Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 20 May 2021
Sec. Emotion Science

Facial Emotion Recognition and Emotional Memory From the Ovarian-Hormone Perspective: A Systematic Review

  • Experimental Psychology II and Biological Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Osnabrück, Osnabrück, Germany

Background: We review original papers on ovarian-hormone status in two areas of emotional processing: facial emotion recognition and emotional memory. Ovarian-hormone status is operationalized by the levels of the steroid sex hormones 17β-estradiol (E2) and progesterone (P4), fluctuating over the natural menstrual cycle and suppressed under oral contraceptive (OCs) use. We extend previous reviews addressing single areas of emotional processing. Moreover, we systematically examine the role of stimulus features such as emotion type or stimulus valence and aim at elucidating factors that reconcile the inconsistent results.

Methods: We followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines and included papers published until September 2020 indexed in PubMed and Web of Science databases. Search terms were MeSH terms (emotional OR emotion) AND (X) AND (estrogen OR progesterone OR menstrual cycle OR oral contraceptives) with (X) representing our separately searched areas, resulting in (processing OR recognition OR empathy), and (memory OR recall). To be included, articles had to (1) be written and published in English, (2) examine healthy, non-pregnant adult women in their reproductive age, and (3) measure or at least estimate levels of E2 and P4. In PubMed, the search was (4) limited to humans and (5) to the search term present in the title or abstract.

Results: Features of the provided stimulus material (emotion type and/or valence) constitute a relevant influence that interacts with E2- and P4-related ovarian-hormone status. For instance, recognition of basic emotions appears to be more related to P4- than E2-levels. Quite consistent, OC intake (vs. natural menstrual cycling) was accompanied by impaired recognition accuracy of basic and also complex emotions, although not in a recent large-sample study assessing complex emotions. Memory recall of negative content was mainly enhanced by P4, especially after having been stressed.

Discussion and Conclusion: We document the methodological diversity in the field, presumably contributing to the heterogeneity of results. More studies explicitly contrasting the early follicular phase, mid-cycle phase, mid-luteal, and OC intake while standardizing tasks are needed. Research would take advantage of using within-subject designs and accounting for the recognition of complex emotions.

Introduction

A growing number of original and review papers on the role of naturally fluctuating ovarian hormones and oral contraceptive (OCs) use for emotional processing indicates an increased interest in this field (for reviews see: Andreano et al., 2018; Osório et al., 2018; Lewis et al., 2019). Due to significant level variation of ovarian hormones over the menstrual cycle (Becker et al., 2005), women go through monthly hormonal shifts. The menstrual cycle is regulated via the hypothalamus-pituitary-gonadal axis with a prominent fluctuation of 17-β estradiol (E2) and progesterone (P4) levels. E2 and P4 are low in the early follicular phase. E2 reaches its peak right before ovulation (mid-cycle), while P4 remains low. P4-levels peak during the luteal phase, precisely in the mid-luteal phase, accompanied by a second, less prominent E2 rise. Both hormones reach low levels again by the onset of a new cycle. OCs, typically applied in a 21-day pill-on and a 7-day pill-off regimen, suppress the endogenous production of both, E2 and P4 resulting in steadily low levels of both hormones. Therefore, OC use has often been considered as a control condition for high-hormone phases during the natural menstrual cycle. However, OC use is not simply a state of low endogenous ovarian hormones: Synthetic ovarian hormones, i.e., ethinylestradiol and progestins, contained in the OCs, have the potential to modulate neuro-regulatory mechanisms involved in cognitive and emotional processing by interacting with E2- and P4- receptors in the brain (for review see: Montoya and Bos, 2017; Lewis et al., 2019; Brønnick et al., 2020). Thus, studies on the influence of OC use on emotional processing are informative in their own right.

Women also may go through cognitive and emotional shifts during the menstrual cycle: E2 and P4 contribute to cognitive and affective processes via acting on both, genomic (nuclear) and non-genomic (transmembrane) receptors in brain areas associated with emotion, memory, and executive functions (e.g., amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex) (Gasbarri et al., 2012; Sundström-Poromaa, 2018). They also modulate synaptic transmission in the noradrenergic, dopaminergic, serotonergic, glutamatergic, and GABAergic systems and can thereby affect cognitive and affective processes (Toffoletto et al., 2014).

Areas of emotional processing that are often studied regarding the role of ovarian hormones are facial emotion recognition and emotional memory. Facial emotion recognition is indicated via accurately recognizing basic emotions from the facial expressions of protagonists, or complex emotions - requiring inferring mental states beyond the categorization of mere basic emotion (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001; Dziobek et al., 2008; Drimalla et al., 2019). This ability is essential in social cognition and indicated to be impaired in various psychiatric disorders (see: Sheaffer et al., 2009). Current research related to basic and complex emotion recognition suggests a sex effect where women tend to outperform men, although the results are quite mixed (Hoffmann et al., 2010; Wright et al., 2018; Wingenbach et al., 2018; Saylik et al., 2018; Connolly et al., 2019). Importantly, the link between facial emotion recognition and ovarian hormones is not straightforward. Limited evidence suggests that emotion recognition is enhanced during the follicular phase and impaired under OC use, whereas other studies fail to replicate these results or even reveal a better recognition of specific emotions under high P4 (vs. low P4) levels (for review see: Osório et al., 2018). Emotional memory (or better emotionally biased memory) is a relevant domain to examine the specific role of emotionally valenced (vs. neutral) material for memory consolidation and recall. Cognitive biases, including emotionally biased memory, are also a predictor of developing and maintaining affective disorders (Smith et al., 2018). Thus, understanding the role of ovarian hormones in emotionally biased memory has an important clinical relevance.

When reviewing menstrual cycle studies, it is important to consider what specific comparisons can tell us about the role of E2 and P4 in an emotional processing task. Assuming that cycle phases were identified correctly (see Becker et al., 2005; Sundström-Poromaa and Gingnell, 2014 for recommendations), different comparisons have different informational values. Studies comparing women during a wide range of days in the follicular phase with women during a wide range of days in the luteal phase can tell us something about the role of P4. Because, on average, the follicular phase would have low P4 and low to medium E2 – while the luteal phase would have, on average, high P4 and low to medium E2. However, it remains unclear if any reported effects are due to P4 or the specific combination of high P4 and medium E2 levels during the luteal phase. Also, comparing the luteal vs. the follicular phase using broad definitions of cycle days often will include some women tested during the pre-menstrual days and the actual days of menstruation. This adds potential menstrual or pre-menstrual discomfort as a confound. Studies trying to compare narrowly defined cycle phases deliver more information. For example, studies comparing the early follicular (low E2, low P4) with the late follicular phase (moderate to peak E2, with low P4) or with the ovulatory phase (peak E2, low P4) can provide evidence for the role of E2. Accompanied by a significant correlation between measured E2 levels and task performance, such a comparison delivers strong evidence for E2 modulation in a task. Comparing the mid-luteal phase (peak P4, moderate E2) to the late follicular phase (moderate to peak E2, with low P4) or with the ovulatory phase (peak E2, low P4) and the low-hormone early follicular phase can deliver strong evidence for a specific role of P4. Again, correlations with the measured hormone levels can strengthen this.

