Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 30 May 2017
Sec. Brain Health and Clinical Neuroscience
Volume 11 - 2017 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2017.00285

Age-Related Changes in Resting-State EEG Activity in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Cross-Sectional Study

  • 1Laboratory of Neuroplasticity, Department of Molecular and Cellular Neurobiology, Nencki Institute of Experimental Biology of Polish Academy of Sciences, Warsaw, Poland
  • 2Gösta Ekman Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 3Department of Psychology, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Warsaw, Poland
  • 4Department of Child Psychiatry, Medical University of Warsaw, Warsaw, Poland

Numerous studies indicate that attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is related to some developmental trends, as its symptoms change widely over time. Nevertheless, the etiology of this phenomenon remains ambiguous. There is a disagreement whether ADHD is related to deviations in brain development or to a delay in brain maturation. The model of deviated brain development suggests that the ADHD brain matures in a fundamentally different way, and does not reach normal maturity at any developmental stage. On the contrary, the delayed brain maturation model assumes that the ADHD brain indeed matures in a different, delayed way in comparison to healthy age-matched controls, yet eventually reaches proper maturation. We investigated age-related changes in resting-state EEG activity to find evidence to support one of the alternative models. A total of 141 children and teenagers participated in the study; 67 diagnosed with ADHD and 74 healthy controls. The absolute power of delta, theta, alpha, and beta frequency bands was analyzed. We observed a significant developmental pattern of decreasing absolute EEG power in both groups. Nonetheless, ADHD was characterized by consistently lower absolute EGG power, mostly in the theta frequency band, in comparison to healthy controls. Our results are in line with the deviant brain maturation theory of ADHD, as the observed effects of age-related changes in EEG power are parallel but different in the two groups.

Introduction

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most commonly diagnosed neurodevelopmental disorders, with reported prevalence rates of 3–7% in school-aged children (Scahill and Schwab-Stone, 2000; Burd et al., 2003; Polanczyk and Jensen, 2008). ADHD is characterized by a pattern of developmentally inappropriate levels of inattention, impulsivity, and overactivity (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Moreover, it is frequently related to severe co-morbidities such as oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) or conduct disorder (CD), poor academic performance, and socioeconomic problems (Biederman et al., 2006).

A growing body of research on the causes of behavioral symptoms in ADHD provides evidence for impairments on genetic, neurotransmission, neuroanatomic and functional levels (for review see, Kuntsi et al., 2006), the etiology still remains unknown. As the deficits observed in ADHD may be diverse, it is regarded as a heterogeneous, multidimensional disorder, and may have multiple causes. One of the most prominent hypotheses states that ADHD results from altered brain maturation. However, there is a disagreement whether ADHD is related to a delay in brain maturation (Mann et al., 1992; Rubia, 2007; Shaw et al., 2007) or whether ADHD’s brain maturation process represents complete deviation from the typical development (Chabot and Serfontein, 1996; Dickstein et al., 2006).

Delayed brain maturation is mainly indicated by the results from structural brain imaging studies. Two longitudinal studies investigating large samples of children demonstrated that ADHD individuals follow a similar to normal sequential pattern of cortical thickening. However, it is delayed for about 2–3 years depending on the specific cortex region (Shaw et al., 2007, 2012), with largest delays observed in the prefrontal cortex.

Conversely, the findings from electroencephalographic (EEG) studies indicate persistent abnormalities in ADHDs’ EEG activity (for review see, Barry et al., 2003). For example, Chabot and Serfontein (1996) reported increased absolute and relative theta activity in frontal and frontal midline regions. Other research reported decreased relative alpha activity over parietal and temporal sites (Bresnahan and Barry, 2002; Poil et al., 2014), decreased absolute and relative beta activity in frontal, parietal, and temporal sites (Hobbs et al., 2007), and elevated theta/beta (TBR) and theta/alpha ratios (TAR) (Hermens et al., 2005).

Elevated theta power and TBR are the most frequently reported EEG abnormalities in ADHD and have been interpreted as indices of immature brain activity. Some researchers proposed the aforementioned indices as a biomarker of ADHD (Magee et al., 2005; Snyder and Hall, 2006). However, they did not obtain clinical acceptance and use (Lenartowicz and Loo, 2014; Loo et al., 2016). Moreover, recent studies failed to replicate the effect of elevated theta and TBR in ADHD (Loo et al., 2009, 2013; van Dongen-Boomsma et al., 2010; Liechti et al., 2013). A meta-analysis revealed that TBR effect size is negatively related to the year of publication, methodological factors, inclusion and exclusion criteria, and a decreasing trend in sleep duration across time were proposed as possible explanations for this observation (Arns et al., 2013).

Interestingly, despite the importance of age-related effects for the understanding of ADHD, all of the mentioned EEG results come from quasi-experimental studies that contrasted the EEG indices of younger and older participants. To the best of our knowledge, there are no longitudinal or cross-sectional studies focused on tracking developmental trends in EEG activity using a regression approach. A study by Clarke et al. (2001) investigated differences in two subtypes of ADHD. They reported that the differences in power between ADHD inattentive and control groups remained constant with increasing age (from 8 to 12 years old). However, power in the ADHD combined group changed at a greater rate than in the ADHD inattentive group, with power of the two ADHD groups becoming similar with age. Additionally, sex differences between the ADHD and control subjects in all EEG absolute power measures were more pronounced in males and matured faster in males in comparison to females.

The developmental trends of EEG activity in healthy participants have been reported widely for decades (Matousek and Petersen, 1973; Barriga-Paulino et al., 2011). The index of this trend is a decrease in the power of all frequency bands, with the most prominent decrease in low frequencies: delta and theta (for a review see, Segalowitz et al., 2010; Uhlhaas et al., 2010). Maturational EEG power decrease is observed globally over all scalp sites (Whitford et al., 2007). The exact physiological process underpinning this effect remains unclear. A few recent studies tried to address this problem using simultaneous EEG recordings and MRI scanning. One of them observed that the age-related decrease of EEG power was correlated with a decline in gray matter density (Whitford et al., 2007), which has been interpreted as a reduction of neuropil and elimination of active synapses. The latter may be responsible for the age-related EEG power reduction.

