Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 July 2015
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Stress and cognition View all 7 articles

Mental abilities and performance efficacy under a simulated 480-m helium–oxygen saturation diving

\r\nGonglin Hou*Gonglin Hou1*Youlan ZhangYoulan Zhang1Na ZhaoNa Zhao1Ruiyong ChenRuiyong Chen2Weibing XiaoWeibing Xiao2Hao YuHao Yu2Jiachun WangJiachun Wang2Ti-Fei Yuan*Ti-Fei Yuan3*
  • 1Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Hangzhou, China
  • 2Institute of Naval Medicine, Shanghai, China
  • 3School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing, China

Stress in extreme environment severely disrupts human physiology and mental abilities. The present study investigated the cognition and performance efficacy of four divers during a simulated 480 meters helium–oxygen saturation diving. We analyzed the spatial memory, 2D/3D mental rotation functioning, grip strength, and hand–eye coordination ability in four divers during the 0–480 m compression and decompression processes of the simulated diving. The results showed that except for its mild decrease on grip strength, the high atmosphere pressure condition significantly impaired the hand–eye coordination (especially above 300 m), the reaction time and correct rate of mental rotation, as well as the spatial memory (especially as 410 m), showing high individual variability. We conclude that the human cognition and performance efficacy are significantly affected during deep water saturation diving.

Introduction

Stress in extreme environments severely affects the body and mind. For instance, during diving the physical stress (underwater pressure) increases with the water depth (11 atm at 100 m, and 48.6 atm at 480 m, for example; Brubakk et al., 2014). This creates inflammation signaling (Matsuo et al., 2000; Krog et al., 2010), oxidative stress load (Ikeda et al., 2004), extreme tiring experience of body, stress hormone secretion (Hirayanagi et al., 2003), sleep disruption (Nagashima et al., 2002), and the psychological stress in mind (Curley et al., 1979; Biersner et al., 1984), resulting in alterations of divers’ mood, cognition and performance efficacy, which are critical for diving work.

Previous studies reported different results on relevance between deep water diving (more than 300 m) and mental abilities, potentially due to the limited availability of subjects. In one study, it is found that 360 m helium–oxygen saturation diving did not affect the finger flexibility of divers (Hamilton, 1976); while another study reported increased finger tremors at 485 m diving (especially after 304 m) in six divers (Berghage et al., 1975). Other studies reported significant variability across individuals concerning the finger tremors (300 m), which is reduced in professional divers (Yamasaki et al., 1985). In addition, it is known that the spatial visual capacity is disrupted by high pressure (300–549 m; Lewis and Baddeley, 1981), and that the cognition is affected (Carter, 1979). Such mixed results indicate that depth and gas mixture are probably not the only factors. When exposed to high pressure, divers suffer from not only physical pressure but also psychological stress, such as anxiety, fear, and rapture during compression, underwater operation, and decompression.

The previous Asian record of saturation dive was 450 m Japan, yet the psychological changes during the dive have not been investigated. In addition, few studies systemically investigated the spatial memory, 2D/3D mental rotation, grip strength, and hand–eye coordination abilities during deep water saturation dive. We therefore hypothesized that the deep water diving might impair these aspects of mental abilities and performance efficacy when above certain levels of atmosphere pressure. With present study, we investigated these aspects during the compression and decompression processes of this 480 m (the new Asian record) saturation dive.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

Four male professional divers (right-handed, 27–32 years old, average at 30.3 ± 2.22) were recruited for this study. They passed strict physical and psychological examinations and were healthy.

The study is approved by ethic committee of human study in Zhejiang Sci-Tech University and all subjects provided written consent for experimental procedures. The procedures followed the guidelines of human research from ethic committee in Zhejiang Sci-tech University and Nanjing Normal University.

Experimental Methods

For grip strength measurement, CWJ-1 grip scale (range 0–100 kg) was provided by Service center from Sports department of China government. The measurement was performed three times for each hand, and the maximum value was taken (kg). The subjects recorded the values for each other.

For hand–eye coordination experiment, 4-hole buttons (button diameter 12.5 mm, hole diameter 1.5 mm) were used (three holes blocked). An 80 cm long 1 mm diameter cotton wire was used. In the test, the subjects were asked to thread the wire through the remaining button hole to connect as many buttons as possible in 1 min. The test was repeated three times and the average was taken.

