Skip to main content
Log in

Mental Files and the Lexicon

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper presents the hypothesis that the representational repertoire underpinning our ability to process the lexical items of a natural language (that is, the mental lexicon) can be modeled as a system of mental files. To start, I clarify the basic phenomena that an account of lexical knowledge should be able to elucidate. Then, I propose to evaluate whether the mental files theory can be brought to bear on an account of the representational format of lexical knowledge by modeling mental words as recognitional files.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Alternative versions of MFT include Strawson (1974), Evans (1982), Bach (1987), and Perry (2001). A mental file φ is a mental representation that can typically be described on the basis of three elements: a reference-fixing relation R, a label L, and a content C. R is a relation between φ and some particular object O that φ is about (or to which φ refers). C is the information that φ associates to O. L is a term or a description used by the possessor of φ to designate O.

  2. The most significant analogue I can think of in psycholinguistics is the autonomous search model of Forster (1976), which sees word recognition as a process based on file-like access representations. However, the concept of ‘file’ at work in Forster’s model was developed independently from the notion of ‘file’ featuring in recent philosophical discussion.

  3. The mental lexicon can be broadly defined as the cognitive system hosting the long-term representations of the semantic, syntactic, morphological, and phonographic properties of the words mastered by a speaker of a natural language. Mental lexicons are thus constituents of Chomskyan I-languages (Chomsky 1986) and can be equated with the lexical component of an individual’s language capacity.

  4. To avoid confusion, it is perhaps useful to specify that my talk of ‘modalities’ does not refer to the usual distinction among sensory modalities (e.g., hearing and sight). Following a well-established practice in psycholinguistics, I call ‘modalities’ the types of linguistic input (syntactic, morphological, etc.) whereby an entry of the mental lexicon can be activated.

  5. A version of this idea can be found in Morton’s (1969) logogen model, or in the augmented addressed morphology developed by Caramazza et al. (1985) and Laudanna and Burani (1985). See also Tyler, Waksler and Marslen-Wilson (1993).

  6. The simplification is due to the fact that actual spoken word recognition is normally influenced by factors other than those involved in the analysis of the shape of acoustic stimuli. Agents use a combination of segmentation cues (e.g., syntax and stress pattern) to break up the auditory stream and identify the linguistic items conveyed in continuous speech (e.g., Sander and Neville 2000). However, this does not alter the fact that spoken word recognition can be viewed as a process mapping acoustic stimuli to invariant word-form categories (Dahan and Magnuson 2006; DeWitt and Rauschecker 2012).

  7. Usage-based approaches to lexical meaning are regarded by some as controversial. For instance, Borg’s (2012) organizational lexical semantics maintains that the linguistic content encoded by lexical types yields the semantic information required to build the truth conditions of well-formed declarative sentences under a minimal appeal to context and general-purpose abilities. I cannot do more than gesture toward this controversy here, but the reader should bear in mind that if a minimalist approach to lexical semantics turned out to be correct, the arguments presented in this paper would have to be substantially revised. Ludlow (2014) offers a valuable discussion of the advantages of a usage-based approach to lexical semantics.

  8. GoOpen(x) instructions emulate the GOTO (fc) instructions that are responsible for the recruitment and the update of topical cards in Vallduví’s (1992) work on file change semantics. For more details, see Hendriks and Dekker (1996), Vallduví and Engdahl (1996), and McNally (1998).

  9. Naturally, this is a simplified toy model. The polysemy of find is much wider and more complex than what I may seem to be suggesting here, even in cases where the verb is followed by a sentential complement introduced by that (e.g., “Upon further research, I found that he had been wrong all the time”).