Both imaging evidence on the delayed cortical maturation in ADHD and the well-established age-related changes in EEG activity in healthy participants inspired our study. The main goal of the project was to study the extent to which developmental pattern of resting EEG activity is similar in ADHD and typical development. For that purpose, we investigated a large sample (141 participants) with a broad age range (9–16 years), allowing us to consider age as a continuous predictor in linear regression models. Linear regression analysis has been previously used to detect EEG developmental tendencies (Wackermann and Matousek, 1998), however, not in the context of ADHD and therefore we expect this analysis to reveal different developmental patterns in EEG between ADHD and healthy participants.

Materials and Methods

A total of 150 children and teenagers (Caucasian) aged 9–16 participated in the study. Due to technical issues (five ADHD participants) and excessive motion artifacts (two ADHD participants, two controls), we included 141 participants in the final analysis. The clinical group consisted of 67 participants with an ADHD diagnosis (Mage = 13.11 ± 2.06; 12 females). The healthy control group was age- and sex- matched to the clinical group (n = 74, Mage = 13.17 ± 2.2; 12 females).

The clinical group was recruited among ADHD outpatients under the supervision of the Psychiatry Clinic at Public Pediatric Teaching Hospital in Warsaw, Poland. The diagnosis was performed according to diagnostic criteria of the DSM-IV TR (4th edition, text revision; American Psychiatric Association, 2000) for ADHD subtypes: predominantly inattentive, impulsive/hyperactive, and combined. The diagnostic process was conducted at the clinic by a trained and experienced team of child psychiatrists and psychologists as previously described (Hanć et al., 2014; Racicka et al., 2015), and included: an interview with patients’ parents, Diagnostic Structured Interview for ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder according to ICD-10 and DSM-IV TR (Wolańczyk and Kołakowski, 2005), the Behavioral Disorders Supplement of Diagnostic Interview Kiddie-SADS-Present and Lifetime Version (Kiddie-SADS-PL), and observation of patients’ behavior. Patients’ parents provided background records and school reports. Both parents and teachers completed the behavior rating scales (Wolańczyk and Kołakowski, 2005). ADHD symptoms intensity was rated using ADHD Rating Scale (ADHD-RS, DuPaul et al., 1998). The clinical group exhibit following symptoms intensity as measured with ADHD-RS: total score (M = 38.47, SD = 9.74), inattention (M = 21.08, SD = 3.82), hyperactivity (M = 8.98, SD = 4.42), impulsivity (M = 8.42, SD = 3.76). The comorbidity diagnosis was based on the diagnostic criteria for ICD-10 (World Health Organization, 1994), and the diagnosis was verified during no less than three appointments. If the results of all diagnostic methods were consistent, the diagnosis was confirmed, and such patients were invited to take part in our study. The ADHD group consisted of children diagnosed with combined (n = 47) or predominantly inattentive ADHD subtypes (n = 20). The comorbidity distribution was as follows: 66% of participants manifested comorbidities, 47% of which were diagnosed with ODD/ODC+CD, and 25.7% had learning difficulties.

The inclusion criteria for our ADHD group were: a confirmed diagnosis, no previous head injuries with a loss of consciousness, and a lack of somatic disorders and epilepsy. ADHD participants were asked to abstain from taking stimulant medication at least 24 h before testing.

A healthy control group was recruited from among Warsaw’s primary and secondary schools through advertisement at school-meetings with parents. During such a meeting the child’s parents, if interested in participating in the study, completed a questionnaire providing detailed information about their child’s health condition. It was used to select participants who did not report any attentional problems, neurological or psychiatric diagnosis, dyslexia/dysorthography, previous brain injury with the loss of consciousness, or close family members (parents, siblings) with ADHD/ADD diagnosis.

The study was approved by the local Ethics Committee at the SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities and the Medical University of Warsaw. All participants provided assent, and parents gave informed written consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

EEG Recording and Preprocessing

Resting-state EEG was recorded using a 64-channel EGI HydroCel Geodesic System (Eugen, OR, United States). The signal was sampled at 250 Hz and referenced to the Cz electrode. Electrode impedances were kept below 20 kΩ. We recorded 5 min of resting-state EEG with eyes closed (3 min) and open (2 min), alternating between the two with 1 min long sequences. The eyes open condition was introduced to prevent participants from drowsing. Following analyses were performed on signal registered while the participants had their eyes closed only. EEG data was processed offline using EEGlab software (Delorme and Makeig, 2004) and custom MATLAB scripts.

We created two datasets for each EEG file. Both datasets were 45 Hz low pass and 1 Hz high pass filtered using a Hamming windowed sinc FIR filter (transition bandwidth: 1 Hz) implemented in EEGLAB’s pop_eegfiltnew function. One dataset was then divided into 1-s epochs prior to independent component analysis (ICA) and visually inspected for artifacts. We removed epochs with large irregular events. Noisy electrodes (large visible drifts or net noise) were interpolated. Such short (1 s) epochs allowed for keeping more data to perform the ICA on, but were not ideal for our planned spectral analysis. Therefore, we ran the ICA on the first dataset and subsequently applied the obtained ICA weights to the second dataset, which we later epoched into 3 s long epochs. We used the runica algorithm as implemented in the eeglab pop_runica function. We identified ICA components related to eye blinks and horizontal eye movements by visual inspection, based on their scalp topography, power spectra, time course, and variance among trials (Hipp and Siegel, 2013). These eye components were removed from the signal (4.1 ± 1.84 on average). Next, we re-referenced the data to an average reference and visually inspected the datasets for any remaining artifacts, removing contaminated epochs. The average number of remaining epochs per participant was 50 ± 9.3 (out of 60). All the steps mentioned above were done using a custom MATLAB toolbox (in prep) and EEGLAB functions.

EEG Data Analysis

We calculated a mean power spectrum for every participant using EEGLAB’s pop_spectopo function. Absolute EEG power was log transformed in order to normalize the distribution of the data for statistical analysis. All further analyses were conducted using RStudio Team (2015).