So far, reviews on the role of ovarian-hormone status address facial emotion recognition or emotional memory separately. In the present review, we update the state of the art of the corresponding research in both, facial emotion recognition and emotional memory. For both areas, we aim to find out whether hormone status is relevant in itself (main effects) or via interaction with stimulus features. Moreover, we intend to compare the result-pattern of both areas of emotion processing. Specifically, we hypothesize that high hormone states enhance emotional processing depending on the specific emotional valence of stimuli, i.e., we expect an interaction between hormone status and stimulus features. Additionally, we expect impaired emotional processing in OC users due to low endogenous ovarian hormones. Finally, we ask whether inconsistencies in the results may be associated with methodological differences among the studies.

Materials and Methods

Data Source and Search Strategy

We followed the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher et al., 2009). The systematic literature search was performed using electronic databases PubMed and Web of Science covering all publications until September 4, 2020. We allocated studies in two areas: (1) emotion recognition and (2) emotional memory by using the MeSH terms (emotional OR emotion) AND (X) AND (estrogen OR progesterone OR menstrual cycle OR oral contraceptives) with (X) representing the separately searched areas of emotional processing, concretely (processing OR recognition OR empathy), and (memory OR recall) in the title and/or abstract. We noticed that some studies on facial-emotion-recognition can be found using the search term “empathy”. Therefore, we also added empathy to the search to avoid overlooking relevant papers.

Publication Screening and Eligibility Criteria

After conducting the first selection of the search, we eliminated study duplicates. We organize the presentation of the search results based on the PICOS criteria. We specify the study design, population, interventions, comparators, and outcomes. Eligible studies had to examine healthy adult women in their reproductive age to restrict the influence of postmenopausal hormone conditions or physical and/or mental health conditions. Most studies compare the performance of free-cycling women in different phases of the menstrual cycle. Some studies also compare free-cycling women (overall) vs. OC users and free-cycling women vs. men. Only one study used a true experimental manipulation of the women’s hormone status by administering exogenous P4 (van Wingen et al., 2008). In the remaining studies, hormone status served as a quasi-experimental factor, relying on hormone levels and variations as naturally found across menstrual cycle phases and/or during OC use. In addition to comparisons between different hormone-status conditions, some of the emotional memory studies (10 of 21) included an experimental stressor vs. a non-stressful control condition as a between-subject factor. In these cases, experiments mainly used established stress-induction procedures such as the Cold Pressor Test (Hines and Brown, 1936) or the Trier Social Stress Test (Kirschbaum et al., 1993).

For studies on facial emotion recognition and emotional memory, the following criteria led to the inclusion of a study: (1) published in English, (2) examining healthy adult (18 or older) non-pregnant women in their reproductive age, and (3) including measurement or estimation of ovarian hormones (endogenous production or exogenous administration of E2 and/or P4) during the natural menstrual cycle and/or OC use. While available only in PubMed, the search was (4) limited to humans and (5) the search term being included in the title or abstract. No other limitations were applied.

Results

Search Result

Through Web Search, we identified the following number of publications fulfilling the criteria described above. We first eliminated study duplicates. This resulted in 18 studies on emotion recognition (from initial 380 non-duplicate publications, with 362 excluded based on title and abstract, one of them after full-text assessment, Figure 1). For emotional memory, 21 publications (from initial 257 non-duplicate publications, 236 excluded) were included (Figure 2). The flow charts in Figures 1, 2 list the exact reasons for exclusion and the number of studies excluded based on a specific criterion. Figure 3 gives an overview of the definition of cycle phases across the reviewed studies.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart showing detailed systematic search and selection process for emotion recognition.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Flow chart showing detailed systematic search and selection process for emotional memory.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Classification of the definition of cycle phases across the reviewed menstrual cycle studies. The bold-face titles of the rectangular boxes show the labels we use throughout the review, to refer to phases of the menstrual cycle. The three horizontal stripes in the middle show the idealized 28 cycle days and the associated levels of 17β-estradiol (E2, green) and progesterone (P4, orange). The color gradient signifies hormone levels, with the highest levels shown in the darkest color and the lowest in the lightest color. The boxes are arranged so that the width of a box reflects the menstrual cycle days included under the specific label and the associated hormone levels. Entries in each box (small font) reflect the individual studies we combined under the same label, with their specific cycle days and their own cycle-phase labels. E.g., 2 × LU: 15-28 in the “Luteal”-box implies that two studies labeled cycle days from 15 to 28 days as the luteal phase. The figure serves to illustrate the variety of labels and included cycle days for the follicular and the luteal phase. Abbreviations. E2: 17β-estradiol; FO: follicular; LU: luteal; P4: progesterone. [number] × …: number of studies using the referred labeling; blue print: emotion recognition studies; black print: emotional memory studies; (+): more than a 28-day cycle.

Study Characteristics

Tables 1, 2 contain the selected studies addressing facial emotion recognition, Tables 3, 4 summarize the corresponding data for emotional memory in healthy subjects. The tables account for the studies’ design, including the factor hormone status (predominantly realized as a between-subject factor, but in a few studies also as a within-subject factor, within a cycle phase, or as “pill on/off” phase in OC users) and at least one within-subject factor covering stimulus features (number of conditions in italics).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Emotion recognition in hormone-status groups: Design and effects.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Emotion recognition and ovarian hormones: Operationalization of the factors hormone status and emotion type.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Emotional memory in hormone-status groups: Design and effects.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Emotional memory, ovarian hormones, and stress: Operationalization of the factors hormone status and stimuli (instrument and valence), and specification of the recall procedure.

The factor hormone status was defined as phases during the menstrual cycle, or OC use, or men (low ovarian-hormone profile). Moreover, menstrual cycle studies use a range of different labels to refer to specific cycle phases. In some cases, the same range of cycle days receives a different label, depending on the study, while in other cases different cycle-day ranges are labeled identically (see: Figure 3, non-bold, small font entries). To aid understanding, we use the following labels (relative to an idealized 28-day cycle) throughout this review (given in bold font in Figure 3): Cycle days 1-14 after the onset of the menstrual cycle are labeled as the follicular phase. Within the follicular phase, days 1-6 are referred to as early follicular, and days 7-14 as the late follicular phase. The term luteal phase is used for days 15-28, with a subdivision into early luteal phase for cycle days 15-20, and late luteal phase for cycle days 22-28. As a special case, some studies use narrower phase definitions. These are labeled as an ovulatory phase (days 11-18, cycle length-dependent, verified by ovulation tests) and mid-luteal phase (days 18-24).

