Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-27gpq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T01:31:11.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

How arousal influences neural competition: What dual competition does not explain

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 June 2015

Steven G. Greening
Affiliation:
Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089. greening@usc.edu Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA90089.
Mara Mather
Affiliation:
Davis School of Gerontology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089. greening@usc.edu Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA90089. Neuroscience Graduate Program, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA90089. mara.mather@usc.eduhttp://www.usc.edu/matherlab

Abstract

We argue that although the “dual competition” model is useful when considering interactions between emotional and neutral stimuli, it fails to account for the influence of emotional arousal on perceptual or goal-directed behavior involving neutral stimuli. We present the “arousal-biased competition” framework as an alternative that accounts for both scenarios.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Amting, J. M., Greening, S. G. & Mitchell, D. G. (2010) Multiple mechanisms of consciousness: The neural correlates of emotional awareness. Journal of Neuroscience 30(30):10039–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, A. K., Wais, P. E. & Gabrieli, J. D. (2006) Emotion enhances remembrance of neutral events past. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 103(5):1599–604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, J. M., Padmala, S. & Pessoa, L. (2012) Impact of state anxiety on the interaction between threat monitoring and cognition. NeuroImage 59(2):1912–23.Google Scholar
Dolcos, F. & McCarthy, G. (2006) Brain systems mediating cognitive interference by emotional distraction. Journal of Neuroscience 26(7):2072–79. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=16481440.Google Scholar
Knight, M. & Mather, M. (2009) Reconciling findings of emotion-induced memory enhancement and impairment of preceding items. Emotion 9(6):763–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, T. H., Sakaki, M., Cheng, R., Velasco, R. & Mather, M. (2014) Emotional arousal amplifies the effects of biased competition in the brain. Social Cognitive, Affective Neuroscience 9(12):2067–77.Google Scholar
Mather, M. & Sutherland, M. R. (2011) Arousal-biased competition in perception and memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science 6(2):114–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Padmala, S. & Pessoa, L. (2008) Affective learning enhances visual detection and responses in primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience 28(24):6202–10.Google Scholar
Pessoa, L. (2013) The cognitive-emotional brain. From interactions to integration. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A., Ling, S. & Carrasco, M. (2006) Emotion facilitates perception and potentiates the perceptual benefits of attention. Psychological Science 17(4):292–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakaki, M., Fryer, K. & Mather, M. (2014) Emotion strengthens high-priority memory traces but weakens low-priority memory traces. Psychological Science 25(2):387–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steidl, S., Mohi-uddin, S. & Anderson, A. K. (2006) Effects of emotional arousal on multiple memory systems: Evidence from declarative and procedural learning. Learning and Memory 13(5):650–58.Google Scholar
Strange, B. A., Hurlemann, R. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) An emotion-induced retrograde amnesia in humans is amygdala- and beta-adrenergic-dependent. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100(23):13626–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutherland, M. R. & Mather, M. (2012) Negative arousal amplifies the effects of saliency in short-term memory. Emotion 12(6):1367–72.Google Scholar