We divided our data into regions of interests with regard to anterior–posterior and left–right localization on the scalp. This division resulted in nine clusters (Figure 1): three anteriori (left, middle, right), three central (left, middle, right), and three posterior (left, middle, right). Each participant’s power estimates were averaged across electrodes within each cluster and across frequencies within the following four frequency bands: delta (1–3 Hz), theta (3–8 Hz), alpha (8–12 Hz), and beta (12–30 Hz). Before computing the aggregates, values differing more than 2.24 median absolute deviations from the median were removed (Wilcox and Keselman, 2003).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Cluster division and location of electrodes used for analysis.

Age, ADHD, and EEG Power

To test the combined effect of age and ADHD diagnosis on EEG power spectrum, we performed a regression analysis using R’s lm() function. Regression analyses were performed for each cluster and frequency band and included age, ADHD diagnosis, and their interaction. To explore the potential moderating role of the symptoms intensity we estimated a series of regression models in the ADHD group including ADHD-RS scores (total score or one of the three subscale scores) as moderators of age effects. To correct for multiple comparisons, we employed a false discovery rate (FDR) (Benjamini and Hochberg, 1995) within each frequency band using R’s p.adjust function (RStudio Team, 2015) with the p < 0.05 threshold.

Results

None of the regression models estimated for all participants revealed a significant interaction effect of Age and ADHD diagnosis (all p-values for the interaction term > 0.14, all ΔR2 < 1.3%). Therefore, we decided to report detailed results of the simpler models, including only main effects of age and ADHD (Table 1). As the effects of both predictors were additive, we discuss them in turn.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Linear regression analysis for the resting EEG predicted by age and diagnosis.

The obtained results revealed a typical developmental effect of decreasing absolute EEG power with increasing age. Absolute EEG power was found to decrease linearly for delta, theta, and beta frequency bands across each of the scalp regions: anterior, central, and posterior in both groups (Figure 2A). Age-related changes in absolute power were much more pronounced in slow-wave frequencies, i.e., delta and theta (Figure 3), than in beta. The increase in the proportion of explained variance attributable to the effects of age varied across frequencies and clusters. For delta and theta ΔR2 was within 9.3–23.3% range, whereas for beta frequency it was more than five times smaller, ranging from 1.3 to 6.2%. The alpha absolute power was not significantly related to age.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Linear regression analysis of the resting EEG predicted by age and diagnosis. Beta coefficient maps for two main effects (age and diagnosis) are plotted respectively for all frequency bands. (A) Represents beta coefficients for age main effect, (B) represents beta coefficients for diagnosis effect. Significant clusters (p < 0.05, uncorrected) are marked with dots in cluster-corresponding colors. Star signs indicate significant clusters after FDR correction.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Scatter plots illustrating the main effects of age and diagnosis observed for delta and theta frequency bands over three posterior clusters (from left: left, middle, right).

Additionally, we observed the main effect of diagnosis, indicating differences between groups. The ADHD group had significantly lower absolute power in all frequency bands across centro-posterior clusters (Figure 2B), with the most pronounced difference in lower theta absolute power.

Additional analysis of the moderating effects of symptoms intensity on power spectra age-related changes in the ADHD group did not reveal any significant effects involving ADHD-RS scores in none of the models (all uncorrected p-values > 0.10).

Discussion

The aim of this study was to investigate age-related changes in eyes-closed resting EEG activity in children and teenagers diagnosed with ADHD, in comparison to healthy controls. For that purpose, we included a large sample of participants across a broad age range and used regression analysis as a statistical tool. Such an approach allowed us to consider age as a continuous variable and obtain a more detailed view of the pace of the developmental changes in resting EEG in both studied groups. Our results did not reveal any age and ADHD interactions, suggesting that the rate of EEG power decrease is similar in ADHD and healthy participants across all regions and frequency bands. This pattern of results is in stark contrast with the delayed maturation hypothesis and suggests that age and presence of the disorder have an additive influence on resting EEG, which is congruent with the deviated brain activity maturation viewpoint.

We replicated the commonly reported EEG developmental pattern in healthy participants. Moreover, age-related changes in resting EEG activity were evident in both healthy and ADHD groups of children. EEG power underwent linear reductions with age, with the most prominent decreases of absolute power in the slow-wave frequencies, i.e., delta and theta, which is in line with the results of previous studies on healthy cohorts (Gibbs and Knott, 1949; Matousek and Petersen, 1973; Matsuura et al., 1985; Gasser et al., 1988; Somsen et al., 1997; Dustman et al., 1999; Boord et al., 2007). As aforementioned, the EEG activity developmental pattern has been linked to structural brain maturation expressed in significant gray matter tissue loss, possibly resulting from significant elimination of synapses (Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997) and reduction in the neuropil (Purves, 1998; Whitford et al., 2007). These processes are thought to improve the efficiency of information processing, cognitive capacity, and executive functions (Kharitonova et al., 2013; Tamnes et al., 2013; Zhong et al., 2014). Neuroimaging studies have shown that the decrease in cortical gray matter thickness has substantial regional variation (Salat et al., 2005). Moreover, this development-related process is a combination of regionally specific cortical thinning in the cortical sulci and cortical thickening on the gyri (Vandekar et al., 2015). In pathologies, such as schizophrenia and ADHD, deficiencies of the expected cortical thinning during adolescence have been observed (Bakalar et al., 2009; Shaw et al., 2013). ADHD participants have been demonstrated to have a thinner cortex than controls in extensive areas of the brain, primarily the medial area (with the anterior cingulum included), superior prefrontal and precentral cortex (Shaw et al., 2006; Narr et al., 2009), and temporal lobe (Langevin et al., 2015) the surface of cortical areas was reported to be reduced in comparison to healthy participants as well (Shaw et al., 2012; for the review see, Kasparek et al., 2015). There is no direct evidence for the altered synaptic pruning that might be related to deficits in cortical thickness in ADHD. However, deficient pruning has been implicated in explanations of neuropsychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia and autism (Penzes et al., 2011). Moreover, clinical, neuroimaging, and animal studies indicate that dopamine neurotransmission is dysregulated in ADHD. In a non-human primate model, the decrease in prefrontal pyramidal cell spine density associated with profound cognitive dysfunction has been shown to be caused by dopamine hyperstimulation (Tripp and Wickens, 2009; Genro et al., 2010).