As for the within-subject factor, in emotion recognition studies (Tables 1, 2), it typically includes emotion type, mostly basic emotions (n = 16 of 18 studies). Only very few studies (n = 4 of 18) account for the recognition of complex emotions using the “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test” (RMET; Baron-Cohen et al., 2001) with two of these four studies also measuring basic emotions. In emotional memory studies (Tables 3, 4), the within-subject factor covered emotional valence categories of negative (n = 3), additionally neutral (n = 10), or all three including positive valence (n = 8).

Emotional memory studies (Tables 3, 4) are additionally divided into those considering the quasi-experimental factor hormone status only (n = 11 of 21 studies), and those with additional experimental stress-induction (n = 10 of 21). The stressor mainly (n = 5 of 10 studies) consisted in the Cold Pressor Test (Hines and Brown, 1936) which is validated as an effective tool to induce a quick first-wave stress response with increased peripheral sympathetic activation and brain increase of monoaminergic transmitters. Further studies used a psychosocial stressor (Kirschbaum et al., 1993, n = 3 studies) with first- and second-wave stress responses (including a cortisol increase), a physical sympathetic stressor (handgrip, n = 1 study), and a pharmacological hydrocortisone administration (n = 1 study).

For all studies, we report the main effects of hormone status, stimulus features, and their interactions as well as correlations for E2 and P4 for emotion recognition (Table 1) and emotional memory studies (Table 3). We extend the analysis of main effects and interactions to the factor stress (in the case of memory studies). Table 2 (emotion recognition) and Table 4 (emotional memory) provide additional information about hormone-status groups, stimuli, and tasks used.

Emotion Recognition and Hormone Status

The main effects of hormone status (Table 1) are reported in a few studies with a better emotion recognition in the follicular compared to the luteal phase (Derntl et al., 2008a, 2013; Guapo et al., 2009). An experimental P4-administration (400 mg vs. placebo) in the early-follicular phase increased the amygdala and decreased the fusiform gyrus activity (two brain sites critically involved in extracting emotional information from facial expressions) without affecting recognition accuracy (van Wingen et al., 2008). In contrast, high (vs. low) P4 levels were shown to be accompanied by better recognition accuracy (Maner and Miller, 2014) and stronger emotion intensity perception (Conway et al., 2007). Notably, these enhancing effects of P4 emerged when only negative emotions were considered (Conway et al., 2007; Maner and Miller, 2014). On the other hand, there were no changes in emotion recognition during the cycle phases with high E2 versus low/high P4 levels. E.g., there are null results when comparing between free-cycling women in their high-E2 late-follicular and/or ovulatory phase with women in their early follicular phase and/or different stages of their luteal phase (Derntl et al., 2008b; Zhang et al., 2013; Kamboj et al., 2015; Dan et al., 2019). OC use is consistently accompanied by impaired facial emotion recognition (Hamstra et al., 2014, 2015, 2016 [trendwise]; 2017; Pahnke et al., 2019). Although, these findings also have to face null results in small (Radke and Derntl, 2016) and large sample sizes (Shirazi et al., 2020).

Considering the interaction between hormone status and single emotion type (or valence) reveals whether the recognition of single emotions is facilitated vs. impaired in specific cycle phases. The majority of studies did not report (n = 6) or find (n = 9) any interactions. On the other hand, Guapo et al. (2009) found a better recognition of anger and sadness in free-cycling women in the early follicular phase as compared to the luteal phase and men. The ovulatory phase was associated with better recognition of fear as compared to men (Guapo et al., 2009). Similarly, in the ovulatory compared to the early follicular phase, women showed better recognition of fear (Pearson and Lewis, 2005). Besides, using a within-subject comparison, free-cycling women perceived more intense fear and tended to perceive stronger disgust in averted-gaze faces in their high-P4 stage, while there was no difference for happiness (Conway et al., 2007).

Emotional Memory and Hormone Status

Emotional memory studies report fewer hormone status main effects (Table 3) compared to emotion recognition studies. Notably, emotion recognition studies often rely on testing one-way (or multivariate) ANOVAs for single emotions, whereas emotional memory studies usually use repeated-measures analyses including more than one valence category as a within-subject factor. Limited data show that memory recall was impaired in the follicular phase and enhanced in the ovulatory (Pompili et al., 2016) or luteal phases (Ertman et al., 2011; Zoladz et al., 2015). A reliable pattern emerges for intrusive content with more frequent intrusions in the luteal versus follicular phase (Ferree and Cahill, 2009; Ferree et al., 2011; Soni et al., 2013). Moreover, P4-levels, characteristic of the luteal phase, were predictive of mental imagery strength (Wassell et al., 2015). Regarding OC use, neither pill phase (on and off) (Mordecai et al., 2017), nor OC use per se (vs. free-cycling in the luteal or follicular phases) affected emotional memory (Kuhlmann and Wolf, 2005; Nielsen et al., 2011, 2013b, 2014). Nevertheless, OC use was accompanied by fewer false memories in a misinformation task as compared to free-cycling women (follicular and luteal) (Petersen et al., 2015). Moreover, hormone status × valence interactions were often not reported (n = 5) or found (n = 6). Available data show that women in the (early) follicular phase displayed a bias to recall negative content (Bayer et al., 2014; Maki et al., 2015), whereas in the ovulatory phase positive content was predominantly recalled (Pompili et al., 2016). In contrast, negative intrusions were more prominent in the luteal than follicular phase (Ferree and Cahill, 2009; Ferree et al., 2011). Interestingly, in a large sample (1215 OC users and 954 free-cycling women) OC users showed better recall of positive contents than free-cycling women (Spalek et al., 2019).

Stress Effects and Interactions in Emotional Memory

A main effect of stress (Table 3), where post-encoding stress improved delayed free recall, was only found in the minority of studies (Felmingham et al., 2012 [trendwise]; Kuhlmann and Wolf, 2005; Zoladz et al., 2015). In contrast, Maki et al. (2015) found better memory recall in the non-stressed group. A stress × valence interaction was reported (Mordecai et al., 2017) as an impairment of the recall of words of negative and neutral content in the stressed vs. control group (sample consisting exclusively of OC users). Likewise, Zoladz et al. (2015) showed that the stressed group recalled more negative words than the non-stressed controls. Stress affects women differentially in different cycle phases as evident in three-way interactions between valence, hormone status, and stress. Here data converge to show that stress experience after encoding leads to better negative memory recall during a high-E2 and high-P4-state in the free cycling women (vs. OC users), and similarly in the luteal (vs. follicular) phase (Felmingham et al., 2012; Nielsen et al., 2014, 2015). In contrast, using verbal material (words instead of images), Maki et al. (2015) found better memory for negative content in the follicular phase, and Zoladz et al. (2015) reported better recall of arousing (vs. non-arousing) words in the stressed (vs. not stressed) follicular group. On the other hand, Schoofs and Wolf (2009) and Nielsen et al. (2013a) did not find any stress-related interactions.

Discussion

In the discussion, we address the role of hormone status in facial emotion recognition and emotional memory, discuss some of the methodological constraints of the reviewed studies, suggest some future directions, and finally name limitations of the present systematic review.

The Role of Hormone Status

In both areas of emotional processing, i.e., emotion recognition and emotional memory, some common patterns can be identified. Not surprisingly, stimulus features, i.e., emotion type in case of emotion recognition, and emotional valence of the stimulus material significantly affect performance in both areas. The results are more complex and heterogeneous when accounting for the main effects of hormone status, and importantly its interaction with stimulus features. This is partly due to methodological differences between the studies regarding the selected cycle (sub-) phases. Moreover, even within studies on the same area of emotional processing (i.e., facial emotion recognition, and emotionally biased memory), the employed tasks and stimulus material varied.

Concerning emotion recognition, there is only limited evidence that the follicular phase with low E2- and P4-levels, on average, facilitates facial emotion recognition (Derntl et al., 2008a, 2013; Guapo et al., 2009). Accounting for the interaction with the valence of the recognized emotion (and thus a stimulus feature) reveals some facilitating role of P4 (higher in the luteal phase), particularly in recognition of negative emotions (Conway et al., 2007; Maner and Miller, 2014). However, the majority of studies addressing basic-emotion recognition do not find a difference between cycle phases of different P4-levels (Derntl et al., 2008b; Zhang et al., 2013; Kamboj et al., 2015; Dan et al., 2019). But some of these studies do not use the optimal cycle-phases to entail high P4-conditions, e.g., when the late luteal phase is used to account for a high-P4 stage (Zhang et al., 2013; Kamboj et al., 2015).

Reports of a facilitating role of P4 (higher in the luteal phase), particularly in recognition of negative emotions, are in line with enhanced emotional memory (Ertman et al., 2011; Zoladz et al., 2015) and negative intrusions (Ferree and Cahill, 2009; Ferree et al., 2011) in the luteal phase. There is also an interesting hint that it might be especially helpful to rely on the E2/P4 ratio when identifying stages of the higher vulnerability of intrusion frequencies (Soni et al., 2013): The early luteal phase (with initially low post-ovulatory E2 and relatively high P4) was shown to set a stage for more frequent intrusions in healthy women. On the brain-functional level, a bias of processing emotionally negatively-valenced information emerges under high P4-levels possibly due to increased amygdala reactivity in this hormonal state (van Wingen et al., 2008).

The luteal phase has recently been described as a “window of vulnerability” with increased brain network connectivity between the default-mode network and the salience network due to the actions of high levels of progesterone and its metabolite allopregnanolone but also because of the moderate estrogen levels (Andreano et al., 2018). This is accompanied by higher autonomic and stress reactivity, an increase in memory for negative events, a rise in negative affective symptoms (see: Andreano et al., 2018), and a higher risk of developing affective disorders during this cycle phase (Bryant et al., 2011). Future studies should find out whether the negativity bias might also favor a “better” recognition of negative emotions in a state of high P4 and moderate E2 levels. This would need a more sophisticated assessment of emotion recognition, not only relying on typically 4-5 basic negative emotions, and one positive emotion but by assessing an equal number of complex positive and negative emotions (see: “Methodological Constraints” and Limitations of the Present Review).

In correspondence with our expectations, OC use is rather associated with impaired emotion recognition (basic and complex) with no impact on emotional memory performance. In contrast, Radke and Derntl (2016) (in basic emotions) as well as a recent large-sample study (in complex emotions) (Shirazi et al., 2020) dismiss the impairing role of OCs in emotion recognition. Thus, OC effects on emotion recognition range from no influences to impairing effects. These contradicting results might come from a vast difference in the sample size and accordingly in the power of the studies. Moreover, inconsistencies may also be due to differences in the precision of subdividing the menstrual cycle phases of the free-cycling women that serve as a comparison group. OC intake is usually taken as a hormone state of low endogenous levels of E2 and P4. However, there is a difference between low levels of E2 and P4 in free-cycling women vs. in an OC condition (low endogenous levels of ovarian hormones). E.g., although synthetic ovarian hormones act on the same receptors as endogenous E2 and P4 (Jung-Hoffman and Kuhl, 1987), in general, there is evidence for many neurobiological differences between OC users and free-cycling women (regardless of cycle phase). OC use was shown to be accompanied with heightened emotional reactivity (depression, irritability), specifically in women prone to negative mood symptoms (Sundström-Poromaa and Segebladh, 2012; for review see: Montoya and Bos, 2017; Lewis et al., 2019). In adolescents, there is evidence for a higher prevalence of depression and suicide when taking OCs as compared to non-user adolescents, especially during the initial intake (debut) of the pill (for review see: Brønnick et al., 2020). OC use is further associated with structural and functional changes in areas involved in affective and cognitive processing such as the amygdala, hippocampus, prefrontal cortex, and cingulate gyrus (Brønnick et al., 2020). For example, there is data showing that gray-matter volume in prefrontal and hippocampal/parahippocampal regions is reduced in OC users compared to free-cycling women (Brønnick et al., 2020). In sum, uncovering the impact of OC use needs intensive research on its own.

Methodological Constraints

Facial emotion recognition: The review consistently demonstrates the importance of stimulus features, namely valence of the stimulus material and/or emotion type. Facial emotion recognition (Table 2) varies with basic-emotion type (e.g., Derntl et al., 2008a, b, 2013; Guapo et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2013; Kamboj et al., 2015) and/or valence of complex emotions (positive, negative, [neutral]) (e.g., Pahnke et al., 2019; Shirazi et al., 2020). In basic- as well as in complex-emotion recognition, positive emotions are identified with more accuracy than the negative ones (for basic emotions: Derntl et al., 2008a, b, 2013; Guapo et al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2013; Kamboj et al., 2015; for complex emotions: Pahnke et al., 2019). Current research on facial emotion recognition is predominantly restricted to basic emotions (n = 15), and only seldom on complex emotions (n = 4, Hamstra et al., 2016, 2017 [also assessing basic emotions]; Pahnke et al., 2019; Shirazi et al., 2020). Studies using basic emotions (happiness, fear, sadness, anger, disgust, [neutral]) to assess emotion recognition accuracy rely on one positive and typically 4 negative emotions. Using only one positive emotion (happiness) possibly leads to emotion type (valence) main effect and impairs understanding the role of hormone status in emotion recognition. Therefore, balancing out and representing positive and negative emotions is important to achieve comparability and avoid ceiling effects in performance. Since basic emotions do not offer multiple positive emotions, using a variety of complex emotions should be considered to provide this balance (e.g., via Multifaceted Empathy Test; Dziobek et al., 2008; Drimalla et al., 2019). Moreover, increasing the number of items for both types, positive and negative emotions, would improve the reliability of the measurement. Furthermore, concerning the recognition of basic emotions (16 of 18 studies here), we suggest reporting the validity and reliability of test instruments. E.g., in the 16 basic-emotion studies here, 6 different measurement instruments were used, and data on reliability and validity are not available (except for a 160-item version of the Vienna Emotion Recognition Task, VERT, Hoheisel and Kryspin-Exner, 2005). This will help to achieve a higher standardization and comparability of studies.

Emotional memory: While the bias of enhanced memory for emotional vs. neutral content is well known (e.g., Cahill et al., 1994), stimulus valence interacted with hormone status, supporting the relevance to be addressed in the present research field. In emotional memory tasks, only nine of the 21 studies include all three valences (negative, positive, neutral). Moreover, stimulus material used in these studies also differs between videos, words, and IAPS pictures. Here standardization should lead to assessing all three valence types (negative, positive, and neutral). Notably, the emotional memory tasks varied across the studies in several procedural features (see: Table 4), i.e., valence type, number, and order of stimuli, but also the temporal delay between encoding and recall of the material (10 min to 1 week), placement and variation of other factors such as stressor, etc. This makes it difficult to interpret the results. For example, if ovarian-hormone modulation of emotional memory is found, ideally one would want to disentangle the effect. Is it a long-term memory or a short-term memory effect? More studies including an immediate, a short-delay, and a long-delay retention test would address this problem. This would also inform us if ovarian hormones are involved preferentially in encoding or consolidation of emotional material (or both). Further, including material in different modalities (e.g., pictures, written words, spoken words) in every study could inform us if ovarian-hormone influence is global or modality and domain-specific. Also, the differences in hormone-status-group definition across the studies of both facial emotion recognition and emotionally biased memory might just indicate that varying outcomes come from the procedural diversity. Thus, it is reasonable to conduct more replication studies, instead of modifying study designs constantly to provide novelty.

Quasi-Experimental Approach – Advantages and Further Suggestions

The quasi-experimental approach, as used in the vast majority of reviewed studies, holds the major advantage of natural physiological hormone fluctuations characteristic for the menstrual cycle (Gasbarri et al., 2012). However, it is worthwhile to choose cycle phases that represent a clear difference of E2 and P4 levels, i.e., by comparing the early follicular (low E2, P4) phase with the ovulatory (high E2) and with the mid-luteal (high P4, moderate E2) phase. OC users should be also included within the same study. Moreover, in the reviewed studies, hormone status was usually measured in a between-subject design by comparing subjects in different phases of the menstrual cycle (sometimes only self-reported and not validated with a hormone assessment). We strongly recommend considering the following points when comparing groups in different menstrual cycle phases: (1) proper tracking of the individual menstrual cycle in combination with hormonal assessment as recommended by Sundström-Poromaa and Gingnell (2014), (2) studying cycle phase effects preferably in a within-subject design, as it is fundamentally a within-subject process, and (3) defining narrower ranges within the follicular and luteal phase to account for the different combinations of E2 and P4 (Schmalenberger et al., 2021) (see Figure 3 for ranges used in reviewed studies). Even when all these three recommendations are met, comparing women in their natural cycle phases remains a quasi-experimental design that does not allow a causal interpretation of hormone-status effects since the subjects are not randomly assigned to the hormone-status groups. Thus, conducting experimental studies with administration of estradiol, progesterone, or a combination of both in the low-hormone windows during the menstrual cycle (i.e., in the early follicular phase) might be reasonable. E.g., exogenous E2 constituted a successful approach in fear-conditioning studies that revealed improved extinction recall (Graham and Milad, 2013).

Future Research Questions

Addressing the role of sex hormones on emotion recognition and emotional memory also entails clinical implications. Sex differences in prevalence rates of affective disorders suggest that shifts in gonadal hormones are partly a contributing factor. E.g., women (compared to men) are twice as likely to develop affective disorders, such as depression and posttraumatic stress disorder, even when keeping the traumatic event constant (e.g., combat exposure in female and male soldiers, Luxton et al., 2010), or after accounting for trauma types by each sex (Ney et al., 2019). Interestingly, the risk of developing such disorders increases around puberty with the rise of sex hormones (Paus et al., 2008), or in the premenstrual, postpartum, and perimenopausal phases with low ovarian-hormone levels (Rapkin et al., 2002). Thus, understanding the role of ovarian-hormone status in emotional processing would result in important clinical implications by identifying stages of heightened vulnerability for the development of affective disorders (Andreano et al., 2018). Therefore, more research is needed in this direction.

Moreover, both, emotional learning and memory on the one hand (e.g. LaBar and Cabeza, 2006), and tasks requiring empathy and social recognition share several brain areas, i.e., hippocampus, amygdala, and prefrontal cortex (mainly ventromedial prefrontal cortex [vmPFC] and anterior cingulate cortex [ACC]). E.g., overall emotion recognition in the follicular phase (Derntl et al., 2008b) was related to amygdala activation but also ventral regions of the prefrontal cortex activated during fear and anger. The amygdala is a major site for affective empathy, especially in women (Derntl et al., 2010), but empathic abilities also include frontotemporal, occipital regions and brainstem areas (Derntl et al., 2010). Areas specific for cognitive empathy-related processes, also labeled as the theory of mind, cover the ventral temporoparietal junction (Kanske et al., 2015). Also, experimental administration of progesterone was effective in increasing amygdala reactivity, reducing activity in the fusiform gyrus, and impairing functional connectivity between the amygdala and fusiform cortex (van Wingen et al., 2008). Imaging studies addressing resting-state activity and functional connectivity are thus of further interest to contribute to the identification of brain areas specifically relevant for emotion recognition and empathy. Therefore, we further argue that research on emotion recognition should be extended. We suggest examining both, cognitive and affective empathy. While cognitive aspects of empathy cover the ability to infer the mental states of others (e.g., by recognizing and responding to facial expressions of emotion) and are at least partly included when assessing emotion recognition, assessing affective aspects of empathy is comparably rare in the area of ovarian-hormone research. Such aspects involve the observer’s emotional response to another individual’s emotional state (Dziobek et al., 2008; Drimalla et al., 2019). Therefore, investigating cognitive and affective empathy in different hormone-status groups as well as in OC use can provide more insights into the role of the ovarian hormones in emotional processing.

Furthermore, examining the role of other bodily signals, such as hand gestures for emotion recognition (e.g., Jospe et al., 2020), in addition to social information from facial expressions of the counterpart could be interesting to examine in women in different cycle phases as well.

Lastly, there are very recent systematic reviews on the brain sites responding to naturally fluctuating sex hormones during the female menstrual cycle (Dubol et al., 2021) and under contraceptive use (Brønnick et al., 2020). It is now time to relate the identified action sites to specific areas of emotional processing such as emotion recognition and emotional memory. We also suggest addressing an even broader scope of areas of emotional processing covering peripheral-physiological reactivity (such as skin-conductance responses) and mood assessment.

Limitations of the Present Review

We decided to limit literature to healthy adult women in their reproductive age not exhibiting pregnancy or lactation. In contrast to other reviews on the role of ovarian hormones for selected emotion components, we here reviewed two emotion components, i.e., facial emotion recognition and emotional memory. In future reviews, it is worthwhile to consider additional age groups characterized by distinct concentrations of ovarian hormones, including puberty and menopause but also women during pregnancy and post-partum (see Osório et al., 2018 for facial emotion recognition). Moreover, other areas of emotional processing, i.e., emotion regulation, mood, fear and stress responses, and also reward sensitivity (Montoya and Bos, 2017) are of interest. Last but not least, clinical subgroups with anxiety and affective disorders could be also helpful to investigate. The underlying vulnerable affective states may demonstrate the role of ovarian hormones more intensely.

Conclusion

There is evidence that variation in ovarian-hormone status – as evident in different phases of the natural menstrual cycle (i.e., E2 and P4 levels) – affects facial emotion recognition and emotional memory, especially while interacting with stimulus features. The reports quite consistently point to a negativity bias across emotion components with more intrusions and better memory for negative content during the luteal phase. This could be a potential indicator of higher vulnerability to developing affective disorders under these hormonal conditions. Notably, the quasi-experimental approach in ovarian-hormone research hinders any causal interpretation. Thus, more studies using an experimental administration of E2 and P4 should be considered. There is also questionable evidence for OC-related impairments in facial emotion recognition. Studies on facial emotion recognition and emotional memory addressing the role of ovarian-hormone status are quite diverse in methodology. Therefore, methodological consistency regarding tasks, hormone status validation, cycle phase definition, etc., is important. Further, we suggest extending the scope of facial emotion-recognition research to focus on complex instead of basic emotions.

By addressing the role of ovarian hormones for emotion processing, we also would like to extend the concept of “embodied emotions” to a hormonal perspective. The traditional concept of embodied emotions uses the manipulation of sensory (Williams and Bargh, 2008, as a traditional study), and motor input (Strack et al., 1988, for the initial study) and demonstrates that these manipulations affect subsequent interpersonal judgments of stimulus material or mood (for review see: Niedenthal, 2007; Wiswede et al., 2009; Fuchs and Koch, 2014). Similarly, we here suggest that bidirectional communication between peripheral steroid hormones and brain receptors in areas of emotional processing (including the amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex) affects facial emotion recognition and emotional memory.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Author Contributions

DG: investigation, formal analysis, writing – original draft, writing – review and editing, and visualization. MA: writing – review and editing and supervision. US: conceptualization, formal analysis, writing – review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Gefördert durch die Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG) − Projektnummer GRK-2185/1 (DFG-Graduiertenkolleg Situated Cognition). Funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) − project number GRK-2185/1 (DFG Research Training Group Situated Cognition).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Andreano, J. M., Touroutoglou, A., Dickerson, B., and Barrett, L. F. (2018). Hormonal cycles, brain network connectivity, and windows of vulnerability to affective disorder. Trends Neurosci. 41, 660–676. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., and Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 241–251.

Google Scholar

Bayer, J., Schultz, H., Gamer, M., and Sommer, T. (2014). Menstrual-cycle dependent fluctuations in ovarian hormones affect emotional memory. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 110, 55–63. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Becker, J. B., Arnold, A. P., Berkley, K. J., Blaustein, J. D., Eckel, L. A., Hampson, E., et al. (2005). Strategies and methods for research on sex differences in brain and behavior. Endocrinology 146, 1650–1673. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-1142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brønnick, M. K., Økland, I., Graugaard, C., and Brønnick, K. K. (2020). The effects of hormonal contraceptives on the brain: a systematic review of neuroimaging studies. Front. Psychol. 11:556577. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.556577

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, R. A., Felmingham, K. L., Silove, D., Creamer, M., Donnell, M. O., and McFarlane, A. C. (2011). The association between menstrual cycle and traumatic memories. J. Affect. Disord. 131, 398–401. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.10.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., and McGaugh, J. L. (1994). β-Adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature 371, 702–704. doi: 10.1038/371702a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheung, J., Chervonsky, L., Felmingham, K. L., and Bryant, R. A. (2013). The role of estrogen in intrusive memories. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 106, 87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Connolly, H. L., Lefevre, C. E., Young, A. W., and Lewis, G. J. (2019). Sex differences in emotion recognition: evidence for a small overall female superiority on facial disgust. Emotion 19, 455–464. doi: 10.1037/emo0000446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, C. A., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Welling, L. L. M., Law Smith, M. J., Perrett, D. I., et al. (2007). Salience of emotional displays of danger and contagion in faces is enhanced when progesterone levels are raised. Horm. Behav. 51, 202–206. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dan, R., Canetti, L., Keadan, T., Segman, R., Weinstock, M., Bonne, O., et al. (2019). Sex differences during emotion processing are dependent on the menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology 100, 85–95. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.09.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derntl, B., Finkelmeyer, A., Eickhoff, S., Kellermann, T., Falkenberg, D. I., Schneider, F., et al. (2010). Multidimensional assessment of empathic abilities: neural correlates and gender differences. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35, 67–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derntl, B., Hack, R. L., Kryspin-Exner, I., and Habel, U. (2013). Association of menstrual cycle phase with the core components of empathy. Horm. Behav. 63, 97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2012.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derntl, B., Kryspin-Exner, I., Fernbach, E., Moser, E., and Habel, U. (2008a). Emotion recognition accuracy in healthy young females is associated with cycle phase. Horm. Behav. 53, 90–95. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Derntl, B., Windischberger, C., Robinson, S., Lamplmayr, E., Kryspin-Exner, I., Gur, R. C., et al. (2008b). Facial emotion recognition and amygdala activation are associated with menstrual cycle phase. Psychoneuroendocrinology 33, 1031–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drimalla, H., Landwehr, N., Hess, U., and Dziobek, I. (2019). From face to face: the contribution of facial mimicry to cognitive and emotional empathy. Cogn. Emot. 33, 1672–1686. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1596068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dubol, M., Epperson, C. N., Sacher, J., Pletzer, B., Derntl, B., Lanzenberger, R., et al. (2021). Neuroimaging the menstrual cycle: a multimodal systematic review. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 60:100878. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2020.100878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dziobek, I., Rogers, K., Fleck, S., Bahnemann, M., Heekeren, H. R., Wolf, O. T., et al. (2008). Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 464–473. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0486-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ertman, N., Andreano, J. M., and Cahill, L. (2011). Progesterone at encoding predicts subsequent emotional memory. Learn. Mem. 18, 759–763. doi: 10.1101/lm.023267.111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felmingham, K. L., Fong, W. C., and Bryant, R. A. (2012). The impact of progesterone on memory consolidation of threatening images in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 37, 1896–1900. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.03.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferree, N. K., and Cahill, L. (2009). Post-event spontaneous intrusive recollections and strength of memory for emotional events in men and women. Conscious. Cogn. 18, 126–134. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferree, N. K., Kamat, R., and Cahill, L. (2011). Influences of menstrual cycle position and sex hormone levels on spontaneous intrusive recollections following emotional stimuli. Conscious. Cogn. 20, 1154–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuchs, T., and Koch, S. C. (2014). Embodied affectivity: on moving and being moved. Front. Psychol. 5:508. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasbarri, A., Tavares, M. C. H., Rodrigues, R. C., Tomaz, C., and Pompili, A. (2012). Estrogen, cognitive functions and emotion: an overview on humans, non-human primates and rodents in reproductive years. Rev. Neuroscience. 23, 587–606. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, B. M., and Milad, M. R. (2013). Blockade of estrogen by hormonal contraceptives impairs fear extinction in female rats and women. Biol. Psychiatry 73, 371–378. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Guapo, V. G., Graeff, F. G., Zani, A. C. T., Labate, C. M., dos Reis, R. M., and Del-Ben, C. M. (2009). Effects of sex hormonal levels and phases of the menstrual cycle in the processing of emotional faces. Psychoneuroendocrinology 34, 1087–1094. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamstra, D. A., de Kloet, E. R., Quataert, I., Jansen, M., and van der Does, W. (2017). Mineralocorticoid receptor haplotype, estradiol, progesterone and emotional information processing. Psychoneuroendocrinology 76, 162–173.