For 2D mental rotation test, two “R” characters rotated at 45°/90°/135°/180° (five questions for each degree, 20 questions in total) were used (Figure 1A). The subjects were asked to judge whether the two “R” characters were exactly the same or in mirror image. The reaction time and correct rate were recorded.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The test materials and the compression procedure. (A) 2D mental rotation, (B) 3D mental rotation, (C) Spatial memory test, (D) The compression and depression procedures.

For 3D mental rotation test, two 3D images were used for each question (20 questions in total) as previously described (Shepard and Metzler, 1971; Figure 1B). The subjects were asked to judge whether the two images could merge to each other. The reaction time and correct rate were recorded.

For spatial memory test, the 6 × 6 grids paper printed with Chinese chess characters were used (Figure 1C). Each paper was printed with five characters at random grids. The subjects were asked to remember the name and the place of the five characters on the paper in 30 s. The experiment was repeated for five times with different papers each time. The correct rate was recorded.

Simulation of Saturation Diving

The simulation was conducted at the diver living chamber of 500 m saturation diving system at Institute of Naval medicine, Shanghai, China. The chamber has an internal diameter of 2.2 m and length 5.5 m and living volume 19.5 m3. The chamber is filled with helium–oxygen, temperature 31 ± 1°, moisture 50–70%, noise lower than 75 dB and light intensity 50–100 lx.

The study was divided into predive, compression, stay, decompression, and postdive phases. For all tests presented above, the divers were trained repeatedly to reach a stable baseline, and the last score was taken as baseline. During the compression phase, the atmosphere pressure supplied at 0–10 m was 21% helium–oxygen, and 10–480 m with pure helium. The rate of pressure increase was at 0.6 m/min for 0–300 m, 0.1 m/min for 301–400 m, and 0.04 m/min for 401–480 m. The subjects stayed at 300, 400, and 443 m for 7–9 h each. The compression and depression procedures were described in Figure 1D.

For cognition tests (spatial memory and mental rotation), the data was collected at 0 m before, 150/350/410/463 m during compression, the early/late phase of 480 m stay, 400/308/230/154/83/25 m during decompression, 3 days and 1 month after the test at 0 m.

For grip strength test, the data was collected at 0 m before, 150/270/300/400 m during compression, the early/late phase of 480 m stay, 446/398/357/314/274/230/192/154/119/85/54/25 m during decompression, 3 days and 1 month after the test at 0 m.

For hand–eye coordination test, the data was collected at 0 m before, 150/270/300/400/463 m during compression, the early/late phase of 480 m stay, 400/308/230/154/83/25 m during decompression, 3 days and 1 month after the test at 0 m.

Statistics

The data is analyzed with SPSS 17.0 software (Chicago, IL, USA) for database building. The results were tested with analyses of variances and LSD post hoc test. The data was normalized to baseline point (100%) for better visualization and understanding, and to decrease the effects of individual variability at a small n.

Results

Mental Abilities

2D Mental Rotation

In 2D mental rotation, the correct rates for four divers (Figure 2A) and average (Figure 2B) exhibited mild changes during the compression and decompression phases (ranged 85%∼111.11% of baseline). While the reaction times of four divers (Figure 2C) and the average (Figure 2D) exhibited greater fluctuations (ranged 67.31%∼151.22% of baseline). There was also noticeable variability among individual divers. The correct rates were worse than baseline for diver A, B, and D (ranged 85%∼100% of baseline), but better for diver C (ranged 100∼111.11% of baseline). The variability of reaction time of the four divers was much larger. Reaction times were negatively affected as compression continued until the depth of 410 m for diver B, C, and D, but not for diver A (except the depth of 150 m in compression and 400 m in decompression).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The results from 2D mental rotation. (A) Correct rate for four divers, (B) Correct rate on Average, (C) Reaction time for four divers, (D) Reaction time on average.

Repeated variance analyses revealed that the depth had a significant effect on reaction time [F(14,42) = 4.226, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.585], but not on the correct rate [F(14,42) = 0.864, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.224]. During the compression phase, the reaction time continuously increased until 410 m (LSD post hoc test P < 0.01 when compared to predive).