As EEG activity is mainly driven by the synchronized synaptic activity at the cortex, a reduction in the number of synapses in the cortex has been proposed to lead to decreases in slow-wave delta and theta, but not high-frequencies of alpha or beta (Jenni and Carskadon, 2004; Whitford et al., 2007). The age in which the maximum dendritic spine density on pyramidal neurons occurs (Petanjek et al., 2011) correspond perfectly with the median age by which the peak thickness of the cortex is attained (Shaw et al., 2007), and a tight correlation between slow-wave activity and a variety of indices of cortical maturation derived from MR has been shown (Kurth et al., 2010; Buchmann et al., 2011). According to this view, the higher number of synapses during childhood could explain the high power of delta and theta oscillations. However, the process of synaptic pruning cannot be an explanation for the beta power decrease, since it presumably arises from asynchronous activity between pyramidal neurons (Whitford et al., 2007). On the other hand, the alpha frequency is thought to be driven by thalamic activity and therefore might be affected by structural changes in subcortical regions rather than cortical ones. This might explain the lack of the age-related decrease in alpha power in our study. Therefore, lower gray matter thickness implying a lower number of synapses in ADHD in comparison with healthy participants should result in lower delta and theta absolute power. However, contrarily, increased delta and theta power, mainly in the frontal regions, has been reported in ADHD in the vast majority of previous studies (for review see, Barry et al., 2003). We observed lower EEG power in all frequency bands in centro-posterior clusters in ADHD participants in comparison to healthy controls. The most prominent differences referred to decreased delta and theta in the ADHD group. This pattern of results, even though counterintuitive, is not unique in studies on the developmental changes in ADHD. For example, Shaw et al. (2011) reported age-related decrease in cortical thickness where, at a certain age, results of older controls match the gray matter thickness observed in younger ADHD children.

Nonetheless, our results, especially those for theta power, are in contrast to the previous results indicating elevated levels of theta power and theta/beta ratio (Chabot and Serfontein, 1996; Bresnahan et al., 1999; Koehler et al., 2009). However, more recent studies failed to replicate these effects (Loo et al., 2009; Ogrim et al., 2012; Liechti et al., 2013; Poil et al., 2014), showing no significant differences between ADHD and healthy participants in theta power. What is more, Liechti et al. (2013) noted the tendency of decreased relative theta and theta/beta ratio for ADHD, but the results did not reach significance. Instead, observed decreased power of alpha and beta was a commonly reported effect (for review see, Dykman et al., 1982; Barry et al., 2003; Clarke et al., 2008), also in the posterior regions (for review see, Lazzaro et al., 1998; Barry et al., 2003), and linked to cortical hypoarousal. Moreover, the high heterogeneity of ADHD in terms of EEG sub-types has been implicated by Clarke et al. (2011), who revealed five different groups on the basis of EEG characteristics. Two of the groups were characterized by decreased theta activity, which is similar to our observation. Patients in one of these groups were more likely to be aggressive than other ADHD children; patients of the other group presented autistic-like behaviors. ADHD is a highly comorbid disorder. More than 60% of ADHD patients exhibit one or more of the following co-existing disorders: 42–90% ODD and/or CD, 13–51% anxiety or depression, and 20–25% learning difficulties (Angold et al., 1999; Pliszka, 2000; Jensen et al., 2001; Bauermeister et al., 2007). Our ADHD group consisted of resident patients of one clinic, who were diagnosed according to both DSM-IV and ICD-10 diagnostic criteria using structuralized methods (Wolańczyk and Kołakowski, 2005) that involved interviews with parents and the observation of the patients’ behavior. Such an extensive process of diagnosis allowed us to include only children with a widely confirmed diagnosis. What is more, the distribution of comorbidities in our sample was close to those previously reported. That allows us to consider our groups as highly representative. It should be noted that most of the studies used different recruiting criteria, e.g., excluding children with comorbidities. This may be beneficial for the interpretation of results, however, such a group is not representative of the usual clinical outcomes.

It should be noted that Clarke et al. (2011) observed decreased theta power in the fronto-central regions, while we observed decreases in slow-waves absolute power in the centro-posterior clusters. The ADHD behavioral heterogeneity (subtypes and comorbid disorders) is likely linked with neurophysiological heterogeneity (diverse changes in brain morphology). Indeed, different studies reported different changes in various brain areas in ADHD (for review see, Kasparek et al., 2015) and this variability might be related to ADHD heterogeneity. Moreover, it is possible that regional variation in the development-related decrease in cortical gray matter thickness and regionally specific cortical thinning and thickening in sulci and gyri reported for healthy participants is not accurately followed in ADHD patients. Putatively, this is reflected in diverse EEG subtypes. Up to nine EEG subtypes have been proposed in children with and without ADHD (Chabot and Serfontein, 1996; Arns et al., 2008; Clarke et al., 2011; Johnstone et al., 2013) and this might explain differences between studies due to the uneven sampling of these subgroups.

Arns et al. (2013) conducted a meta-analysis on TBR in ADHD and found that heterogeneity among studies was very high. Studies varied on characteristics and factors such as study design, EEG technology, type of analysis, age of participants studied, sample size, medicating history, and whether or not participants diagnosed with co-morbidities were included. Moreover, in addition to differences between studies in characterizing EEG activity, studies of ADHD are also confounded by the high behavioral heterogeneity of this disorder.

The differences in our observations in comparison to previously published findings may also result from the application of a different methodology. First of all, we applied different statistical testing. We used regression analysis instead of comparing mean EEG power of each tested group with the others groups. We investigated a large group of children and teenagers in a wide age-range instead of comparing young participants to adults. Additionally, we applied different methods to analyze EEG activity. We used a different data cleaning approach, removing artifacts both manually and by excluding ICA components. The analysis was performed on clusters instead of testing the effects on single electrodes, which reduces the risk of interpreting noise rather than actual brain activity. The time of the recordings was also different; our recordings last for 5 min, and we applied a procedure is intended to mitigate drowsiness (eyes closed/eyes open). Most of the previous research adopted 20 min-long recordings.