References

  • Aitchison, J. 2012. Words in the mind: an introduction to the mental lexicon, 4th ed. London: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allott, N., and M. Textor. 2012. Lexical pragmatic adjustment and the nature of ad hoc concepts. International Review of Pragmatics 4: 185–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allwood, J. 2003. Meaning potentials and context: some consequences for the analysis of variation in meaning. In Cognitive approaches to lexical semantics, ed. H. Cuyckens, R. Dirven, and J. Taylor. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asher, N. 2011. Lexical meaning in context: a web of words. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bach, K. 1987. Thought and reference. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, M., and S. Kemmer (eds.). 2000. Usage-based models of language. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. 1999. Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 577–609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. 2003. Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language and Cognitive Processes 5: 513–562.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bormann, T., and C. Weiller. 2012. Are there lexicons?” a study of lexical and semantic processing in word-meaning deafness suggests “yes”. Cortex 48: 294–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., and A.E. Hillis. 1990. Where do semantic errors come from? Cortex 26: 95–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., G. Miceli, M.C. Silveri, and A. Laudanna. 1985. Reading mechanisms and the organization of the lexicon: evidence from acquired dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 2: 81–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, P. 2012. The philosophy of phonology. In Philosophy of linguistics, ed. R. Kempson, T. Fernando, and N. Asher. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. 1986. Knowledge of language. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Copestake, A., & Briscoe, T. (1996). Semi-productive polysemy and sense extension. In. J. Pustejovsky, & B. Boguraev (Eds.), Lexical semantics: The problem of polysemy. New York, NY: Clarendon Press.

  • Dahan, D., and J.S. Magnuson. 2006. Spoken word recognition. In Handbook of psycholinguistics, 2nd ed, ed. M. Traxler and M. Gernsbacher. London: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Brabanter, P. 2010. Constraints on metalinguistic anaphora. In Constraints in discourse 2, ed. P. Kühnlein, A. Benz, and C.L. Sidner. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denes, G. 2009. Talking heads: the neuroscience of language. Philadelphia: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeWitt, I., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 109, E505-E514.

  • Dilkina, K., J.L. McClelland, and D.C. Plaut. 2010. Are there mental lexicons? the role of semantics in lexical decision. Brain Research 1365: 66–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elman, J.L. 2011. Lexical knowledge without a lexicon? The Mental Lexicon 6: 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, G. 1982. The varieties of reference. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, V. 2009. How words mean: lexical concepts, cognitive models, and meaning construction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Fillmore, C. (1982). Frame semantics. In The Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm. Seoul: Hanshin Publishing.

  • Forster, K.I. 1976. Accessing the mental lexicon. In New approaches to language mechanisms, ed. F. Wales and E. Walker. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frauenfelder, U.H., and L.K. Tyler. 1987. The process of spoken word recognition: an introduction. Cognition 25: 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., M. Ahissar, R. Gotesman, and S. Tayeb. 2003. Are phonological effects fragile? The effect of luminance and exposure duration on form priming and phonological priming. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 346–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glanzberg, M. 2011. Meaning, concepts, and the lexicon. Croatian Journal of Philosophy 11: 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L., and A. Papafragou. 2013. Relations between language and thought. In The Oxford handbook of cognitive psychology, ed. D. Reisberg. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, A.E. 2006. Constructions at work: the nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldinger, S.D., P.A. Luce, and D.B. Pisoni. 1989. Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language 28: 501–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harder, P. 2009. Meaning as input: the instructional perspective. In New directions in cognitive linguistics, ed. V. Evans and S. Pourcel. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, M., K. McRae, and J.L. Elman. 2003. Sense and structure: meaning as a determinant of verb subcategorization preferences. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 281–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, H., and P. Dekker. 1996. Links without locations. In Proceedings of the tenth Amsterdam colloquium, ed. P. Dekker and M. Stokhof. Amsterdam: ILLC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herweg, M. 1992. Aspectual requirements of temporal connectives: evidence for a two-level approach to semantics. In Lexical semantics and knowledge representation, ed. J. Pustejovsky and S. Bergler. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jarema, G., and G. Libben. 2007. Introduction: matters of definition and core perspectives. In The mental lexicon: core perspectives, ed. G. Jarema and G. Libben. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemmerer, D., and D. Tranel. 2000. A double dissociation between linguistic and perceptual representations of spatial relationships. Cognitive Neuropsychology 17: 393–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., B. Dessalegn, and A.M. Goldberg. 2010. Language and space: momentary interactions. In Language, cognition and space: the state of the art and new directions, ed. P. Chilton and V. Evans. London: Equinox.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langacker, R.W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar, Vol. 1. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

