Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 29 March 2019
Sec. Comparative Psychology

Red Wood Ants Display Natural Aversive Learning Differently Depending on Their Task Specialization

  • 1Institute of Systematics and Ecology of Animals, Siberian Branch of the Russian Academy of Sciences, Novosibirsk, Russia
  • 2V. Zelman Institute for Medicine and Psychology, Novosibirsk State University, Novosibirsk, Russia

The adaptive benefits of individual specialization and how learning abilities correlate with task performance are still far from being well-understood. Red wood ants are characterized by their huge colonies and deep professional specialization. We hypothesized that red wood ants Formica aquilonia form aversive learning after having negative encounters with hoverfly larvae differently, depending on their task specialization. We tested this hypothesis, first, by examining whether hunters and aphid milkers learn differently to avoid the nuisance of contacts with syrphid larvae, and, second, by analyzing the difference between learning in “field” and laboratory-reared (naïve) foragers. During the first interaction with the syrphid larva in their lives the naïve foragers showed a significantly higher level of aggressiveness than the members of a natural colony. Naïve foragers applied the “mortal grip,” “prolonged bites,” and “nibbling” toward the enemy with a significantly higher frequency, whereas members of both “field” groups behaved more carefully and tried to avoid encounters with the larva. The aphid milkers, who had a negative experience of interaction with the larva, being “glued” with its viscous secretion, behaved much less aggressively in the follow-up experiments after 10 min and even 3 days, thus exhibiting the shaping of both short- and long-term memories. However, both “field” hunters and naïve foragers demonstrated no signs of aversive learning. These data provide some new insights into the relationship between task specialization and learning performance in ants. Given our previous results, we speculate that scouts and aphid milkers are the most cognitively gifted specialists in red wood ants, whereas hunters and guards are rather brave than smart.

Introduction

Eusocial insect species form persistent colonies, where groups of individuals (castes) perform different tasks (division of labor). Not only members of the reproductive castes, such as queens, have clearly defined responsibilities with workers, but the workers themselves also have particular tasks such as caring for the young, defense, foraging, nest-building activities, and so on (for a review see: Robinson, 1992). The main characteristic of insect societies is the way tasks are distributed among group members. The adaptive benefits of individual specialization and how learning abilities correlate with task performance are still far from being understood. Ants are good candidates for studying these problems as a highly diverse and successful group of social hymenopterans, which, unlike bees and wasps, consists only of eusocial species (for a review see: Reznikova, 2018). There are more than 12,000 ant species in the world, with different colony sizes (from tens to millions of individuals), social life and styles of cooperation, from single foraging to mass recruiting. Ant species display different modes of the division of labor and the specialization of workers on various tasks. Caste polyphenism is rather expressive in some ant species, which harbor special sub-castes of workers profoundly different by morphology, physiology, and behavior (for a review see: Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990). In leaf-cutting ants, for instance, tiny “mini workers” cultivate fungi in the subterranean nest to feed the larvae. Other workers have an up to 200-fold increased body weight, leaving the nest for long foraging trips, and bringing back leaves which are used as substrate for the fungi (Wilson, 1980). However, a morphological caste system may also have costs, as it may prevent a colony from rapidly adjusting caste ratios, increase the energetic cost of rearing or limit the task repertoire (Oster and Wilson, 1978). In the majority of ant species, workers only specialize in different tasks behaviorally. In some ant species, behavioral specialization is strongly determined by chronological age and physiological development (temporal polyethism): young workers typically perform safe tasks inside the nest, such as nursing the brood, and only later in life move on to more risky tasks outside the nest, such as foraging or territorial defense (Hölldobler and Wilson, 1990; Beshers et al., 2001; Giraldo and Traniello, 2014). In other species, task specialization among workers may be shaped by their size polymorphism, genetic background, experience, and social interactions, which is also partly influenced by age (Tripet and Nonacs, 2004; Schwander et al., 2005; Ravary et al., 2007; Helanterä et al., 2013; Giehr et al., 2017; Doering et al., 2018). Behavioral and cognitive mechanisms of the specialization of workers on different tasks remain the least studied in this area.

Red wood ants (the Formica rufa group) are possibly the most promising group for studying the role of learning and experience in task specialization among workers (Reznikova, 2018). In comparison with many sympatric species, the mound-building red wood ants have hundreds of times more individuals in their colonies and spacious feeding territories (Dlussky, 1967; Rosengren and Sundström, 1987). Every day red wood ants face complex vital problems: for example, in order to obtain honeydew, the basic food for adults, thousands of colony members have to find and memorize locations of a large number of aphid colonies within such a huge three-dimensional space as a tree is for an ant (Reznikova, 2008). Dobrzanska (1958) demonstrated that in red wood ants, groups of individuals return repeatedly to approximately the same parts of the colony’s feeding territory on the ground and work together there. Studying site allegiance in red wood ants, Rosengren and Fortelius (1987) characterized red wood ants as “replete ants” storing not lipids in their fat bodies but habitat information in their brains, and members of this group of species used to be a relevant model for studying spatial learning and intelligence (Reznikova and Ryabko, 1994; Nicholson et al., 1999).