Our analysis did not reveal any significant effects of interaction. Instead, we observed two main effects of development and group differences. The lack of interaction (a significant interaction would have provided evidence in favor of the developmental lag hypothesis) speaks for the presence of a permanent abnormality of EEG, at least within the examined time range. We observed the same pattern of age-related decreases in EEG power, particularly in slow wave activity, in both groups, whereas the difference between groups was stable in time; the EEG power of ADHD participants was always lower.

Taking into consideration the EEG-heterogeneity of ADHD and the structural differences reported in MRI studies, we assume that the lower EEG power may result from abnormally diminished gray matter volume in ADHD. Different trajectories of age-related decrease in cortical thickness in control and ADHD youths were reported where, at some point of time, results of older controls match the thickness observed in younger ADHD patients (Shaw et al., 2011). This might reflect two fundamentally different neurodevelopmental effects resulting in the observable changes in a single measured parameter. Nevertheless, there is a lack of the studies using simultaneous MRI/EEG recordings in ADHD, which would confirm our assumption.

Several alternatives to the explanations of decreasing EEG power with age based on reductions in cortical gray matter density have been proposed previously. For example, absolute metabolic rates were found to parallel gray matter maturation and maturational decrease in electrical activity (Feinberg et al., 1990; Boord et al., 2007; Feinberg and Campbell, 2010). Also, it has been shown that developmental global EEG low frequency power decrease is paralleled by global BOLD signal power decrease (Lüchinger et al., 2012). All of these global decreases in structural and metabolic markers, as well as global EEG and BOLD signal power decreases, indicate profound neuronal reorganization with age. Another explanation is that the spectral density of slow wave activity in resting EEG is largely determined by resonant thalamo-cortical loops (Llinás et al., 2005; Miskovic et al., 2015). Thalamic and thalamo-cortical loop maturation was implicated by EEG–MRI coupling and normalized BOLD power (Lüchinger et al., 2012). In this study performed in adults, adolescents and children it has been demonstrated that developmental EEG amplitude reductions match those found for the same age range with magnetoencephalography (MEG) (Lüchinger et al., 2012). The fact that MEG is insensitive to changes in physical skull properties (Puligheddu et al., 2005) corroborates neurodevelopmental nature of the observed effects. Therefore, we do not believe that the EEG effects in our study are strongly influenced by differences in physical properties of the head (e.g., skull thickness), which change during development. Based on the results obtained in the study with adult subjects it has been postulated that skull thickness may be neglected as a source of error in such cases (Hagemann et al., 2008). Moreover, the relationship between age and gamma revealed much stronger effect (Tierney et al., 2013) than an effect between skull thickness and resting EEG power (Hagemann et al., 2008). We cannot exclude, that developmental differences in skull thickness or other physical characteristics of the head could influence the age-related changes in EEG power spectra. However, according to Tierney et al. (2013) data, factors related to maturation explain more variance in EEG activity than differences in these physical factors do.

Several limitations of this study should be noted. Firstly, the cross-sectional character of this experiment restricts the possibility of obtaining a more dynamic overview of developmental changes in EEG activity. The younger group of subjects in our study might possibly be more heterogeneous than the older group, which may influence the results. Older ADHD children presented a persistent neurodevelopmental deficit in our study, whereas some of the younger participants might be expected to remit. Certainly, the cause and effect pathway could be established more reliably in a longitudinal study. There is a need to conduct longitudinal studies on EEG activity in healthy as well as clinical cohorts, which may provide valuable information on the dynamics of the alternations observed in ADHD and may have an impact on treatment approaches, e.g., stimulant and neurofeedback therapies. Secondly, the clinical group consisted of both combined and predominantly inattentive subtypes of ADHD. We found it highly representative of a typical ADHD population. The distribution of the ADHD subtypes and comorbidities in our sample was similar to that previously reported for ADHD cohorts. However, the “inattentive” subgroup was significantly smaller which did not allow us to investigate it in comparison to the “combined” and control groups. Also, the number of the female participants was too small to obtain valuable information on sex-differences.

Conclusion

This study revealed that the resting-EEG developmental pattern was similar in ADHD and healthy controls. Even so, the ADHD group had consistently lower absolute EGG power, mostly in the theta frequency band. Our results are in line with deviant brain maturation hypothesis, as ADHD brain activity would not be considered the same as in healthy controls at any developmental stage, at least within the studied age range (9–16 years).

Author Contributions

KG recruited control group of participants, carried out all of the study, performed the recordings, analyses, wrote the manuscript, prepared table and figures. MZ had substantial contribution to the analysis of the data and figures preparation. MB had substantial contribution to the statistical analyses of the data. ER-P recruited and diagnosed the ADHD participants. MK made comments and suggestions on the manuscript. AC-K had contribution to the conception of the work, interpretation of the data, and in writing the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by National Science Centre Poland Grant 2011/01/D/NZ4/04958 and National Science Centre Poland Grant 2015/17/N/HS6/03020.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgment

We thank Prof. A. Brzezicka, Prof. T. Wolanczyk, and Prof. W. Oniszczenko for their helpful scientific comments and suggestions.