We further compared the 2D mental rotation ability across different phases of the experiment (predive, compression, stay, decompression, 3 days, and 1 month after the dive; Table 1). Repeated variance analyses revealed that the phase of the experiment had a significant effect on the reaction time [F(5,15) = 7.09, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.703], but not on the correct rate [F(5,15) = 5.05, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.138]. LSD post hoc test revealed that the reaction times of the four divers significantly increased in the compression phase (when compared to the baseline, P < 0.05) and returned to the baseline levels during the decompression phase (P > 0.05 when compared to predive).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. 2D mental rotation ability across different phases of the experiment (www.frontiersin.org ± SD, n = 4).

3D Mental Rotation

In 3D mental rotation, the correct rates (Figures 3A,B) and reaction times (Figures 3C,D) of the four divers exhibited considerable impairment and large variability across individual, with reaction times ranging 58.0%∼226.0% and correct rates 37.5%∼118.8%. Correct rates of all the four divers declined as compression processed, while diver C and D were less impaired than A and B (ranged 77.8%∼105.6%, 84.2%∼105.3% vs. 37.5%∼118.8%, 56.3%∼100.0%, in compared to baseline). There was enormous variability on reaction times of four divers. Only small impairment was detected on diver C during the compression and decompression but that became a little greater after the dive, on the contrary, the reaction times of diver A and B increased throughout the dive until they came back to the surface. The pattern of reaction time for diver D was different from above, whose reaction time increased as compression was performed but restored since the depth of 463 in compression.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. The results from 3D mental rotation. (A) Correct rate for four divers, (B) Correct rate on Average, (C) Reaction time for four divers, (D) Reaction time on average.

Repeated variance analyses revealed that the depth had a significant effect on reaction time [F(14,42) = 3.105, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.509], as well as on correct rate [F(14,42) = 3.129, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.511]. LSD post hoc test revealed that during the compression phase, the reaction time continuously increased and the correct rate decreased until 410 m for reaction time (P > 0.05 in compared to predive) and 150 m for correct rate (P > 0.05, when compared to predive).

We further compared the 3D mental rotation ability across different phases of the experiment (predive, compression, stay, decompression, 3 days, and 1 month after the dive; Table 2). Repeated variance analyses revealed that the phase of the experiment had a significant effect on the reaction time [F(5,15) = 12.10, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.801], but not on the correct rate [F(5,15) = 6.09, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.465]. LSD post hoc test revealed that the reaction times of the four divers slightly increased in the compression/stay/decompression phases (when compared to the baseline, P > 0.05) but decreased significantly after the dive (P < 0.01 when compared to baseline).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. 3D mental rotation ability across different phases of the experiment (www.frontiersin.org ± SD, n = 4).

Spatial Memory

The spatial memory ability (Figures 4A,B) of the four divers exhibited large variability across individuals. The performance of diver A appears to be stable compared to baseline, except 350–480 m in compression and stay phase ranging from 82.4%∼96.0%. Greater impairment was observed on diver B and C, which decreased as compression processed until 480 m (59.1% for diver B and 76.0% for diver C, compared to baseline). While, the worst performance for diver D was at the depth of 410 m in compression (72.0% compared to baseline).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. The results from spatial memory. (A) Four divers, (B) Average.

Repeated variance analyses and LSD post hoc test revealed that the depth has a significant effect on spatial memory [F(14,42) = 2.809, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.484], which was significant impaired during 410–480 m in compression and stay phase (P < 0.05). During the compression phase, the spatial memory continuously decreased until 480 m (P < 0.05 in compared to baseline); while spatial memory returned to baseline as decompression started (P > 0.05 when compared to predive).

We further compared the spatial memory ability across different phases of the experiment (Predive, compression, stay, decompression, 3 days and 1 month after the dive; Table 3). Repeated variance analyses revealed that the phase of the experiment has a significant effect on spatial memory [F(5,15) = 5.19, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.634]. LSD post hoc test revealed that the spatial memory of the four divers slightly decreased in the compression phases (when compared to the baseline, P > 0.05), and significantly decreased in the stay phase (P < 0.05 to baseline), and returned to baseline level during the decompression phase (P > 0.05 to baseline).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Spatial memory ability across different phases of the experiment (www.frontiersin.org ± SD, n = 4).