It was demonstrated in early studies that in red wood ants, out-nest workers include hunters and collectors of nest material acting on the ground, aphid milkers collecting honeydew on the trees and hunters collecting prey there, as well as guards defending the nest entrances (Otto, 1958; Horstmann, 1973). Studies at the individual level revealed deep professional specialization, that is, considerable behavioral differences between members within different task groups. For instance, the task group of aphid tenders turned out to include professional subgroups such as scouts, aphid milkers (“shepherds”), aphid guards, and carriers (Reznikova and Novgorodova, 1998a; Reznikova, 2007). Experimental studies of interactions of the red wood ant Formica aquilonia with ground beetles, their eternal enemies, large and dangerous, revealed that nest guards, and hunters are much more aggressive than aphid milkers (Reznikova and Dorosheva, 2004; Dorosheva et al., 2011). Experiments with other intruders, such as spiders and the small parasitic rove beetle, revealed a context-dependent specialization in colony defense in the red wood ant F. rufa: small workers were better at preventing brood predation than larger workers, and nurses and workers at nest entrances were more aggressive toward parasitic beetles than extranidal foragers (Parmentier et al., 2015).

Reznikova and Ryabko (1994, 2011) revealed in red wood ants a group-retrieving mode of foraging basing on the difference of the searching activity and cognitive abilities between scouts and recruited foragers. Scouts appeared to be able to grasp regularities in the sequences of turns (right and left) in the “binary tree” maze on the way to the goal, and use them to optimize their messages to recruited foragers, whereas the recruited foragers can only memorize and not transfer the information. The sophisticated communication system between the scouts and the recruited foragers is even more complicated than the honeybee dance language (see details in: Reznikova, 2017). Experiments with a battery of behavioral tests demonstrated that scouts form spatial memory faster and keep information longer than recruited foragers. They were, in general, more exploratory than other out-nest workers, more readily switched between different activities in unfamiliar situations, and, although displaying an intermediate level of aggressiveness between aphid milkers and nest guards, they never attacked the enemy directly (Atsarkina et al., 2014; Reznikova, 2018).

A question then arises about the distribution of cognitive responsibilities within the ant colony (sensu: Reznikova, 2008), that is, about the differentiation between groups in their abilities to perform cognitively demanding tasks. In this study, we concentrate on how representatives of different task groups shape natural aversive learning in the context of repeated interactions with an enemy. Similar with Hollis et al. (2017) study, we use the term “natural aversive learning” meaning ants’ behavior toward their natural enemies or/and predators, although agreeing with Hénaut et al. (2014) that even if a species demonstrates aversive learning in the laboratory, most often it is difficult to determine whether such a capacity is likely to occur in natural conditions (see also: Bernays, 1993).

Aversive learning has been studied in ants mainly on the basis of one-trial learning. Ants’ memory for an aversive event was tested shortly after their unpleasant experience. For example, Ectatomma tuberculatum appeared to learn how to break quickly through the spiders’ web and kept this memory during 15 min after the first experience (Hénaut et al., 2014), Formica pratensis retained the memory of a single unpleasant collision with a hoverfly larva for 10–30 min after the event (Novgorodova, 2015), and pavement Tetramorium ants learned to avoid antlion traps following a single successful escape from a pit for 1 min after the encounter (Hollis et al., 2017). Long-term memory for an aversive event was demonstrated in the experiments of Dejean (1988) with Odontomachus troglodytes. These ants kept the memory about encounters with the chemically defensive larva of the African chrysomeline during 28 days. As far as we know, natural aversive learning in ants has never been considered in the context of task specialization.

The interaction between red wood ants and hoverfly larvae can serve as a natural and somehow unique experimental model in studying differences in learning abilities between members of different task groups. Hoverfly (Diptera, Syrphidae) females use semiochemicals to locate aphid colonies and to oviposit eggs from which aphidophagous larvae hatch (see reviews in: Detrain et al., 2017). It is known that aphids provide a vital energy source that is essential for the survival and growth of the ant colony (Addicott, 1978; Skinner and Whittaker, 1981), and ants actively protect their trophobionts from all aphidophages (Novgorodova and Gavrilyuk, 2012). Hoverfly larvae use adhesive saliva to incapacitate an attacker (Eisner, 1972; Rotheray, 1986). Ecological aspects of intricate interplay between ants and hoverfly larvae have been studied recently on F. pratensis by Novgorodova (2015) and on Lasius niger by Detrain et al. (2017). Both studies showed that to prevent predation of aphids by hoverfly larvae, ants demonstrated aggressive behavior; once bitten by ants, third instar hoverfly larvae released a droplet of viscous and sticky secretion from the mouth, which hardens like glue. These actions make ants stop their attacks to clean themselves and decrease aggression toward the larvae for a few minutes. Since the impact of the larvae on ants is not dangerous, but only unpleasant for them, ants can interact with the enemy many times in a row. This experimental model allows one to check if ants are learning to avoid unpleasant encounters.

We, therefore, hypothesized that the members of different task groups form aversive learning after having negative encounters with hoverfly larvae differently depending on their tasks. We tested this hypothesis, first, by examining whether hunters and aphid milkers from the natural colony of F. aquilonia learn differently to avoid the nuisance of contacts with hoverfly larvae, and, second, by analyzing the difference between learning in the “field” and naïve foragers. We successfully showed that the aphid milkers shaped both short- and long-term memories about a negative experience of interaction with the enemy, whereas hunters and naïve foragers did not change their behavior after the unpleasant event. As far as we know, this is the first demonstration of natural aversive learning in ants in the context of task specialization.

