Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T19:46:29.126Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The role of semantic association and emotional contagion for the induction of emotion with music

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 October 2008

Thomas Fritz
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germanyfritz@cbs.mpg.dehttp://www.cbs.mpg.de/staff/fritz-2064
Stefan Koelsch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Sussex, Brighton BN1 9QU, United Kingdom. mail@stefan-koelsch.dehttp://stefan-koelsch.com/

Abstract

We suggest that semantic association may be a further mechanism by which music may elicit emotion. Furthermore, we note that emotional contagion is not always an immediate process requiring little prior information processing; rather, emotional contagion contributing to music processing may constitute a more complex decoding mechanism for information inherent in the music, which may be subject to a time course of activation.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Carruthers, P. & Smith, P. K. (1996) Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gordon, R. M., Davies, M. & Stone, T. (1995) Mental simulation: Evaluations and applications. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T. & Rapson, R. L. (1994) Emotional contagion. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., von Cramon, D. Y., Müller, K. & Friederici, A. D. (2006) Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping 27:239–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V. & Rizzolatti, G. (2002) Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science 297:846–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipps, T. (1903) Einfühling, innere Nachahmung und Organempfindung. Archiv für die Gesamte Psychologie 1:465519.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1966) Phänomenologie der Wahrnehmung. Aldine de Gruyter. (Original work published in 1945).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nzewi, M., Anyahuru, I. & Ohiaraumunna, T. (2001) Beyond song texts: The lingual fundamentals of African drum music. Research in African Literatures 32:90104.Google Scholar
Preston, S. D. & de Waal, B. M. F. (2002) Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25(1):172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, W. (1990) A common coding approach to perception and action. In: Relationships between perception and action: Current approaches, ed. Prinz, O. N. W., pp. 167201. Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbeis, N. & Koelsch, S. (2008) Comparing the processing of music and language meaning using EEG and fMRI provides evidence for similar and distinct neural representations. PLoS ONE 3(5):e2226. DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0002226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed