Elsevier

Consciousness and Cognition

Volume 67, January 2019, Pages 86-97
Consciousness and Cognition

Influence of cognitive stance and physical perspective on subjective and autonomic reactivity to observed pain and pleasure: An immersive virtual reality study

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2018.11.010Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Virtual Pain and Pleasure elicit reactivity similar to that induced by real stimuli.

  • Physical perspective is fundamental for inducing ownership on the virtual hand.

  • Pain stimuli bring about maximal SCRs regardless of the physical perspective.

  • Physiological reactivity is reduced when taking Other-Oriented stance.

  • Other-Oriented stance induces highest affective and intensity ratings.

Abstract

Observing others’ pain may induce a reaction called personal distress that may be influenced by top-down (imagine self or other in pain, i.e., self- vs other-oriented stance) and bottom-up (physical perspective of those who suffer, i.e., first vs third person perspective- 1PP vs 3PP) processes. The different contributions of these processes have not been teased apart. By capitalizing on the power of Immersive Virtual Reality, we explored how behavioural (subjective ratings) and physiological reactivity (skin conductance reactivity, SCR) to pain and pleasure delivered to an avatar was influenced by Cognitive stance and Physical perspective. Taking an Other-Oriented stance leads to attributing higher congruent valence (i.e. pain rated as unpleasant and pleasure as pleasant) and intensity to the stimuli and induces reduced SCR. Ownership over the virtual limb was maximal in 1PP where physiological reactivity to the stimuli was comparable. Our results highlight different components underpinning reactivity to pain and pleasure.

Introduction

The inter-individual sharing of emotions, feelings and beliefs characterize human social interactions, a condition that is constitutive of empathy. This multidimensional construct includes a variety of components ranging from the automatic and involuntary reactivity that occurs on the self when we see others (e.g. sensorimotor contagion) to the other-oriented stance that allows us to understand others through cognition (e.g. perspective-taking) (Davis, 1980, Decety and Jackson, 2004). Studies on empathy for pain indicate that people who see or imagine others in pain tend to empathically share what others feel at both behavioral and neural levels (Betti and Aglioti, 2016, Lamm et al., 2011). Contrary to empathic concern (which maintains a clear self-other distinction), sensorimotor contagion is likely to (1) increase the personal distress in the observer (e.g. when witnessing painful stimuli being delivered to others) (Batson et al., 1997) and (2) lead to ego-centered reactivity. In contrast, perspective-taking, i.e. the mechanism that allows one to project self upon another individual (Davis, 1980), may allow one to take a distinct stance and consequently react with an adequate response. Taking the perspective of another may occur at two main levels, namely at the perceptual (i.e. seeing the state of a model in first vs. third person perspective) and the cognitive (i.e. imagining feeling the same state observed or imagined in others or imagining the feelings of others in a given state) level (Chiu and Yeh, 2017). More specifically, visual perspective-taking reflects the physical point-of-view (e.g., seeing painful stimuli in different perspectives), while the cognitive perspective-taking refers to a specific attributional stance (e.g. reports of what seen in others would feel like on them or on the self).

Neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies have investigated the contribution of visual and cognitive perspective-taking in the observation of pain in others (Abu-Akel et al., 2014, Avenanti et al., 2006, Bucchioni et al., 2016; Drwecki et al., 2011; Jackson, Brunet, Meltzoff, & Decety, 2006; Lamm et al., 2007a, Lamm et al., 2007b, 2008; Leong et al., 2015; Vistoli, Achim, Lavoie, & Jackson, 2016). For instance, Jackson et al. (2006) explored cognitive perspective-taking by asking participants to observe pictures of others in pain and to imagine and rate the level of the perceived pain from different perspectives (self or other). Results showed that adopting the other’s perspective induced a specific increase in the right temporo-parietal junction, renowned for playing a crucial role in perspective-taking. Bucchioni et al. (2016) found that observing images of hands receiving painful stimulation, inhibits the motor cortex activity only if the hands appear in first-person perspective (1PP) - not when rotated 180° (third-person perspective, 3PP). Recently, Vistoli et al. (2016) manipulated both the visual perspective (1PP and 3PP) and the cognitive stance (imagine oneself or another person in the painful or neutral situation) and showed that the visual perspective-taking modulates the activity of the temporo-parietal junction during the observation of pain (Lamm et al., 2011) which consequently became more activated when observing others in pain in 1PP than in 3PP. This result suggests that the cognitive perspective-taking can be influenced in a bottom-up way for example by changes in the visual perspective.

Advances in computer graphics over the last two decades allowed researchers to use Immersive Virtual Reality (IVR) as a new and powerful tool for psychology and neuroscience (Bohil et al., 2011, Monti and Aglioti, 2018, Pan and Hamilton, 2018, Sanchez-Vives and Slater, 2005, Tieri et al., 2018). Expanding on classical studies that investigated the induction of illusory Feelings of Ownership (FO), i.e. the illusion that an external hand belong to own body (e.g., the rubber hand illusion, Botvinick & Cohen, 1998), IVR studies show that FO over a virtual body part can be easily induced by presenting a virtual body in 1PP; by means of a Head Mounted Display (HMD) (Maselli and Slater, 2013, Maselli and Slater, 2014, Petkova and Ehrsson, 2008, Petkova et al., 2011, Slater et al., 2010). In the same vein, we recently observed that the mere passive observation of a virtual limb from a 1PP (without any multisensory visuo-tactile or visuo-motor boosting) is sufficient to elicit FO at explicit (e.g., questionnaire answers; Tieri, Tidoni, Pavone, & Aglioti, 2015a) and implicit [e.g. skin conductance response (SCR; Fusaro et al., 2016, Tieri et al., 2015b), thermoregulation (Tieri, Gioia, Scandola, Pavone, & Aglioti, 2017) and electroencephalographic response (Pavone et al., 2016; Pezzetta et al., 2018; Spinelli, Tieri, Pavone, & Aglioti, 2018) levels.

It is worth noting that while IVR has been used for exploring empathy for pain (Bouchard et al., 2013, Fusaro et al., 2016, Jackson et al., 2015) the study of empathy for positive states (e.g. the conditions in which imagining, recalling and observing joy in others triggers a positive state of mind in the empathizer) is still in its infancy (Paracampo, Pirruccio, Costa, Borgomaneri, & Avenanti, 2018). Importantly, however, pleasant touch represents an important element in interpersonal connections and leads to positive feelings that forge attachment and social bonds (Suvilehto, Glerean, Dunbar, Hari, & Nummenmaa, 2015). Yet, while studies started to focus on the (cognitive and visual) perspective-taking responses to painful stimulation (Bucchioni et al., 2016, Jackson et al., 2006, Vistoli et al., 2016) less attention has been paid to whether pleasant touch is also influenced by manipulating physical perspective and cognitive stance. By using IVR presented through an HMD, we explored how taking a different physical perspective (1PP vs 3PP) modulates the behavioral and the physiological (Skin Conductance Responses- SCR and Heart Rate) reactivity in participants who observe virtual painful and pleasant stimuli delivered on (i) a virtual body that was embodied when seen from 1PP or (ii) on another avatar seated in the opposite side of a table (3PP). Results showed that both behavioral and physiological effects were qualitatively comparable in the two physical perspectives (Fusaro et al., 2016).

In the present study, building upon previous research, we used IVR to investigate (i) how the physical perspective (i.e., avatar seen from 1PP vs. 3PP) could influence the subjective (Visuo Analogue Scale, VAS) and physiological (SCRs) responses to the observation of virtual stimuli (representing pain and pleasure) on self and others and, importantly, (ii) whether these responses are modulated by the top-down information related to the cognitive stance (reports while in Self- vs. Other- shoes) and how.

Section snippets

Participants

Thirty-six (females 24; mean age ± SD, 25.4 ± 3.2) right-handed participants (Handedness Inventory, Briggs & Nebes, 1975) were recruited with normal or corrected visual acuity and were naïve as to the purposes of the study. The experimental protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the Fondazione Santa Lucia and was carried out in accordance with the ethical standards of the 2013 Declaration of Helsinki. All participants gave their written informed consent to take part in the study. The

Subjective ratings: Self vs. Other-Oriented stance

VAS responses (see Table 2 for means and SDs) were normally distributed according to the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Data were analyzed using three different mixed model ANOVAs (one for each VAS question: (un)Pleasantness, Intensity and Ownership) with Group (Self vs Other-Oriented) as the between-subjects factor and type of observed stimulus (Pain, Pleasure and Neutral) and Physical Perspective (1PP vs 3PP) as the within-subject factors. Multiple comparison tests were performed post-hoc using the

Discussion

In the present study, we capitalized on the power of IVR in order to explore the role of Physical perspective (i.e., avatar seen from 1PP vs. 3PP) and Cognitive stance (Self- vs. Other-Oriented) on the behavioral and physiological reactivity upon the observation of pain and pleasure on self and other. In general, our results suggest that observing Pain (a virtual arm holding a needle approaching the right hand of an avatar’s and penetrating it) and Pleasure (a virtual hand approaching the

Conclusions

By capitalizing on the power of the paradigm we developed in our previous study (Fusaro et al., 2016), we have been able to compare pain and pleasure stimuli not only when they are observed in different physical perspectives (1PP vs 3PP) but also when a different cognitive stance (Self vs. Other- Oriented) is adopted. Our approach demonstrates that top-down variables modulate physiological and behavioral reactivity to observed pain and pleasure. Such an approach may have translational

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the BIAL Foundation (n° 218/2016) and by Italian Ministry of University and Research, PRIN, Progetti di Ricerca di Rilevante Interesse Nazionale, 2015 (Prot. 20159CZFJK).

References (58)

  • V.J. Madden et al.

    Pain by association? Experimental modulation of human pain thresholds using classical conditioning

    The Journal of Pain

    (2016)
  • G. Spinelli et al.

    Wronger than wrong: Graded mapping of the errors of an avatar in the performance monitoring system of the onlooker

    NeuroImage

    (2018)
  • M. Valeriani et al.