Materials and Methods

Insects

We collected ants near Novosibirsk, western Siberia, Russia (54 50.1N, 83 06.1E), in a mixed birch-pine forest. Hunters and aphid milkers were taken from the colony of F. aquilonia. We regarded as hunters those ants which carried insect prey on foraging trails toward the nest. The aphid milkers were collected on colonies of Symydobius oblongus aphids on Betula pendula where ants tend the aphids and obtain honeydew from them. These “field” hunters and aphid milkers were kept in the laboratory, in separate artificial nests located in separate arenas (40 cm × 20 cm × 20 cm) in groups of 100 individuals. We formed the naïve colony from the ants hatched in the laboratory from pupae taken from the same colony of F. aquilonia. The naïve colony included about 800 workers, brood and the queen, and was kept in an artificial nest located in an arena measuring 150 cm × 80 cm × 20 cm during more than a year, without any contacts with the “field” workers or other insects. For the experiments, we selected naïve workers 13–14 months of age that were engaged in collecting both carbohydrate and protein food as well as the nest material in the arena and carrying ant corpses out of the nest. Similar to the study of Reznikova and Novgorodova (1998b) conducted on naïve ants earlier, there were no distinguishable task groups among foraging naïve workers, which is why we regarded them merely as “naïve foragers.” All ants obtained sugar syrup as the carbohydrate food and crumbled eggs as the protein food. Members of all groups were individually marked, which helped us to distinguish them in the follow-up experiments. Paints of 6 colors and a five-point code were used: one mark on the head, two on the thorax, and two on the abdomen. Tags were renewed every 1–2 weeks. Hoverfly larvae were collected in mixed forests near Novosibirsk from various plants and kept individually in aerated plastic boxes (10 cm × 6 cm × 4 cm), with plants infested with aphids.

Behavior of Ants and Hoverfly Larvae in Dyadic Interactions

To record and describe the behavior of the insects, we use the notions “behavioral element” and “behavioral pattern” as it is used in Reznikova et al., 2017, that is, referring to a certain behavioral act (such as “bite”), and a certain sequence of elements (such as “hunting”).

To compare the agonistic behavior toward an enemy in different groups of ants (“field” aphid milkers and hunters, and naïve foragers), we observed pairwise interactions between ants and hoverfly larvae in plastic containers (20 cm × 8 cm × 4 cm) covered with glass. Each test lasted 10 min after the first contact between the insects. Similar with the Detrain et al. (2017) study, we used the third instar syrphid larvae of 9–13 mm length. We tested 36 aphid milkers, 20 hunters, and 16 naïve foragers. It is worth to note that, in contrast with the interaction with dead prey, when the behavior of red wood ants depends on the size of the group (Szczuka and Godziñska, 2004), they behave very actively in pairwise interaction with living enemies and competitors (Reznikova and Dorosheva, 2004).

We recorded ten elements of agonistic behavior of ants (ordered here by the degree of aggressiveness): (i) “avoidance”: running away; (ii) “ignoring”: an ant comes close to a larva and does not pay any attention to it; (iii/iv) “antennation”: investigation of a larva by antennae with open/closed mandibles; (v) “hit-and-run attack”: a sudden lunge toward a larva with open mandibles followed by a motion backward; (vi) “nibbling”: short series of touching the enemy with open mandibles; (vii) “short bites” (lasting 1–5 s); (viii) “prolonged bites” (more than 5 s); (ix) “mortal grip”: prolonged capturing of the larva using mandibles and legs usually accompanied by spraying the larva with acid; (x) “transporting”: the ant carries the larva in its mandibles and runs around. We also recorded self-grooming behavior in ants, that is, the behavior of cleaning the body, legs, and antennae. We split behavioral elements into two groups: “non-aggressive” (i–iv) and “aggressive” (v–x).

Two behavioral elements of the target hoverfly larvae were recorded: (i) “freezing” motionless and (ii) “defensive flexion of the anterior body part” where the larva bent its head toward the attacking ant. The last element could be accompanied with a releasing of defensive adhesive secretion from the mouth which hardens like glue. Being glued by a larva, an ant stops its attack and has to clean itself for a time.

Studying Ants’ Learning Abilities to Avoid Unpleasant Encounters With an Enemy

We studied short-term and long-term learning abilities of ants to avoid the nuisance of contacts with the chemically defensive enemy. To examine short-term learning (less than 10 min after gaining negative experience), we compared two ants’ behaviors toward a hoverfly larva during the first tests before and after the larvae glued the ant. Sometimes the larva glued the ant 2–3 times during the first test; in this case, we considered only the behavior of the ant after the first “gluing.”

To examine long-term learning, we conducted the follow-up experiment with an interval of 1–3 days. To show that the training of different groups took place in a similar mode, we checked the differences between the groups by inter-test intervals. No significant differences in the inter-test intervals between ant groups were observed: ANOVA, F2,25 = 0.309, p = 0.737; means ± SEM: 1.8 ± 0.3 days for aphid milkers, 2.0 ± 0.4 days for hunters, 1.7 ± 0.3 days for naïve foragers. Thus, we can compare the ability for long-term learning between groups of ants. Out of the ants that were glued by syrphids and thus obtained negative experience in the first test, we selected for the follow-up experiment nine aphid milkers, seven hunters, and twelve naïve foragers. Each syrphid larva was used only once. Test containers were washed with ethanol after each test. In total 100 tests were conducted, and the whole time of observation made up about 17 h.

Data Analysis

We measured ants’ behavior during contacts with the enemy assigning each behavior a score according to the following order of aggressiveness: 1 – avoidance, 2 – ignoring, 3 – antennation, 4 – short attack (hit-and-run attack, nibbling, and short bites), 5 – prolonged attack (prolonged bites, mortal grip, and transporting). For each test, the level of ants’ aggressiveness was calculated as the sum of the scores of all behaviors of the ant divided by the total number of ant-syrphid contacts during the test. If an ant demonstrated a range of behavioral elements, quickly changing from one to another during its contact with the larva, we analyzed the most aggressive elements only. When analyzing short-term learning, aggression scores were counted only for two encounters, before, and after the larva glued the and for the first time, that is, we considered only the very first chemical defensive reaction of the larvae. We calculated the occurrence of distinct behavioral elements for each ant group as the percentage of ants that demonstrated a distinct behavioral element toward hoverfly larvae from the total number of ants tested.

Since the data were normally distributed (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, p > 0.05), we used parametric tests. Comparison of the levels of aggressiveness between groups of ants was performed using univariate analysis of variance (one-way ANOVA, Tukey HSD post hoc test). The significance of differences in the occurrence of behavioral elements was determined using Fisher’s exact test. To compare the behavior of the same ants at different stages of the experiment, the t-test for paired samples was used. All results were analyzed using Microsoft Excel and SPSS v.22.

Results

Behavior of Ants and Hoverfly Larvae in Dyadic Interactions

During their encounters with ants, hoverfly larvae demonstrated freezing and active chemical defense, including flexion of their anterior body part toward the attacking ant and releasing a sticky secretion over it (Figures 1A–C). The behavior of syrphids depended on the aggressiveness of the ants. In 54 of the 67 encounters, the larvae demonstrated the defensive behavior in response to the following aggressive behaviors of ants: bites (39 out of 67), nibbling (12 out of 67), and hit-and-run attacks (3 out of 67) (Figure 1A). After being touched by the mouthparts of the syrphid and getting on the body of syrphids’ defensive secrets (Figure 1B), the ants walked in a jerky or staggering way, rubbing their mandibles over the ground, and thoroughly cleaned their antennae (Figure 1C). In the first test, the syrphid larvae “glued” a similar portion of “field” ants in each task group, 17 out of 36 aphid milkers and 11 out of 20 hunters (Fisher’s exact test, p > 0.05), and significantly higher portion of naïve foragers (12 out of 16) relative to “field” aphid milkers (Fisher’s exact test, p < 0.05).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The behavior of red wood ants and hoverfly larvae in pairwise interactions. (A) Ant bites the enemy. (B) Hoverfly larva shows active defense by releasing a droplet of viscous and sticky secretion from the mouth, which hardens like glue. (C) Ant stops attacking to clean itself.

We compared the level of aggressiveness and occurrence of behavioral elements in aphid milkers, hunters, and naïve foragers. First, we found an essential difference between both groups of “field” ants and the naïve foragers during the first encounters with syrphids (Figure 2). During the first interaction with this enemy in their lives, naïve foragers showed a significantly higher level of aggressiveness than the members of a field colony (F2,70 = 7.03, p = 0.002, Tukey HSD, p = 0.003; Figure 2A). Naïve foragers applied the mortal grip and nibbling toward the enemy with a significantly higher frequency, whereas members of both “field” groups tried to avoid encounters with the larvae (Figure 2B). In comparison with “field” hunters, naïve foragers applied prolonged bites much more often, and less frequently they investigated the larvae with their antennae, keeping mandibles closed (Figure 2B). It is worth to note that the transition from the prolonged attack to transporting a syrphid larva occurred much more frequently in the naïve foragers (Figure 2B). Of the seven naïve foragers who applied a mortal grip on the larva, five ants bore it, and two of them killed the enemy. Both “field” aphid milkers and hunters showed similar levels of aggressiveness (F2,70 = 7.03, p = 0.002, Tukey HSD, p > 0.05; Figure 2A) and a similar occurrence of behavioral elements (Figure 2B). Most of the “field” ants showed non-aggressive behaviors toward syrphids (such as avoidance, ignoring, and antennation), and no more than 20% of both aphid milkers and hunters showed attack behaviors such as hit-and-run attacks, nibbling, bites, and the mortal grip (Figure 2B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differences in (A) the level of aggressiveness and (B) the occurrence of behavioral elements between aphid milkers, hunters, and naïve foragers during their interactions with hoverfly larvae in the first test. Significant differences are marked with asterisks: (A) mean ± SEM, one-way ANOVA, p < 0.01, Tukey HSD, p < 0.01; (B) Fisher’s exact test, p < 0.05.

Short-Term Aversive Learning in Ants

To examine the short-term aversive learning in ants, we compared two behaviors of ants toward syrphid larvae during the 10-min test: before and after the first gluing the ant by the larva. After cleaning of larva’s secretion, the aphid milkers reacted to the enemy much less aggressively (t(11) = 2.561, p < 0.05; Figure 3). The hunters and naïve foragers did not change their aggressiveness after cleaning of larva’s secretion (t(6) = 0.42 and t(6) = 0, correspondingly, p > 0.05; Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Short-term changes in the level of aggressiveness in aphid milkers, hunters, and naïve foragers before and after the hoverfly larva glued the ant during the first test. Significant differences are marked with asterisks (mean ± SEM; t-test for paired samples, p < 0.05).

Long-Term Aversive Learning in Ants

To examine the shaping of long-term memory in ants about the nuisance of encounters with hoverfly larvae releasing droplets of viscous and sticky secretion, we conducted follow-up experiments after 1–3 days since the first one. The aphid milkers, who had a negative experience of interaction with the larva in the first test, behaved much less aggressively in the second test (t(8) = 2.943, p < 0.05; Figure 4A). The occurrence of “ignoring” increased in their behavioral patterns, and the occurrence of “antennation” decreased (Figure 4B). In contrast to the aphid milkers, the hunters and the naïve foragers, who had a negative experience of interaction with the larva in the first test, showed the same level of aggressiveness in the second test (t(6) = -0.956 and t(11) = -1.245, correspondingly, p > 0.05; Figure 4A). Both hunters and naïve foragers demonstrated a similar occurrence of different behavioral elements during the first and the follow-up experiments (Figures 4C,D).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Long-term changes in (A) the level of aggressiveness and the occurrence of behavioral elements in (B) aphid milkers, (C) hunters, and (D) naïve foragers between the first and the follow-up tests. Significant differences are marked with asterisks: (A) mean ± SEM, t-test for paired samples, p < 0.05; (B–D) Fisher’s exact test, p < 0.05.

Discussion

In our experiments with F. aquilonia, during the first interaction with the syrphid larva in their lives, the naïve foragers showed a significantly higher level of aggressiveness than the members of a natural colony. Naïve foragers applied the mortal grip, prolonged bites, and nibbling toward the enemy with significantly more frequency, whereas members of both “field” groups behaved more carefully and tried to avoid encounters with the larva. The aphid milkers, who had a negative experience of interaction with the larva, being “glued” with its viscous secretion, behaved much less aggressively in the follow-up experiments after 10 min and even 3 days, thus shaping both short- and long-term memories. However, both “field” hunters and naïve foragers demonstrated no signs of aversive learning. As far as we know, this study provides the first link between the natural aversive learning performance and task specialization in ants. A question then arises, what behavioral and cognitive traits of the members of different task groups are responsible for the differences in their abilities to form natural aversive learning.

First, the difference in the previous experience could influence the abilities to form natural aversive learning in the hunters and the aphid milkers in our experiments with hoverfly larvae. It has been recently demonstrated that in the red wood ant F. rufa workers inside the nest interact more frequently with the myrmecophile parasites than foragers, and prior encounter and greater experience in attacking these parasites could cause nurses and mound workers to recognize them more rapidly as a threat than foragers would do and to have a lower threshold to initiate aggression (Parmentier et al., 2015). In our experiments, aphid milkers taken from aphid colonies on a tree were more likely to have experience of previous encounters with the hoverfly larvae and other aphidophages such as ladybird larvae, all chemically defensive, whereas land hunters were unlikely to encounter these insects, and naïve foragers completely lacked the previous experience of interaction with chemically defensive insects. This assumption, although indirect, is also confirmed by differences in the behavior of hunters and naïve foragers toward the enemy. In our experiments, naïve foragers of 13–14 months of age, applied much more aggressive reactions toward the hoverfly larvae than the members of both “field” groups. During the follow-up experiments, naïve foragers behaved less carefully than “field” hunters. Not only did they not learn to avoid the syrphids, but in some cases, they passed on to predatory behavior toward these insects. Since syrphid larvae may occasionally be part of the ant diet (Punttila et al., 2004), naïve foragers can recognize them as “a general image of a victim” such as many larvae of other insects permanently found in the ants’ prey (Iakovlev et al., 2017), rather than “an enemy image.” The hoverfy larva, with its relatively safe gluing secretion, is much less dangerous than, say, predatory ground beetles who can kill the ants, and also have chemical protection. It has been demonstrated earlier that red wood ants possess an innate template for perception and identification of an “enemy image” including such features of the predatory ground beetles as dark coloration, the size, the presence of “outgrowths” (legs, antennae), body symmetry, the rate of movement, and scent (Dorosheva et al., 2011; Reznikova and Dorosheva, 2013). However, the ability to single out the key features and complete the integral image seems to require accumulation of experience (Reznikova and Iakovlev, 2008), and hunters are much more cautious toward the ground beetles than both nest guards and naïve workers (Iakovlev, 2010). In the present study, “field” hunters, although did not learn to avoid the negative encounters with hoverfly larvae, behaved more careful than naïve foragers, perhaps based on their experience of collisions with some chemically defensive insects on the soil surface.