  • Laudanna, A., and C. Burani. 1985. Address mechanisms to decomposed lexical entries. Linguistics 23: 775–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer, A., and E. Kittay (eds.). 1992. Frames, fields and contrasts, 21–74. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, B. 1993. English verb classes and alternations: a preliminary investigation. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ludlow, P. 2014. Living words: meaning underdetermination and the dynamic lexicon. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Marslen-Wilson, W., L.K. Tyler, R. Waksler, and L. Older. 1994. Morphology and meaning in the english mental lexicon. Psychological Review 101: 3–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNally, L. 1998. On the linguistic encoding of information packaging instructions. In The limits of syntax, ed. P. Culicover and L. McNally. San Diego: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, D.E., and R.W. Schvaneveldt. 1971. Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology 90: 227–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miceli, G., B. Benvegnù, R. Capasso, and A. Caramazza. 1997. The independence of phonological and orthographic lexical forms: evidence from aphasia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 14: 35–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. 1969. Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review 76: 165–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Munnich, E., B. Landau, and B.A. Dosher. 2001. Spatial language and spatial representation: a cross-linguistic comparison. Cognition 81: 171–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nickels, L. 1992. The autocue? Self-generated phonemic cues in the treatment of a disorder of reading and naming. Cognitive Neuropsychology 9: 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, J. 2001. Knowledge, possibility, and consciousness. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietroski, P. 2010. Concepts, meanings and truth: first nature, second nature and hard work. Mind & Language 25: 247–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pustejovsky, J. 1995. The generative lexicon. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, B., and M. Goldrick. 2006. Speaking words: contributions of cognitive neuropsychological research. Cognitive Neuropsychology 23: 39–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapp, B., L. Benzing, and A. Caramazza. 1997. The autonomy of lexical orthography. Cognitive Neuropsychology 14: 71–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayo, A. 2013. A plea for semantic localism. Noûs 47: 647–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Recanati, F. 2004. Literal meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Recanati, F. 2006. Indexical concepts and compositionality. In Two-dimensional semantics, ed. M. Garcia-Carpintero and J. Macia. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Recanati, F. 2012. Mental files. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sander, L.D., and H.J. Neville. 2000. Lexical, syntactic, and stress-pattern cues for speech segmentation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 43: 1301–1321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segui, J., and J. Grainger. 1990. Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: effects of relative prime target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 65–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, P.F. 1974. Subject and predicate in logic and grammar. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. 2000. Toward a cognitive semantics, vol. 2 Vols. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 2003. Constructing a language: a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, L.K., L. Waksler, and W.D. Marslen-Wilson. 1993. Representation and access of derived words in english. In Cognitive models of speech processing, ed. G. Altmann and R. Shillcock. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallduví, E. 1992. The information component. New York: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallduví, E., and E. Engdahl. 1996. The linguistic realization of information packaging. Linguistics 34: 459–519.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wetzel, L. 2009. Type and tokens: on abstract objects. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wurm, L.H., D.A. Vakoch, J. Aycock, and R.R. Childers. 2003. Semantic effects in lexical access: evidence from single-word naming. Cognition and Emotion 17: 547–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J. 2003. Polysemy or generality? Mu. In Cognitive approaches to lexical semantics, ed. H. Cuyckens, R. Dirven, and J. Taylor. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. 2004. The immersed experiencer: toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In The psychology of learning and motivation, vol. 44, ed. B.H. Ross. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to François Recanati, Michael Murez, Tomoo Ueda, and two anonymous reviewers for valuable input on the initial manuscript. The usual disclaimer applies. The research that led to this paper has received funding from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Program (FP7/2007-2013, MSCA-COFUND) under grant agreement No. 245743, Program Braudel-IFER-FMSH Program, in collaboration with the Institut Jean Nicod and the Labex IEC, ENS Paris.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luca Gasparri.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gasparri, L. Mental Files and the Lexicon. Rev.Phil.Psych. 7, 463–472 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-015-0262-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-015-0262-3

Keywords

Navigation