    Seeing the pain of others while being in pain: A laser-evoked potentials study

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • H.E. van Stralen et al.

    Affective touch modulates the rubber hand illusion

    Cognition

    (2014)
  • D. Vistoli et al.

    Changes in visual perspective influence brain activity patterns during cognitive perspective-taking of other people’s pain

    Neuropsychologia

    (2016)
  • F.H. Wilhelm et al.

    Social anxiety and response to touch: Incongruence between self-evaluative and physiological reactions

    Biological Psychology

    (2001)
  • A. Abu-Akel et al.

    The role of oxytocin in empathy to the pain of conflictual out-group members among patients with schizophrenia

    Psychological Medicine

    (2014)
  • K.C. Armel et al.

    Projecting sensations to external objects: Evidence from skin conductance response

    Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences

    (2003)
  • C.D. Batson et al.

    Is empathy-induced helping due to self-other merging?

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1997)
  • V. Betti et al.

    Synchronous with your feelings: Sensorimotor band and empathy for pain

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • C.J. Bohil et al.

    Virtual reality in neuroscience research and therapy

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • M. Botvinick et al.

    Rubber hands “feel” touch that eyes see

    Nature

    (1998)
  • S. Bouchard et al.

    Empathy toward virtual humans depicting a known or unknown person expressing pain

    Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking

    (2013)
  • G. Bucchioni et al.

    Empathy or ownership? Evidence from corticospinal excitability modulation during pain observation

    Journal of cognitive neuroscience

    (2016)
  • I. Bufalari et al.

    Empathy for pain and touch in the human somatosensory cortex

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2007)
  • M. Candidi et al.

    Come together: Human–avatar on-line interactions boost joint-action performance in apraxic patients

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2017)
  • C.D. Chiu et al.

    In your shoes or mine? Shifting from other to self perspective is vital for emotional empathy

    Emotion

    (2017)
  • L. Crucianelli et al.

    Bodily pleasure matters: velocity of touch modulates body ownership during the rubber hand illusion

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2013)
  • M.H. Davis

    A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empahty

    Catalog of Selected Document in Psychology

    (1980)
  • Cited by (32)

    • Doing it Wrong: A Systematic Review on Electrocortical and Behavioral Correlates of Error Monitoring in Patients with Neurological Disorders

      2022, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Paradigms in which EEG activity is recorded while patients observe errors performed by their embodied avatar, seem promising. Immersive virtual reality scenarios can indeed generate the feeling of sensory feedback related to an individual’s virtual body and thus induce effects on the individual’s cognition (Fusaro et al., 2019). However, to the best of our knowledge, immersive virtual reality studies of error monitoring have been done only in healthy adults (Padrao et al., 2016; Pavone et al., 2016; Pezzetta et al., 2018; Spinelli et al., 2018; Tieri et al., 2018), while patients have been tested with speed-response tasks.

    • Owning a virtual body entails owning the value of its actions in a detection-of-deception procedure

      2021, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, the SCRs did not correlate significantly with the subjective ownership and agency measures. This finding is, firstly, consistent with the idea that SCRs do not necessarily reflect the subjective measures of body ownership [see, for example, a study by Tieri, Tidoni, Pavone, & Aglioti, 2015a where no correlations were observed between ownership of a virtual hand and the SCRs to stimuli threatening that hand, or a study by Fusaro, Tieri, & Aglioti, 2019 where SCRs for painful and pleasant stimuli were comparable between the 1PP and the 3PP conditions]. Secondly, in our study, the SCRs are related to the stimuli of the CIT procedure and the underlying cognitive/emotional processes, rather than ownership of the avatar per se.

    • Visual similarity and psychological closeness are neurally dissociable in the brain response to vicarious pain

      2020, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      This observation is line with evidence showing the role of perspective taking in the cognitive resonance mechanisms associated with vicarious pain. In particular, it has been shown that the ability to imagine oneself in painful situations experienced by others is associated with a specific physiological responses (Fusaro, Tieri, & Aglioti, 2016) and brain activation patterns within the temporo-parietal junction, including the SMG (Vistoli, Achim, Lavoie, & Jackson, 2016), the same region found in the present study where Stimulus and Model significantly influenced the neural reactivity to vicarious pain. The neural activity in rSMG and ACC was significantly affected by the joint effect (interaction) of Stimulus (pain, touch) and Model (adult, baby, dog puppy, robot).

    • Midline frontal and occipito-temporal activity during error monitoring in dyadic motor interactions

      2020, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      The photodiode was triggered by a white dot displayed on the screen (not visible to the participants) during the clip frame corresponding to the instant when the avatar grasped its virtual object. The kinematic features of the virtual partner were based on the movements of human participants performing different grasping movements during a human–human joint-grasping task, identical to the procedures described in Candidi et al. (2017) (see Tieri, Tidoni, Pavone, & Aglioti, 2015; Fusaro, Tieri, & Aglioti, 2019 for technical details of the Motion Capture recording). The final processed trajectories were realized and applied to a Caucasian male character by using commercial software MotionBuilder 2017 and 3DS Max 2017 (Autodesk).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    These authors contributed equally to this work.

    View full text