The second reason explaining differences in the behavior of representatives of different ants’ task groups may be the manifestation of a set of task-specific behavioral features. It was demonstrated earlier that in red wood ants members of each task group possess a stable set of distinct behavioral characteristics (Reznikova, 2011). Experiments of Iakovlev (2010) with the battery of behavioral tests, in which ants interacted with artificial models of natural objects as well as with live predatory beetles, revealed particular suits of behavioral features of the members of different task groups. For instance, aphid milkers display a high level of exploratory activity with the preference for artificial grass stems, low aggressiveness, and evasion of contacts with the predators. At the sight of a stuffed blue tit, guards react aggressively and try to bite, whereas aphid milkers continue milking aphids or jump from the branch down (Iakovlev, 2010; Reznikova, 2011, 2017). Hunters display much more agility and aggressiveness than aphid milkers, and the low level of exploration activity (Iakovlev, 2010). There are similarities in behavioral traits of hunters and nest guards; however, the hunters are more careful, and in contrast to the nest guards, they never use the most dangerous methods of dealing with predatory beetles, such as mortal grip and long bites (Reznikova and Iakovlev, 2008). Similar correlations between behavior and specialization were revealed in other ant species. In Myrmica rubra colonies patrollers are bolder, more aggressive and more active than foragers and brood carers (Chapman et al., 2011). In Camponotus aethiops, the exploratory activity of workers in the open field significantly predict learning performance: “active-explorers” were slower in appetitive olfactory learning than “inactive-explorers,” and they are also more aggressive (Udino et al., 2017). In the present experiments, the ability of aphid milkers to learn how to avoid hoverfly larva can be associated with their low aggressiveness and conflict avoidance. It is likely that battles with aphidophages are left to the aphid guards who belong to the same aphid tending group and possess similar behavioral traits with the nest guards (Reznikova and Novgorodova, 1998a).

In sum, previous experience, innate level of aggressiveness and exploration, and templates of vital attention objects, together shape the difference in learning between representatives of different task groups. It is difficult, if not impossible, to separate the task specialization from the experience gained. Naive foragers in our experiments can serve as an example. Lacking experience with natural enemies, competitors and symbionts, having only vague templates of food and enemy images, they applied highly aggressive reactions toward dangerous animals, and did not learn how to avoid encounters. Keeping in mind the high cognitive abilities of scouts as the most intelligent task group in red wood ants’ colony described earlier (Reznikova and Ryabko, 2011), one can suggest that individual variation in aggressiveness, peculiarities of exploratory activity, orientation, learning, and memory underlies the specialization of workers in performing various tasks. Given our previous results, we speculate that scouts, and aphid milkers are the most cognitively gifted task groups in red wood ants, whereas hunters and guards are rather brave than smart.

There is much work to be done to evaluate the fitness consequences of deep professional specialization in red wood ants. The role distribution is somewhat rigid in these species. In our experiments with aphid milkers and aphid guards belonging to the same aphid tending group, when ants were experimentally forced to change their roles, much food was lost (Reznikova and Novgorodova, 1998a; Reznikova, 2007). In species with small colony sizes of about 100–300 workers such as Temnothorax species, specialists are no better at their jobs than generalists, and sometimes even perform worse. In addition, most of the work in the colony is not performed by the most efficient workers (Dornhaus, 2008). Moreover, strict specialization is disadvantageous for a colony’s annual reproduction and growth during slave raids (Jongepier and Foitzik, 2016). It is possible that in red wood ants, with their colony sizes up to million individuals, the effectiveness of deep professional specialization is connected with their sophisticated communication system based on transferring from scouts to foragers the exact messages about the coordinates of remote goals (Reznikova, 2017). We suggest that further study of the distribution of cognitive responsibilities within colonies of different sizes and levels of specialization in different ant species may help to revise our understanding of the benefits of colony organization.

Author Contributions

II conducted the experiments with insects. II and ZR discussed all problems and wrote the manuscript. ZR took part in the planning and conducting the experiments.

Funding

This study was funded by Russian Fund for Basic Research (No. 17-04-00702) and by The Federal Fundamental Scientific Research Program for 2013–2020 No. VI.51.1.10 (AAAA-A16-116121410120-0; Grant No. 0-109-2018-0074).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Daniil Ryabko for the helpful discussion and useful comments. We appreciate the efforts and valuable comments of two reviewers who helped us to improve the manuscript.

References

Addicott, J. F. (1978). Competition for mutualists: aphids and ants. Can. J. Zool. 56, 2093–2096. doi: 10.1007/BF00384310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Atsarkina, N. V., Iakovlev, I. K., and Reznikova, Z. (2014). Individual behavioural features of scouts and recruits in red wood ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Evraziat. Entomol. 13, 209–218.

Google Scholar

Bernays, E. A. (1993). “Aversion learning and feeding,” in Insect Learning, eds D. R. Papaj and A. C. Lewis (Boston, MA: Springer), 1–17.

