiety and performance. Front. Psychol. 6:1543. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01543

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Novak, E., and Tassell, J. (2015). Using video game play to improve education-majors' mathematical performance: an experimental study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 53, 124–130. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.07.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Novak, E., and Tassell, J. L. (2017). Studying preservice teacher math anxiety and mathematics performance in geometry, word, and non-word problem solving. Learn. Individ. Dif. 54, 20–29. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Núñez-Peña, M. I., Suárez-Pellicioni, M., Guilera, G., and Mercadé-Carranza, C. (2013). A Spanish version of the short Mathematics Anxiety Rating Scale (sMARS). Lean. Individ. Dif. 24, 204–210. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Leary, K., Fitzpatrick, C. L., and Hallett, D. (2017). Math anxiety is related to some, but not all, experiences with math. Front. Psychol. 8:2067. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Orbach, L., Herzog, M., and Fritz, A. (2020). State- and trait-math anxiety and their relation to math performance in children: the role of core executive functions. Cognition 200:104271. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Pappas, M. A., Polychroni, F., and Drigas, A. S. (2019). Assessment of mathematics difficulties for second and third graders: cognitive and psychological parameters. Behav. Sci. 9, 76–76. doi: 10.3390/bs9070076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passolunghi, M. C. (2011). Cognitive and emotional factors in children with mathematical learning disabilities. Int. J. Disabil. 58, 61–73. doi: 10.1080/1034912X.2011.547351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Passolunghi, M. C., Cargnelutti, E., and Pellizzoni, S. (2019). The relation between cognitive and emotional factors and arithmetic problem-solving. Educ. Stud. Math. 100, 271–290. doi: 10.1007/s10649-018-9863-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., and Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. J. Educ. Psychol. 108:455. doi: 10.1037/edu0000079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peters, J. L., Sutton, A. J., Jones, D. R., Abrams, K. R., and Rushton, L. (2007). Performance of the trim and fill method in the presence of publication bias and between? study heterogeneity. Stat. Med. 26, 4544–4562. doi: 10.1002/sim.2889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Prast, E. J., Van de Weijer-Bergsma, E., Miocevic, M., Kroesbergen, E. H., and Van Luit, J. E. H. (2018). Relations between mathematics achievement and motivation in students of diverse achievement levels. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 55, 84–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2018.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Prevatt, F., Welles, T. L., Li, H., and Proctor, B. (2010). The contribution of memory and anxiety to the math performance of college students with learning disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 25, 39–47. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.00299.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Rabab'h, B., and Veloo, A. (2015). Spatial visualization as mediating between mathematics learning strategy and mathematics achievement among 8th grade students. Int. Educ. Stud. 8, 1–11. doi: 10.5539/ies.v8n5p1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raghubar, K. P., Barnes, M. A., and Hecht, S. A. (2010). Working memory and mathematics: a review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches. Learn. Individ. Dif. 20, 110–122. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2009.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Ramirez, G., Gunderson, E., Levine, S., and Beilock, S. (2013). Math anxiety, working memory, and math achievement in early elementary school. J. Cogn. Dev. 14, 187–202. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.664593

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramirez, G., Shaw, S. T., and Maloney, E. A. (2018). Math anxiety: past research, promising interventions, and a new interpretation framework. Educ. Psychol. 53, 145–164. doi: 10.1080/00461520.2018.1447384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, F. C., and Suinn, R. M. (1972). The mathematics anxiety rating scale: psychometric data. J. Counsel. Psychol. 19:551. doi: 10.1037/h0033456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robert, M., and Savoie, N. (2006). Are there gender differences in verbal and visuospatial working-memory resources? Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 18, 378–397. doi: 10.1080/09541440500234104

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rücker, G., Carpenter, J. R., and Schwarzer, G. (2011). Detecting and adjusting for small-study effects in meta-analysis. Biometr. J. 53, 351–368. doi: 10.1002/bimj.201000151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwarzer, G. (2007). Meta: an R package for meta-analysis. R News 7, 40–45.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