Performance Efficacy

Grip Strength

The right hand (Figure 5A) and left hand (Figure 5B) grip strength also showed individual variability (ranged 74.24%∼113.04% of baseline). Both left-hand and right-hand grip strength of diver B were associated with the depth, which declined as compression processed and restored during decompression. While, slight impairment was detected for diver D. Interestingly, left-hand grip strength of diver C was even higher than baseline. Furthermore, the best performances of right-hand grip strength for diver A and C were observed at the depth of 480 m, as well as best right-hand performance for diver D (110.4, 104.2, and 109.3% respectively, when compared to baseline).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. The results from grip strength. (A) Right hand for divers, (B) Left hand for four divers, (C) Left and right hand on average, respectively.

Repeated variance analyses revealed that the depth had no significant effect on right hand grip strength [F(20,60) = 1.029, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.255], as well as the left hand grip strength [F(20,60) = 1.078, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.264]. In general (Figure 5C), the depth above 230 m are associated with lower grip strength.

We further compared the grip strength across different phases of the experiment (Predive, compression, stay, decompression, 3 days, and 1 month after the dive; Table 4). Repeated variance analyses revealed that the phase of the experiment has no significant effect on both right [F(5,15) = 0.83, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.217] and left hand [F(5,15) = 0.63, P > 0.05, ηp2 = 0.173] grip strength. Both left and right hand grip slightly decreased at stay and decompression phase.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Grip strength of left and right hand across different phases of the experiment (www.frontiersin.org ± SD, n = 4).

Hand–Eye Coordination

The hand–eye coordination ability of the four divers during compression showed similar trends of change (Figures 6A,B), ranged 68.75%∼118.33%. The lowest values were at 400–480 m.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. The results from hand–eye coordination. (A) Four divers, (B) Average.

Repeated variance analyses revealed that the depth had a significant effect on hand–eye coordination ability [F(15,45) = 6.50, P < 0.05, ηp2 = 0.684]. LSD post hoc test revealed that During the compression phase, the hand–eye coordination ability showed no significant changes at 150, 270, and 300 m, and decreased at 410, 463 m (P < 0.05 when compared to baseline); while during the decompression phase the hand–eye coordination ability restored at 308 m (P > 0.05 when compared to predive).

We further investigated hand–eye coordination ability across different phases of the experiment (Predive, compression, stay, decompression, 3 days, and 1 month after the dive; Table 5). Repeated variance analyses revealed that the phase of the experiment had a significant effect on hand–eye coordination ability [F(5,15) = 8.113, P < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.730]. LSD post hoc test revealed that the hand–eye coordination ability of the four divers slightly decreased in the compression phase (when compared to the baseline, P > 0.05), and significantly decreased in the stay phase (P < 0.05 to predive), and returned to baseline level during the decompression phase (P > 0.05 when compared to predive).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Hand–eye coordination ability across different phases of the experiment (www.frontiersin.org ± SD, n = 4).

Discussion

Chronic psychological or physical stress is known to induced physiological changes in the brain, impairments in cognition, sensation of the external world, and general physiological alterations (Hou et al., 2014, 2015; Tian et al., 2014; Yuan and Hou, 2015; Yuan et al., 2015). Extreme environment stress has been known to induce changes in both the body and mind, as the case of deep water diving with high atmosphere pressure (Curley et al., 1979; Biersner et al., 1984; Nagashima et al., 2002; Hirayanagi et al., 2003; Brubakk et al., 2014). In present study with simulated helium–oxygen saturation diving experiment, we found that the correct rate of 2D mental rotation was relatively impaired under the high atmosphere pressure, while the 3D mental rotation ability was significantly impaired, suggesting that 3D task is more vulnerable to the depth of diving. In addition, the reaction time of 2D and 3D mental rotation increased with the depth. The 410 m depth seems to be the changing point between reaction time increase and becoming stable, potentially due to the adaption effect. This is in line with previous study showing that divers react to high pressure as slowing down their working speed, in order to maintain the working efficacy (30 m, air diving; Petri, 2003). It is also realized that making a mistake in performance will result in serious consequences in diving task (Logie and Baddeley, 1983), and therefore the working speed is relatively less important.