References

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

Google Scholar

Angold, A., Costello, E. J., and Erkanli, A. (1999). Comorbidity. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 40, 57–87. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00424

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arns, M., Conners, C. K., and Kraemer, H. C. (2013). A decade of EEG theta/beta ratio research in ADHD: a meta-analysis. J. Atten. Disord. 17, 374–383. doi: 10.1177/1087054712460087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arns, M., Gunkelman, J., Breteler, M., and Spronk, D. (2008). EEG phenotypes predict treatment outcome to stimulants in children with ADHD. J. Integr. Neurosci. 7, 421–438. doi: 10.1142/S0219635208001897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakalar, J. L., Greenstein, D. K., Clasen, L., Tossell, J. W., Miller, R., Evans, A. C., et al. (2009). General absence of abnormal cortical asymmetry in childhood-onset schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Schizophr. Res. 115, 12–16. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barriga-Paulino, C. I., Flores, A. B., and Gomez, C. M. (2011). Developmental changes in the EEG rhythms of children and young adults: analyzed by means of correlational, brain topography and principal component analysis. J. Psychophysiol. 25, 143–158. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000052

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., and Johnstone, S. J. (2003). A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I. Qualitative and quantitative electroencephalography. Clin. Neurophysiol. 114, 171–183. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(02)00362-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bauermeister, J. J., Shrout, P. E., Ramírez, R., Bravo, M., Alegría, M., Martínez-Taboas, A., et al. (2007). ADHD correlates, comorbidity, and impairment in community and treated samples of children and adolescents. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 35, 883–898. doi: 10.1007/s10802-007-9141-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benjamini, Y., and Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 57, 289–300.

Google Scholar

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., Silva, J. M., et al. (2006). Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychol. Med. 36, 167–179. doi: 10.1017/s0033291705006410

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boord, P., Rennie, C., and Williams, L. (2007). Integrating “brain” and “body” measures: correlations between EEG and metabolic changes over the human lifespan. J. Integr. 6, 205–218. doi: 10.1142/S0219635207001416

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bresnahan, S. M., Anderson, J. W., and Barry, R. J. (1999). Age-related changes in quantitative EEG in attention- deficit / hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 46, 1690–1697. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00042-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bresnahan, S. M., and Barry, R. J. (2002). Specificity of quantitative EEG analysis in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. 112, 133–144. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(02)00190-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchmann, A., Ringli, M., Kurth, S., Schaerer, M., Geiger, A., Jenni, O. G., et al. (2011). EEG sleep slow-wave activity as a mirror of cortical maturation. Cereb. Cortex 21, 607–615. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burd, L., Klug, M. G., Coumbe, M. J., and Kerbeshian, J. (2003). Children and adolescents with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: 1. Prevalence and cost of care. J. Child Neurol. 18, 555–561. doi: 10.1177/08830738030180080101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chabot, R. J., and Serfontein, G. (1996). Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 40, 951–963. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00576-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., Dupuy, F. E., Heckel, L. D., Mccarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., et al. (2011). Clinical neurophysiology behavioural differences between EEG-defined subgroups of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 122, 1333–1341. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.12.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., and Selikovitz, M. (2001). Age and sex effects in the EEG: differences in two subtypes of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neuropshysiol. 112, 815–826. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00487-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., Selikowitz, M., and Johnstone, S. J. (2008). Effects of imipramine hydrochloride on the EEG of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder who are non-responsive to stimulants. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 68, 186–192. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Delorme, A., and Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickstein, S. G., Bannon, K., Castellanos, F. X., and Milham, M. P. (2006). The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 47, 1051–1062. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DuPaul, G. J., Power, T. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., and Reid, R. (1998). ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. New York, NY: Guilford.

Google Scholar

Dustman, R. E., Shearer, D. E., and Emmerson, R. Y. (1999). Life-span changes in EEG spectral amplitude, amplitude variability and mean frequency. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1399–1409. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00102-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykman, R. A., Holcomb, P. J., Oglesby, D. M., and Ackerman, P. T. (1982). Electrocortical frequencies in hyperactive, learning-disabled, mixed, and normal children. Biol. Psychiatry 17, 675–685.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Feinberg, I., and Campbell, I. G. (2010). Sleep EEG changes during adolescence: an index of a fundamental brain reorganization. Brain Cogn. 72, 56–65. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinberg, I., Thode, H. C. Jr., Chugani, H. T., and March, J. D. (1990). Gamma distribution model describes maturational curves for delta wave amplitude, cortical metabolic rate and synaptic density. J. Theor. Biol. 142, 149–161. doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(05)80218-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gasser, T., Verleger, R., Bächer, P., and Sroka, L. (1988). Development of the EEG of school-age children and adolescents. I. Analysis of band power. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 69, 91–99. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(88)90204-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Genro, J. P., Kieling, C., Rohde, L. A., and Hutz, M. H. (2010). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the dopaminergic hypotheses. Expert Rev. Neurother. 10, 587–601. doi: 10.1586/ern.10.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gibbs, F. A., and Knott, J. R. (1949). Growth of the electrical activity of the cortex. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 1, 223–229. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(49)90179-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagemann, D., Hewig, J., Walter, C., and Naumann, E. (2008). Skull thickness and magnitude of EEG alpha activity. Clin. Neurophysiol. 119, 1271–1280. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanć, T., Słpień, A., Wolańczyk, T., Dmitrzak-Wȩglarz, M., Szwed, A., Czapla, Z., et al. (2014). ADHD and overweight in boys: cross-sectional study with birth weight as a controlled factor. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 24, 41–53. doi: 10.1007/s00787-014-0531-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermens, D. F., Kohn, M. R., Clarke, S. D., Gordon, E., and Williams, L. M. (2005). Sex differences in adolescent ADHD: findings from concurrent EEG and EDA. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 1455–1463. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hipp, J., and Siegel, M. (2013). Dissociating neuronal gamma-band activity from cranial and ocular muscle activity in EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:338. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00338

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hobbs, M. J., Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., and Selikowitz, M. (2007). EEG abnormalities in adolescent males with AD/HD. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 363–371. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2006.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huttenlocher, P., and Dabholkar, A. (1997). Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 387, 167–178. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::AID-CNE1>3.0.CO;2-Z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jenni, O. G., and Carskadon, M. A. (2004). Spectral analysis of the sleep electroencephalogram during adolescence. Sleep 27, 774–783.