Google Scholar

Hamstra, D. A., de Kloet, E. R., van Hemert, A. M., de Rijk, R. H., and van der Does, A. J. W. (2015). Mineralocorticoid receptor haplotype, oral contraceptives and emotional information processing. Neuroscience 286, 412–422. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamstra, D. A., de Rover, M., de Rijk, R. H., and van der Does, W. (2014). Oral contraceptives may alter the detection of emotions in facial expressions. Eur. Neuropsychopharm. 24, 1855–1859. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamstra, D. A., de Kloet, E. R., Tollenaar, M., Verkuil, B., Manai, M., Putman, P., et al. (2016). Mineralocorticoid receptor haplotype moderates the effects of oral contraceptives and menstrual cycle on emotional information processing. J. Psychopharmacol. 30, 1054–1061. doi: 10.1177/0269881116647504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hines, E. A., and Brown, G. E. (1936). The cold pressor test for measuring the reactibility of the blood pressure: data concerning 571 normal and hypertensive subjects. Am. Heart J. 11, 1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0002-8703(36)90370-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffmann, H., Kessler, H., Eppel, T., Rukavina, S., and Traue, H. C. (2010). Expression intensity, gender and facial emotion recognition: women recognize only subtle facial emotions better than men. Acta Psychol. 135, 278–283. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoheisel, B., and Kryspin-Exner, I. (2005). Emotionserkennung in Gesichtern und emotionales Gesichtergedächtnis. Zeitschrift Neuropsychol. 16, 77–87. doi: 10.1024/1016-264x.16.2.77

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jospe, K., Flöel, A., and Lavidor, M. (2020). The interactive effect of empathy and motor cortex stimulation on hand gesture comprehension. Neuropsychologia 141:107412. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2020.107412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung-Hoffman, C., and Kuhl, H. (1987). Divergent effects of two low-dose oral contraceptives on sex hormone-binding globulin and free testosterone. Am. J. Obstet. Gynecol. 156, 199–203. doi: 10.1016/0002-9378(87)90238-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamboj, S. K., Krol, K. M., and Curran, H. V. (2015). A specific association between facial disgust recognition and estradiol levels in naturally cycling women. PLoS One 10:e0122311. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0122311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F. M., and Singer, T. (2015). Dissecting the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain-behavior relations for empathy and Theory of Mind. Neuroimage 122, 6–19. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirschbaum, C., Pirke, K., and Hellhammer, D. (1993). The ‘Trier social stress test’ – A tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology 28, 76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhlmann, S., and Wolf, O. T. (2005). Cortisol and memory retrieval in women: influence of menstrual cycle and oral contraceptives. Psychopharmacology 183, 65–71. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0143-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LaBar, K. S., and Cabeza, R. (2006). Cognitive neuroscience of emotional memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 54–64. doi: 10.1038/nrn1825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (2008). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Affective Ratings of Pictures and Instruction Manual. Gainesville, FL: Technical Report A-8. University of Florida.

Google Scholar

Lewis, C. A., Kimmig, A.-C. S., Zsido, R. G., Jank, A., Derntl, B., and Sacher, J. (2019). Effects of hormonal contraceptives on mood: a focus on emotion recognition and reactivity, reward processing, and stress response. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 21:115. doi: 10.1007/s11920-019-1095-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luxton, D. D., Skopp, N. A., and Maguen, S. (2010). Gender differences in depression and PTSD symptoms following combat exposure. Depress. Anxiety 27, 1027–1033. doi: 10.1002/da.20730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maki, P., Mordecai, K., Rubin, L., Sundermann, E., Savarese, A., Eatough, E., et al. (2015). Menstrual cycle effects on cortisol responsivity and emotional retrieval following a psychosocial stressor. Horm. Behav. 74, 201–208. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maner, J. K., and Miller, S. L. (2014). Hormones and social monitoring: menstrual cycle shifts in progesterone underlie women’s sensitivity to social information. Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., and The PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 6:e1000097. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montoya, E. R., and Bos, P. A. (2017). How oral contraceptives impact social-emotional behavior and brain function. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 125–136. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mordecai, K. L., Rubin, L. H., Eatough, E., Sundermann, E., Drogos, L., Savarese, A., et al. (2017). Cortisol reactivity and emotional memory after psychosocial stress in oral contraceptive users. J. Neurosci. Res. 95, 126–135. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ney, L. J., Gogos, A., Ken Hsu, C. M., and Felmingham, K. L. (2019). An alternative theory for hormone effects on sex differences in PTSD: The role of heightened sex hormones during trauma. Psychoneuroendocrinology 109:104416. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Niedenthal, P. M. (2007). Embodying emotion. Science 316, 1002–1005. doi: 10.1126/science.1137651

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, S., Barber, S., Chai, A., Clewett, D., and Mather, M. (2015). Sympathetic arousal increases a negative memory bias in young women with low sex hormone levels. Psychoneuroendocrinology 62, 96–106. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, S. E., Ahmed, I., and Cahill, L. (2013b). Sex and menstrual cycle phase at encoding influence emotional memory for gist and detail. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 106, 56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, S. E., Ahmed, I., and Cahill, L. (2014). Post-learning stress differentially affects memory for emotional gist and detail in naturally cycling women and women on hormonal contraceptives. Behav. Neurosci. 128:482. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, S. E., Ertman, N., Lakhani, Y. S., and Cahill, L. (2011). Hormonal contraception usage is associated with altered memory for an emotional story. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 96, 378–384. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nielsen, S. E., Segal, S. K., Worden, I. V., Yim, I. S., and Cahill, L. (2013a). Hormonal contraception use alters stress responses and emotional memory. Biol. Psychol. 92, 257–266.