Google Scholar

Beshers, S. N., Huang, Z. Y., Oono, Y., and Robinson, G. E. (2001). Social inhibition and the regulation of temporal polyethism in honey bees. J. Theor. Biol. 213, 461–479. doi: 10.1006/jtbi.2001.2427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chapman, B. B., Thain, H., Coughlin, J., and Hughes, W. O. (2011). Behavioural syndromes at multiple scales in Myrmica ants. Anim. Behav. 82, 391–397. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.05.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dejean, A. (1988). Memory effect on predatory behaviour of Odontomachus troglodytes (Formicidae, Ponerinae). Behaviour 107, 131–137. doi: 10.1163/156853988X00313

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Detrain, C., Fichaux, M., and Verheggen, F. (2017). Tuned protection of aphids by ants against a predatory hoverfly. Ecol. Entomol. 42, 235–244. doi: 10.1111/een.12378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dlussky, G. M. (1967). Ants of the Genus Formica. Moscow: Nauka.

Google Scholar

Dobrzanska, J. (1958). Partition of foraging grounds and modes of conveying information among ants. Acta Biol. Exp. 18, 55–67.

Google Scholar

Doering, G. N., Kamath, A., Wright, C. M., and Pruitt, J. N. (2018). Evidence for contrasting size-frequency distributions of workers patrolling vegetation vs. the ground in the polymorphic African ant Anoplolepis custodiens. Insects Soc. 65, 663–668. doi: 10.1007/s00040-018-0645-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dornhaus, A. (2008). Specialization does not predict individual efficiency in an ant. PLoS Biol. 6:e285. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dorosheva, E. A., Yakovlev, I. K., and Reznikova, Z. I (2011). An innate template for enemy recognition in red wood ants. Entomol. Rev. 91, 274–280. doi: 10.1134/S0013873811020151

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisner, T. (1972). Chemical ecology: on arthropods and how they live as chemists. Zool. Ges. Verh. 65, 123–137.

Google Scholar

Giehr, J., Grasse, A. V., Cremer, S., Heinze, J., and Schrempf, A. (2017). Ant queens increase their reproductive efforts after pathogen infection. R. Soc. Open Sci. 4:170547. doi: 10.1098/rsos.170547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giraldo, Y. M., and Traniello, J. F. (2014). Worker senescence and the sociobiology of aging in ants. Behav. Ecol. Soc. 68, 1901–1919. doi: 10.1007/s00265-014-1826-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Helanterä, H., Aehle, O., Roux, M., Heinze, J., and d’Ettorre, P. (2013). Family-based guilds in the ant Pachycondyla inversa. Biol. Lett. 9:20130125. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2013.0125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hénaut, Y., Machkour-M’Rabet, S., and Lachaud, J. P. (2014). The role of learning in risk-avoidance strategies during spider–ant interactions. Anim. Cogn. 17, 185–195. doi: 10.1007/s10071-013-0651-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hölldobler, B., and Wilson, E. O. (1990). The Ants. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-10306-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hollis, K. L., McNew, K., Sosa, T., Harrsch, F. A., and Nowbahari, E. (2017). Natural aversive learning in Tetramorium ants reveals ability to form a generalizable memory of predators’ pit traps. Behav. Process. 139, 19–25. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2017.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horstmann, K. (1973). Untersuchungen zur Arbeitsteilung unter den Außendienst-arbeiterinnen der Waldameise Formica polyctena Foerster. Z. Tierpsychol. 32, 532–543.

Google Scholar

Iakovlev, I. K. (2010). Ethological aspects of task specialization in red wood ant (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) colonies. Proc. Russ. Entomol. Soc. 81, 180–187.

Google Scholar

Iakovlev, I. K., Novgorodova, T. A., Tiunov, A. V., and Reznikova, Z. I (2017). Trophic position and seasonal changes in the diet of the red wood ant Formica aquilonia as indicated by stable isotope analysis. Ecol. Entomol. 42, 263–272. doi: 10.1111/een.12384

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jongepier, E., and Foitzik, S. (2016). Fitness costs of worker specialization for ant societies. Proc. Biol. Sci. 283:20152572. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.2572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholson, D. J., Judd, S. P., Cartwright, B. A., and Collett, T. S. (1999). Learning walks and landmark guidance in wood ants (Formica rufa). J. Exp. Biol. 202, 1831–1838.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Novgorodova, T. A. (2015). Role of social and individual experience in interaction of the meadow ant Formica pratensis (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) with ladybird imagines and hoverfly larvae. Insect Sci. 22, 440–450. doi: 10.1111/1744-7917.12127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Novgorodova, T. A., and Gavrilyuk, A. V. (2012). The degree of protection different ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) provide aphids (Hemiptera: Aphididae) against aphidophages. Eur. J. Entomol. 109, 187–196. doi: 10.14411/eje.2012.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oster, G. F., and Wilson, E. O. (1978). Caste and Ecology in the Social Insects. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Google Scholar

Otto, D. (1958). Uber die Arbcitsteilung im Staate von Formica rufa rufo-pratensis minor GoRw. und ihrc verhaltcnsphysiologischen Grundlagen. Wiss. Abh. 30, 1–166..

Google Scholar

Parmentier, T., Dekoninck, W., and Wenseleers, T. (2015). Context-dependent specialization in colony defence in the red wood ant Formica rufa. Anim. Behav. 103, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.02.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Punttila, P., Niemelä, P., and Karhu, K. (2004). The impact of wood ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) on the structure of invertebrate community on mountain birch (Betula pubescens ssp. czerepanovii). Ann. Zool. Fenn. 41, 429–446.