*Semeraro, C., Giofre, D., Coppola, G., Lucangeli, D., and Cassibba, R. (2020). The role of cognitive and non-cognitive factors in mathematics achievement: the importance of the quality of the student-teacher relationship in middle school. PLoS ONE 15:e0231381. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Shaw, S. T., Pogossian, A. A., and Ramirez, G. (2020). The mathematical flexibility of college students: the role of cognitive and affective factors. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 90, 981–996. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Skagerlund, K., Östergren, R., Västfjäll, D., and Träff, U. (2019). How does mathematics anxiety impair mathematical abilities? Investigating the link between math anxiety, working memory, and number processing. PLoS ONE 14:e0211283. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Sokolowski, H. M., Hawes, Z., and Lyons, I. M. (2019). What explains sex differences in math anxiety? A closer look at the role of spatial processing. Cognition 182, 193–212. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2018.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Soltanlou, M., Artemenko, C., Dresler, T., Fallgatter, A. J., Ehlis, A. C., and Nuerk, H. C. (2019). Math anxiety in combination with low visuospatial memory impairs math learning in children. Front. Psychol. 10:89. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sorvo, R., Koponen, T., Viholainen, H., Aro, T., Räikkönen, E., Peura, P., et al. (2019). Development of math anxiety and its longitudinal relationships with arithmetic achievement among primary school children. Learn. Individ. Dif. 69, 173–181. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J. A., and Egger, M. (2001). Funnel plots for detecting bias in meta-analysis: guidelines on choice of axis. J. Clin. Epidemiol. 54, 1046–1055. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(01)00377-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, L., and Kim, K. (2007). Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children's mathematical performance. Intelligence 35, 151–168. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2006.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Tomasetto, C., Morsanyi, K., Guardabassi, V., and O'Connor, P. A. (2020). Math anxiety interferes with learning novel mathematics contents in early elementary school. J. Educ. Psychol. 113, 315–29. doi: 10.1037/edu0000602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Trezise, K., and Reeve, R. A. (2014a). Cognition-emotion interactions: patterns of change and implications for math problem solving. Front. Psychol. 5:840. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Trezise, K., and Reeve, R. A. (2014b). Working memory, worry, and algebraic ability. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 121, 120–136. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Trezise, K., and Reeve, R. A. (2018). Patterns of anxiety in algebraic problem solving: a three-step latent variable analysis. Lean. Individ. Dif. 66, 78–91. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2018.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Vallée-Tourangeau, F., Sirota, M., and Vallée-Tourangeau, G. (2016). Interactivity mitigates the impact of working memory depletion on mental arithmetic performance. Cogn. Res. 1:26. doi: 10.1186/s41235-016-0027-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Valle-Tourangeau, F., Sirota, M., and Villejoubert, G. (2013). Reducing the impact of math anxiety on mental arithmetic: the importance of distributed cognition. Proc. Annu. Meet. Cogn. Sci. Soc. 35, 3615–3620.

Google Scholar

*Vukovic, R. K., Kieffer, M. J., Bailey, S. P., and Harari, R. R. (2013a). Mathematics anxiety in young children: concurrent and longitudinal associations with mathematical performance. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 38, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2012.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vukovic, R. K., Roberts, S. O., and Green Wright, L. (2013b). From parental involvement to children's mathematical performance: the role of mathematics anxiety. Early Educ. Dev. 24, 446–467. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2012.693430

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Hart, S. A., Kovas, Y., Lukowski, S., Soden, B., Thompson, L. A., et al. (2014). Who is afraid of math? Two sources of genetic variance for mathematical anxiety. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 1056–1064. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wang, Z., Rimfeld, K., Shakeshaft, N., Schofield, K., and Malanchini, M. (2020). The longitudinal role of mathematics anxiety in mathematics development: issues of gender differences and domain-specificity. J. Adolesc. 80, 220–232. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2020.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wigfield, A., and Meece, J. L. (1988). Math anxiety in elementary and secondary school students. J. Educ. Psychol. 80:210. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.2.210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiklund-Hörnqvist, C., Jonsson, B., Korhonen, J., Eklöf, H., and Nyroos, M. (2016). Untangling the contribution of the subcomponents of working memory to mathematical proficiency as measured by the national tests: a study among Swedish third graders. Front. Psychol. 7:1062. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*and Witt, M. (2012). The impact of mathematics anxiety on primary school children's working memory. Eur. J. Psychol. 8, 263–274. doi: 10.5964/ejop.v8i2.458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Wu, S. S., Chen, L., Battista, C., Smith Watts, A. K., Willcutt, E. G., and Menon, V. (2017). Distinct influences of affective and cognitive factors on children's non-verbal and verbal mathematical abilities. Cognition 166, 118–129. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2017.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*Zhang, J., Cheung, S. K., Wu, C. G., and Meng, Y. X. (2018). Cognitive and affective correlates of Chinese children's mathematical word problem solving. Front. Psychol. 9:2357. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, J., Zhao, N., and Kong, Q. P. (2019). The relationship between math anxiety and math performance: a meta-analytic investigation. Front. Psychol. 10:1613. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

*^Studies included in our meta-analyses (n = 56).

Keywords: math anxiety, math performance, meta-analysis, working memory, Attentional Control Theory (ACT)

Citation: Finell J, Sammallahti E, Korhonen J, Eklöf H and Jonsson B (2022) Working Memory and Its Mediating Role on the Relationship of Math Anxiety and Math Performance: A Meta-Analysis. Front. Psychol. 12:798090. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.798090

Received: 19 October 2021; Accepted: 28 December 2021;
Published: 20 January 2022.

Edited by:

Andreas Eichler, University of Kassel, Germany

Reviewed by:

Ilaria Berteletti, Gallaudet University, United States
Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2022 Finell, Sammallahti, Korhonen, Eklöf and Jonsson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jonatan Finell, jonatan.finell@umu.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.