In addition, the spatial memory was significantly impaired with compression between 410 and 480 m, showing large individual variability. Previous study also revealed decreased visual spatial ability in diving at 300–540 m (Lewis and Baddeley, 1981). Notably, the parietal lobe is known to be involved in both spatial cognition (Culham and Valyear, 2006) and mental rotation (Harris and Miniussi, 2003). Whether the high atmosphere pressure leads to dysfunction of parietal lobe is yet to be investigated in future studies.

The mental abilities are critical elements determining the task performance efficacy. We chose the grip strength and hand–eye coordination ability as the two measurements as previously described (Lewis and Baddeley, 1981; Logie and Baddeley, 1983; Petri, 2003). We found that the grip strength is mildly affected. In previous study with open sea dive, the high pressure led to increased tremor, decreased grip strength, decreased speed of steeping, and slower spontaneous reaction (Vaernes et al., 1987). It is also found that during the compression, the EEG θ wave significantly increased, while α, β1, and β2 activity decreased (Bennett and Towse, 1971), which might explain the changes in grip strength.

The hand–eye coordination was clearly impaired by the high pressure environment, which declined continuously to 480 m. In one study, it was found that the finger flexibility decreased by 2% at 180 m and 10% at 450 m (Rostain et al., 1983); while in another study with helium–oxygen saturation diving, the authors reported no changes in finger flexibility at 360 m (Hamilton, 1976). We believe that this might be due to the large individual differences. Three-hundred meter might be the turning point of tremor induction, and therefore the decrease in hand–eye coordination ability.

In summary, we report that the mental abilities and performance efficacy are significantly impaired during deep water helium–oxygen saturation diving.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Science Philosophy Betterment Society (Registered British Virgin Islands). TY received supports by “Hundred Talents program,” “Qing Lan Project” of Nanjing Normal University and Jiangsu Provincial Natural Science Foundation (No. BK20140917).

References

Bennett, P. B., and Towse, E. J. (1971). The high pressure nervous syndrome during a simulated oxygen-helium dive to 1500 ft. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 31, 383–393. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(71)90234-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berghage, T. E., Lash, L. E., Braithwaite, W. R., and Thalmann, E. D. (1975). Intentional tremor on a helium-oxygen chamber dive to 49.5 ATA. Undersea Biomed. Res. 2, 215–222.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Biersner, R. J., Mchugh, W. B., and Rahe, R. H. (1984). Biochemical and mood responses predictive of stressful diving performance. J. Hum. Stress 10, 43–49. doi: 10.1080/0097840X.1984.9934958

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brubakk, A. O., Ross, J. A., and Thom, S. R. (2014). Saturation diving; physiology and pathophysiology. Compr. Physiol. 4, 1229–1272. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c130048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carter, R. C. (1979). Mental abilities during a simulated dive to 427 meters underwater. J. Appl. Psychol. 64, 449–454. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.64.4.449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Culham, J. C., and Valyear, K. F. (2006). Human parietal cortex in action. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 16, 205–212. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curley, M. D., Berghage, T. E., Raymond, L. W., Sode, J., and Leach, C. (1979). Emotional stability during a chamber saturation dive to 49.5 atmospheres absolute. J. Appl. Psychol. 64, 548–557. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.64.5.548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hamilton, R. W. (1976). Psychomotor performance of men in neon and helium at 37 atmosphere. Underwater Physiol. 5, 651–664.