Google Scholar

Jensen, P. S., Hinshaw, S. P., Kraemer, H. C., Lenora, N., Newcorn, J. H., Abikoff, H. B., et al. (2001). ADHD comorbidity findings from the MTA study: comparing comorbid subgroups. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 40, 147–158. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200102000-00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnstone, S. J., Barry, R. J., and Clarke, A. R. (2013). Ten years on: a follow-up review of ERP research in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 644–657. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kasparek, T., Theiner, P., and Filova, A. (2015). Neurobiology of ADHD from childhood to adulthood: findings of imaging methods. J. Atten. Disord. 19, 931–943. doi: 10.1177/1087054713505322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kharitonova, M., Martin, R. E., Gabrieli, J. D. E., and Sheridan, M. A. (2013). Cortical gray-matter thinning is associated with age-related improvements on executive function tasks. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.07.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koehler, S., Lauer, P., Schreppel, T., Jacob, C., Heine, M., Boreatti-Hümmer, A., et al. (2009). Increased EEG power density in alpha and theta bands in adult ADHD patients. J. Neural. Transm. 116, 97–104. doi: 10.1007/s00702-008-0157-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuntsi, J., McLoughlin, G., and Asherson, P. (2006). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuromolecular Med. 8, 461–484. doi: 10.1385/NMM:8:4:461

CrossRef Full Text

Kurth, S., Ringli, M., Geiger, A., LeBourgeois, M., Jenni, O. G., and Huber, R. (2010). Mapping of cortical activity in the first two decades of life: a high-density sleep electroencephalogram study. J. Neurosci. 30, 13211–13219. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2532-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Langevin, L. M., Macmaster, F. P., and Dewey, D. (2015). Distinct patterns of cortical thinning in concurrent motor and attention disorders. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 57, 257–264. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazzaro, I., Gordon, E., Whitmont, S., Plahn, M., Li, W., Clarke, S., et al. (1998). Quantified EEG activity in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Electroencephalogr. 29, 37–42. doi: 10.1177/155005949802900111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lenartowicz, A., and Loo, S. K. (2014). Use of EEG to Diagnose ADHD. Curr. Psychiatry Rep. 16, 498. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0498-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liechti, M. D., Valko, L., Muller, U. C., Dohnert, M., Drechsler, R., Steinhausen, H. C., et al. (2013). Diagnostic value of resting electroencephalogram in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across the lifespan. Brain Topogr. 26, 135–151. doi: 10.1007/s10548-012-0258-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llinás, R., Urbano, F. J., Leznik, E., Ramirez, R. R., and van Marle, H. J. (2005). Rhythmic and dysrhythmic thalamocortical dynamics: GABA systems and the edge effect. Trends Neurosci. 28, 325–333. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2005.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loo, S. K., Cho, A., Hale, T. S., McGough, J., McCracken, J., and Smalley, S. L. (2013). Characterization of the theta to beta ratio in ADHD: identifying potential sources of heterogeneity. J. Atten. Disord. 17, 384–392. doi: 10.1177/1087054712468050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loo, S. K., Hale, T. S., Macion, J., Hanada, G., Mcgough, J. J., Mccracken, J. T., et al. (2009). Cortical activity patterns in ADHD during arousal, activation and sustained attention. Neuropsychologia 47, 2114–2119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loo, S. K., Lenartowicz, A., and Makeig, S. (2016). Research review?: use of EEG biomarkers in child psychiatry research – current state and future directions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 57, 4–17. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lüchinger, R., Michels, L., Martin, E., and Brandeis, D. (2012). Brain state regulation during normal development: intrinsic activity fluctuations in simultaneous EEG-fMRI. Neuroimage 60, 1426–1439. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Magee, C. A., Clarke, A. R., Barry, R. J., McCarthy, R., and Selikowitz, M. (2005). Examining the diagnostic utility of EEG power measures in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin. Neurophysiol. 116, 1033–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, C. A., Lubar, J. F., Zimmerman, A. W., Miller, C. A., and Muenchen, R. A. (1992). Quantitative analysis of EEG in boys with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder: controlled study with clinical implications. Pediatr. Neurol. 8, 30–36. doi: 10.1016/0887-8994(92)90049-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matousek, M., and Petersen, I. (1973). “Frequency analysis of the EEG in normal children and adolescents,” in Automation of Clinical Electroencephalography, eds P. Kellaway and I. Petersen (New York, NY: Raven Press), 75–102.

Google Scholar

Matsuura, M., Yamamoto, K., Fukuzawa, H., Okubo, Y., Uesugi, H., Moriiwa, M., et al. (1985). Age development and sex differences of various EEG elements in healthy children and adults–quantification by a computerized wave form recognition method. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 60, 394–406. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(85)91013-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miskovic, V., Ma, X., Chou, C.-A., Fan, M., Owens, M., Sayama, H., et al. (2015). Developmental changes in spontaneous electrocortical activity and network organization from early to late childhood. Neuroimage 118, 237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Narr, K. L., Woods, R. P., Lin, J., Kim, J., Phillips, O. R., Del’Homme, M., et al. (2009). Widespread cortical thinning is a robust anatomical marker for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 48, 1014–1022. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b395c0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ogrim, G., Kropotov, J., and Hestad, K. (2012). The quantitative EEG theta/beta ratio in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and normal controls: sensitivity, specificity, and behavioral correlates. Psychiatry Res. 198, 482–488. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.12.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penzes, P., Cahill, M. E., Jones, K. A., VanLeeuwen, J.-E., and Woolfrey, K. M. (2011). Dendritic spine pathology in neuropsychiatric disorders. Nat. Neurosci. 14, 285–293. doi: 10.1038/nn.2741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petanjek, Z., Judas, M., Simic, G., Rasin, M. R., Uylings, H. B. M., Rakic, P., et al. (2011). Extraordinary neoteny of synaptic spines in the human prefrontal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 13281–13286. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105108108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pliszka, S. R. (2000). Patterns of psychiatric comorbidity with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 9, 525–540, vii.