Google Scholar

Osório, F. L., de Paula Cassis, J. M., Machado de Sousa, J. P., Poli-Neto, O., and Martín-Santos, R. (2018). Sex hormones and processing of facial expressions of emotion: a systematic literature review. Front. Psychol. 9:529. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pahnke, R., Mau-Moeller, A., Junge, M., Wendt, J., Weymar, M., Hamm, A. O., et al. (2019). Oral contraceptives impair complex emotion recognition in healthy women. Front. Neurosci. 12:1041. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.01041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paus, T., Keshavan, M., and Giedd, J. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 947–957. doi: 10.1038/nrn2513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pearson, R., and Lewis, M. B. (2005). Fear recognition across the menstrual cycle. Horm. Behav. 47, 267–271. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petersen, N., Patihis, L., and Nielsen, S. E. (2015). Decreased susceptibility to false memories from misinformation in hormonal contraception users. Memory 23, 1029–1038. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2014.949777

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pompili, A., Arnone, B., D’Amico, M., Federico, P., and Gasbarri, A. (2016). Evidence of estrogen modulation on memory processes for emotional content in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology 65, 94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.12.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radke, S., and Derntl, B. (2016). Affective responsiveness is influenced by intake of oral contraceptives. Eur. Neuropsychopharm. 26, 1014–1019. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2016.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapkin, A. J., Mikacich, J. A., Moatakef-Imani, B., and Rasgon, N. (2002). The clinical nature and formal diagnosis of premenstrual, postpartum, and perimenopausal affective disorders. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 4, 419–428. doi: 10.1007/s11920-002-0069-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saylik, R., Raman, E., and Szameitat, A. J. (2018). Sex differences in emotion recognition and working memory tasks. Front. Psychol. 9:1072. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmalenberger, K. M., Tauseef, H. A., Barone, J. C., Owens, S. A., Lieberman, L., Jarczok, M. N., et al. (2021). How to study the menstrual cycle: Practical tools and recommendations. Psychoneuroendocrinology 123:104895. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2020.104895

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoofs, D., and Wolf, O. T. (2009). Stress and memory retrieval in women: no strong impairing effect during the luteal phase. Behav. Neurosci. 123, 547–554. doi: 10.1037/a0015625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sheaffer, B. L., Golden, J. A., and Averett, P. (2009). Facial expression recognition deficits and faulty learning: implications for theoretical models and clinical applications. Int. J. Behav. Consult. Ther. 5, 31–55. doi: 10.1037/h0100871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shirazi, T. N., Rosenfield, K. A., Cárdenas, R. A., Breedlove, S. M., and Puts, D. A. (2020). No evidence that hormonal contraceptive use or circulating sex steroids predict complex emotion recognition. Horm. Behav. 119:104647. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2019.104647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, E. M., Reynolds, S., Orchard, F., Whalley, H. C., and Chan, S. W. (2018). Cognitive biases predict symptoms of depression, anxiety and wellbeing above and beyond neuroticism in adolescence. J. Affect. Disord. 241, 446–453. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.08.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soni, M., Curran, V. H., and Kamboj, S. K. (2013). Identification of a narrow post-ovulatory window of vulnerability to distressing involuntary memories in healthy women. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 104, 32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2013.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spalek, K., Loos, E., Schicktanz, N., Hartmann, F., Quervain, D., Stier, C., et al. (2019). Women using hormonal contraceptives show increased valence ratings and memory performance for emotional information. Neuropsychopharmacology 44, 1258–1264. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0362-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strack, F., Martin, L. L., and Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: a nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54, 768–777. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.768

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundström-Poromaa, I. (2018). The menstrual cycle influences emotion but has limited effect on cognitive function. Vitam. Horm. 107, 349–376. doi: 10.1016/bs.vh.2018.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundström-Poromaa, I., and Gingnell, M. (2014). Menstrual cycle influence on cognitive function and emotion processing-from a reproductive perspective. Front. Neurosci. 8:380. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2014.00380

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sundström-Poromaa, I. S., and Segebladh, B. (2012). Adverse mood symptoms with oral contraceptives. Acta Obstet. Gynecol. Scand. 91, 420–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0412.2011.01333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toffoletto, S., Lanzenberger, R., Gingnell, M., Sundström-Poromaa, I., and Comasco, E. (2014). Emotional and cognitive functional imaging of estrogen and progesterone effects in the female human brain: a systematic review. Psychoneuroendocrinology 50, 28–52. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Wingen, G. A., van Broekhoven, F., Verkes, R. J., Petersson, K. M., Bäckström, T., Buitelaar, J., et al. (2008). Progesterone selectively increases amygdala reactivity in women. Mol. Psychiatr. 13, 325–333. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4002030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassell, J., Rogers, S., Felmingam, K., Pearson, J., and Bryant, R. (2015). Progesterone and mental imagery interactively predict emotional memories. Psychoneuroendocrinology 51, 1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, L. E., and Bargh, J. A. (2008). Experiencing physical warmth promotes interpersonal warmth. Science 322, 606–607. doi: 10.1126/science.1162548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingenbach, T. S. H., Ashwin, C., and Brosnan, M. (2018). Sex differences in facial emotion recognition across varying expression intensity levels from videos. PLoS One 13:e0190634. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0190634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wiswede, D., Münte, T. F., Krämer, U. M., and Rüsseler, J. (2009). Embodied emotion modulates neural signature of performance monitoring. PLoS One 4:e5754. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0005754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, R., Riedel, R., Sechrest, L., Lane, R. D., and Smith, R. (2018). Sex differences in emotion recognition ability: the mediating role of trait emotional awareness. Motiv. Emot. 42, 149–160. doi: 10.1007/s11031-017-9648-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, W., Zhou, R., and Ye, M. (2013). Menstrual cycle modulation of the late positive potential evoked by emotional faces. Percept. Mot. Ski. 116, 707–723. doi: 10.2466/22.27.PMS.116.3.707-723

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zoladz, P., Peters, D., Cadle, C., Kalchik, A., Aufdenkampe, R., Dailey, A., et al. (2015). Post-learning stress enhances long-term memory and differentially influences memory in females depending on menstrual stage. Acta Psychol. 160, 127–133. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional memory, emotion recognition, menstrual cycle, estrogen, progesterone, oral contraceptives, ovarian hormones, sex hormones

Citation: Gamsakhurdashvili D, Antov MI and Stockhorst U (2021) Facial Emotion Recognition and Emotional Memory From the Ovarian-Hormone Perspective: A Systematic Review. Front. Psychol. 12:641250. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.641250

Received: 13 December 2020; Accepted: 19 April 2021;
Published: 20 May 2021.

Edited by:

Frank A. Russo, Ryerson University, Canada

Reviewed by:

Birgit Derntl, University of Tübingen, Germany
Hubert Hannes Kerschbaum, University of Salzburg, Austria

Copyright © 2021 Gamsakhurdashvili, Antov and Stockhorst. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dali Gamsakhurdashvili, dgamsakhurda@uni-osnabrueck.de; Ursula Stockhorst, ursula.stockhorst@uni-osnabrueck.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.