Harris, I. M., and Miniussi, C. (2003). Parietal lobe contribution to mental rotation demonstrated with rTMS. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 15, 315–323. doi: 10.1162/089892903321593054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirayanagi, K., Nakabayashi, K., Okonogi, K., and Ohiwa, H. (2003). Autonomic nervous activity and stress hormones induced by hyperbaric saturation diving. Undersea Hyperb. Med. 30, 47–55.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hou, G., Xiong, W., Wang, M., Chen, X., and Yuan, T. F. (2014). Chronic stress influences sexual motivation and causes damage to testicular cells in male rats. J. Sex Med. 11, 653–663. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hou, G., Zhao, Y., Yang, X., and Yuan, T. F. (2015). Autophagy does not lead to the asymmetrical hippocampal injury in chronic stress. Physiol. Behav. 144, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ikeda, M., Nakabayashi, K., Shinkai, M., Hara, Y., Kizaki, T., Oh-Ishi, S., et al. (2004). Supplementation of antioxidants prevents oxidative stress during a deep saturation dive. Tohoku J. Exp. Med. 203, 353–357. doi: 10.1620/tjem.203.353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krog, J., Tonnesen, E. K., Jepsen, C. F., Parner, E., Segadal, K., Hope, A., et al. (2010). Natural killer cells as biomarkers of hyperbaric stress during a dry heliox saturation dive. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 81, 467–474. doi: 10.3357/ASEM.2528.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, V. J., and Baddeley, A. D. (1981). Cognitive performance, sleep quality and mood during deep oxyhelium diving. Ergonomics 24, 773–793. doi: 10.1080/00140138108924899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logie, R. H., and Baddeley, A. D. (1983). A Trimix saturation dive to 660 m. Studies of cognitive performance, mood and sleep quality. Ergonomics 26, 359–374. doi: 10.1080/00140138308963352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matsuo, H., Shinomiya, N., and Suzuki, S. (2000). Hyperbaric stress during saturation diving induces lymphocyte subset changes and heat shock protein expression. Undersea Hyperb. Med. 27, 37–41.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nagashima, H., Matsumoto, K., Seo, Y., Mohri, M., Naraki, N., and Matsuoka, S. (2002). Sleep patterns during 30-m nitrox saturation dives and in a confined atmospheric environment. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 56, 267–268. doi: 10.1046/j.1440-1819.2002.01021.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petri, N. M. (2003). Change in strategy of solving psychological tests: evidence of nitrogen narcosis in shallow air-diving. Undersea Hyperb. Med. 30, 293–303.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Rostain, J. C., Lemaire, C., Gardette-Chauffour, M. C., Doucet, J., and Naquet, R. (1983). Estimation of human susceptibility to the high-pressure nervous syndrome. J. Appl. Physiol. Respir. Environ. Exerc. Physiol. 54, 1063–1070.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science 171, 701–703. doi: 10.1126/science.171.3972.701

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, R., Hou, G., Li, D., and Yuan, T. F. (2014). A possible change process of inflammatory cytokines in the prolonged chronic stress and its ultimate implications for health. ScientificWorldJournal 2014, 780616. doi: 10.1155/2014/780616

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vaernes, R. J., Aarli, J. A., Klove, H., and Tonjum, S. (1987). Differential neuropsychological effects of diving to 350 meters. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 58, 155–165.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Yamasaki, M., Sasaki, T., Sakamoto, K., Shidara, F., Taya, Y., Nakano, M., et al. (1985). Power spectral analysis of intentional and postural tremor during a simulated heliox dive to 31ATA. Ann. Physiol. Anthropol. 4, 129–136. doi: 10.2114/ahs1983.4.129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, T. F., and Hou, G. (2015). The effects of stress on glutamatergic transmission in the brain. Mol. Neurobiol. 51, 1139–1143. doi: 10.1007/s12035-014-8783-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yuan, T. F., Hou, G., and Arias-Carrion, O. (2015). Chronic stress impacts on olfactory system. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets 14, 486–491. doi: 10.2174/1871527314666150429111356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: saturation diving, spatial memory, mental rotation, grip strength, hand–eye coordination, cognition, helium–oxygen, stress

Citation: Hou G, Zhang Y, Zhao N, Chen R, Xiao W, Yu H, Wang J and Yuan T-F (2015) Mental abilities and performance efficacy under a simulated 480-m helium–oxygen saturation diving. Front. Psychol. 6:979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00979

Received: 12 May 2015; Accepted: 29 June 2015;
Published: 09 July 2015.

Edited by:

Jianhui Wu, Institute of Psychology – Chinese Academy of Sciences, China

Reviewed by:

Shane M. O’Mara, Trinity College Dublin, Ireland
Sergio Machado, Federal University of Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Copyright © 2015 Hou, Zhang, Zhao, Chen, Xiao, Yu, Wang and Yuan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Gonglin Hou, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Sci-Tech University, Hangzhou 310018, China, 13505818948@163.com; Ti-Fei Yuan, School of Psychology, Nanjing Normal University, Nanjing 210097, China, ytf0707@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.