Google Scholar

Poil, S., Bollmann, S., Ghisleni, C., Gorman, R. L. O., Klaver, P., Ball, J., et al. (2014). Clinical neurophysiology age dependent electroencephalographic changes in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Clin. Neurophysiol. 125, 1626–1638. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.12.118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polanczyk, G., and Jensen, P. (2008). Epidemiologic considerations in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a review and update. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 17, 245–260, vii. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puligheddu, M., de Munck, J. C., Stam, C. J., Verbunt, J., de Jongh, A., van Dijk, B. W., et al. (2005). Age distribution of MEG spontaneous theta activity in healthy subjects. Brain Topogr. 17, 165–175. doi: 10.1007/s10548-005-4449-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Purves, D. (1998): Body and Brain: A Trophic Theory of Neural Connections. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Racicka, E., Hanc, T., Giertuga, K., Brynska, A., and Wolanczyk, T. (2015). Prevalence of overweight and obesity in children and adolescents with ADHD: the significance of comorbidities and pharmacotherapy. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054715578272 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

RStudio Team (2015). RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc.

Google Scholar

Rubia, K. (2007). Neuro-anatomic evidence for the maturational delay hypothesis of ADHD. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 104, 19663–19664. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0710329105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salat, D. H., Tuch, D. S., Greve, D. N., Van Der Kouwe, A. J. W., Hevelone, N. D., Zaleta, A. K., et al. (2005). Age-related alterations in white matter microstructure measured by diffusion tensor imaging. Neurobiol. Aging 26, 1215–1227. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2004.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scahill, L., and Schwab-Stone, M. (2000). Epidemiology of ADHD in school-age children. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am 9, 541–555, vii.

Google Scholar

Segalowitz, S. J., Santesso, D. L., and Jetha, M. K. (2010). Electrophysiological changes during adolescence: a review. Brain Cogn. 72, 86–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Eckstrand, K., Sharp, W., Blumenthal, J., Lerch, J. P., Greenstein, D., et al. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 19649–19654. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Gilliam, M., Liverpool, M., Weddle, C., Malek, M., Sharp, W., et al. (2011). Cortical development in typically developing children with symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity: support for a dimensional view of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 143–151. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10030385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Lerch, J., Greenstein, D., Sharp, W., Clasen, L., Evans, A., et al. (2006). Longitudinal mapping of cortical thickness and clinical outcome in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 63, 540–549. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.5.540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Greenstein, D., de Rossi, P., and Sharp, W. (2013). Trajectories of cerebral cortical development in childhood and adolescence and adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 74, 599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Sharp, W., Evans, A., and Greenstein, D. (2012). Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 72, 191–197. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snyder, S. M., and Hall, J. R. (2006). A meta-analysis of quantitative EEG power associated with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 23, 440–455. doi: 10.1097/01.wnp.0000221363.12503.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Somsen, R. J. M., Van’t Klooster, B. J., Van Der Molen, M. W., Van Leeuwen, H. M. P., and Licht, R. (1997). Growth spurts in brain maturation during middle childhood as indexed by EEG power spectra. Biol. Psychol. 44, 187–209. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(96)05218-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tamnes, C. K., Walhovd, K. B., Torstveit, M., Sells, V. T., and Fjell, A. M. (2013). Performance monitoring in children and adolescents: a review of developmental changes in the error-related negativity and brain maturation. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 6, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tierney, A., Strait, D. L., O’Connell, S., and Kraus, N. (2013). Developmental changes in resting gamma power from age three to adulthood. Clin. Neurophysiol. 124, 1040–1042. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.09.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tripp, G., and Wickens, J. R. (2009). Neuropharmacology neurobiology of ADHD. Neuropharmacology 57, 579–589. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2009.07.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uhlhaas, P. J., Roux, F., Rodriguez, E., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., and Singer, W. (2010). Neural synchrony and the development of cortical networks. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 72–80. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Dongen-Boomsma, M., Lansbergen, M. M., Bekker, E. M., Kooij, J. J., van der, M. M., Kenemans, J. L., et al. (2010). Relation between resting EEG to cognitive performance and clinical symptoms in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 469, 102–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.11.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandekar, S. N., Shinohara, R. T., Raznahan, A., Roalf, D. R., Ross, M., DeLeo, N., et al. (2015). Topologically dissociable patterns of development of the human cerebral cortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 599–609. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3628-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wackermann, J., and Matousek, M. (1998). From the ‘EEG age’ to a rational scale of brain electric maturation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 107, 415–421. doi: 10.1016/S0013-4694(98)00090-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitford, T. J., Rennie, C. J., Grieve, S. M., Clark, C. R., Gordon, E., and Williams, L. M. (2007). Brain maturation in adolescence: concurrent changes in neuroanatomy and neurophysiology. Hum. Brain Mapp. 28, 228–237. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilcox, R. R., and Keselman, H. J. (2003). Modern robust data analysis methods: measures of central tendency. Psychol. Methods 8, 254–274. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolańczyk, T., and Kołakowski, A. (2005). Kwestionariusze do diagnozy ADHD i Zaburzeñ Zachowania (The Diagnostic Structured Interview for ADHD and Hyperkinetic Disorder). Warsaw: Janssen-Cilag.

World Health Organization (1994). International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 10th Revision. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Zhong, J., Rifkin-Graboi, A., Ta, A. T., Yap, K. L., Chuang, K. H., Meaney, M. J., et al. (2014). Functional networks in parallel with cortical development associate with executive functions in children. Cereb. Cortex 24, 1937–1947. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, electroencephalography, resting-state, linear regression, development, ADHD, EEG

Citation: Giertuga K, Zakrzewska MZ, Bielecki M, Racicka-Pawlukiewicz E, Kossut M and Cybulska-Klosowicz A (2017) Age-Related Changes in Resting-State EEG Activity in Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: A Cross-Sectional Study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:285. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00285

Received: 08 February 2017; Accepted: 16 May 2017;
Published: 30 May 2017.

Edited by:

Juliana Yordanova, Bulgaria Academy of Sciences, Bulgaria

Reviewed by:

Ernesto Pereda, University of La Laguna, Spain
Björn Albrecht, University of Göttingen, Germany

Copyright © 2017 Giertuga, Zakrzewska, Bielecki, Racicka-Pawlukiewicz, Kossut and Cybulska-Klosowicz. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Katarzyna Giertuga, kasia.giertuga@gmail.com Anita Cybulska-Klosowicz, a.cybulska@nencki.gov.pl

Download