Google Scholar

Ravary, F., Lecoutey, E., Kaminski, G., Châline, N., and Jaisson, P. (2007). Individual experience alone can generate lasting division of labor in ants. Curr. Biol. 17, 1308–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.06.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z. (2007). Animal Intelligence: From Individual to Social Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z. (2008). Experimental paradigms for studying cognition and communication in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol. News 11, 201–214.

Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z. (2011). Division of labour and communication at the individual level in highly social Formica ants (Hymenoptera. Formicidae). Russ. Entomol. J. 20, 315–319. doi: 10.15298/rusentj.20.3.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z. (2017). Studying Animal Languages without Translation: An Insight from Ants. New York, NY: Springer, doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-44918-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z. (2018). “Ants – individual and social cognition,” in Field and Laboratory Methods in Animal Cognition. A Comparative Guide, eds N. Bueno-Guerra and F. Amici (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 8–30. doi: 10.1017/9781108333191.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Dorosheva, E. (2013). Catalog learning: carabid beetles learn to manipulate with innate coherent behavioral patterns. Evol. Psychol. 11:147470491301100304. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100304

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Dorosheva, H. (2004). Impacts of red wood ants Formica polyctena on the spatial distribution and behavioural patterns of ground beetles (Carabidae). Pedobiologia 48, 15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.pedobi.2003.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Iakovlev, I. K. (2008). Development of aggression as a possible basis of “professional” specialization in ants. Dokl. Biol. Sci. 418, 56–58. doi: 10.1134/S0012496608010195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., Levenets, J., Panteleeva, S., and Ryabko, B. (2017). Studying hunting behaviour in the striped field mouse using data compression. Acta Ethol. 20, 165–173. doi: 10.1007/s10211-017-0260-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Novgorodova, T. A. (1998a). Division of labour and exchange of information within ant settlements. Usp. Sovrem. Biol. 118, 345–357.

Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Novgorodova, T. A. (1998b). The importance of individual and social experience for interaction between ants and symbiotic aphids. Dokl. Biol. Sci. 359, 173–175.

Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Ryabko, B. (1994). Experimental study of the ants’ communication system with the application of the Information Theory approach. Mem. Zool. 48, 219–236.

Google Scholar

Reznikova, Z., and Ryabko, B. (2011). Numerical competence in animals, with an insight from ants. Behaviour 148, 405–434. doi: 10.1163/000579511X568562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, G. E. (1992). Regulation of division of labor in insect societies. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 37, 637–665. doi: 10.1146/annurev.en.37.010192.003225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosengren, R., and Fortelius, W. (1987). Trail communication and directional recruitment to food in red wood ants (Formica). Ann. Zool. Fenn. 24, 137–146.

Google Scholar

Rosengren, R., and Sundström, L. (1987). Foraging system of a red wood ant colony (Formica s. str.) - collecting and defending food through an extended phenotype. Exp. Suppl. 54, 117–137.

Google Scholar

Rotheray, G. E. (1986). Colour, shape and defence in aphidophagous syrphid larvae (Diptera). Zool. J. Linn. Soc. 88, 201–216. doi: 10.1111/j.1096-3642.1986.tb01188.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwander, T., Rosset, H., and Chapuisat, M. (2005). Division of labour and worker size polymorphism in ant colonies: the impact of social and genetic factors. Behav. Ecol. Soc. 59, 215–221. doi: 10.1007/s00265-005-0027-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Skinner, G. J., and Whittaker, J. B. (1981). An experimental investigation of inter-relationships between the wood-ant (Formica rufa) and some tree-canopy herbivores. J. Anim. Ecol. 50, 313–326. doi: 10.2307/4047

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Szczuka, A., and Godziñska, E. J. (2004). The role of group size in the control of expression of predatory behavior in workers of the red wood ant Formica polyctena (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Sociobiology 43, 295–326.

Google Scholar

Tripet, F., and Nonacs, P. (2004). Foraging for work and age-based polyethism: the roles of age and previous experience on task choice in ants. Ethology 110, 863–877. doi: 10.1111/j.1439-0310.2004.01023.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Udino, E., Perez, M., Carere, C., and d’Ettorre, P. (2017). Active explorers show low learning performance in a social insect. Curr. Zool. 63, 555–560. doi: 10.1093/cz/zow101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, E. O. (1980). Caste and division of labor in leaf-cutter ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae: Atta). Behav. Ecol. Soc. 7, 157–165. doi: 10.1007/BF00299521

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: ants, specialization, task, syrphid, aversive learning, aggressive behavior

Citation: Iakovlev I and Reznikova Z (2019) Red Wood Ants Display Natural Aversive Learning Differently Depending on Their Task Specialization. Front. Psychol. 10:710. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00710

Received: 11 November 2018; Accepted: 13 March 2019;
Published: 29 March 2019.

Edited by:

Thomas Bugnyar, University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

Lucia Regolin, University of Padova, Italy
Noam Miller, Wilfrid Laurier University, Canada

Copyright © 2019 Iakovlev and Reznikova. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ivan Iakovlev, ivaniakovlev@gmail.com orcid.org/0000-0002-4639-3343

orcid.org/0000-0001-6585-9463

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.