Skip to main content
Log in

Human capabilities, mild autism, deafness and the morality of embryo selection

  • Scientific Contribution
  • Published:
Medicine, Health Care and Philosophy Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A preimplantation genetic test to discriminate between severe and mild autism spectrum disorder might be developed in the foreseeable future. Recently, the philosophers Julian Savulescu and Guy Kahane claimed that there are strong reasons for prospective parents to make use of such a test to prevent the birth of children who are disposed to autism or Asperger’s disorder. In this paper we will criticize this claim. We will discuss the morality of selection for mild autism in embryo selection in a hypothetical in vitro fertilization (IVF) situation where preimplantation genetic diagnosis is performed and compare this with a similar selection for congenital deafness. To do this we first discuss relevant human differences. We then introduce the principle of human capabilities (PC) and compare this principle with the principle of procreative beneficence (PB) introduced by Savulescu and Kahane. We apply the two principles to selection for mild autism and selection for congenital deafness. We argue that PC allows for the selection for mild autism but rules out selection for congenital deafness. PB will not give clear answers; the ruling of PB depends to a large extent on expected social, cultural and political developments. We will argue that PC is preferable to PB. We will discuss arguments for the value of mild autism for individuals who have this condition and argue that they are able to lead a life with human dignity provided autism-friendly social circumstances are present. Neither PC nor PB yields strong reasons for prospective parents to seek to prevent the birth of children who are disposed to mild autism spectrum disorder.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In a fertilized human egg (an early embryo) consisting of up to eight cells, one of the cells can be removed for genetic investigation. That cell is destroyed in the process, but the rest of the embryo develops in a normal way.

  2. The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (fourth edition, text revision) states that the primary diagnostic abnormalities in autistic disorder are qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairments in communication, and restricted repetitive and stereotyped patterns of behavior, interests, and activities. Asperger’s disorder is at the mild end of autism spectrum disorders, and severe autistic disorder is at the other end. Asperger’s disorder differs from ‘classic’ autism in that those diagnosed with Asperger’s do not show evidence of intellectual deficiency or language delay.

  3. The term ‘neurotypical’ is used by people with autism spectrum conditions for people without such conditions.

  4. A neurotypical person appears to be able to change into a person with autistic-like behavior. When young children are reared in depriving circumstances, they may develop post-institutional autistic syndrome, also called ‘quasi-autism’ (Rutter et al. 2010).

  5. Contemporary research suggests the presence of minicolumnopathy (minicolumnar abnormalities in the brain) in autism (Williams and Casanova 2010; Casanova et al. 2010).

  6. According to Kruijver et al. (2000), there may be a neurobiological basis of gender identity disorder. Recent research using magnetic resonance imaging sheds a different light on this matter. “[r]egional gray matter variation in MTF (male to female) transsexuals is more similar to the pattern found in men than in women (Luders et al. 2009).” However, Luders et al. support the assumption that brain anatomy plays a role in gender identity.

  7. According to Garcia-Falgueras and Swaab (2010), our gender identity (the conviction of belonging to the male or female gender) is intrauterinely programmed or organized into our brain structures.

  8. This ‘psychological’ explanation is highly debatable. “Some studies, […], suggest that instead of being preconditions for the development of GID (Gender Identity Disorder), psychiatric problems may be the consequences of persistent psychological difficulties due to the incongruence of biological sex and gender identity on the one hand and the social rejection on the other (Lajos et al. 2011)”.

  9. Although this is a very influential theory, we do not wish to suggest that the extreme male brain theory of autism has reached scientific consensus.

  10. “All we know about the extreme female brain is that, […] it is predicted to arise. […] Their empathizing would be significantly better than other people in the general population, but their systemizing would be impaired. These would be people who have difficulty understanding maths or physics or machines or chemistry as systems, but who are extremely good at tuning into others’ feelings and thoughts (Baron-Cohen 2002b, p. 253)”.

  11. “Through a mutation, a recessive gene brought out by inbreeding, a form of hereditary deafness existed for 250 years on Martha’s Vineyard, Massachusetts, following the arrival of the first deaf settlers in the 1690’s. By the mid nineteenth century […] the incidence of deafness had risen to one in four. In response to this, the entire community learned Sign, and there was free and complete intercourse between the hearing and the deaf. Indeed the deaf were scarcely seen as ‘deaf’, and certainly not seen as being at all ‘handicapped’ (Sacks 1989, p. 33)”.

  12. The blind hen analogy may seem a bit farfetched. However, we believe it serves pretty well to illustrate the intuition that is at work in the (for humans) more important matter of choosing embryos without one or more central human functional capabilities. The function of the analogy is not to convince, but merely to compare it with the intuition (‘emotion’ or ‘upheaval of thought’) at work in the case of choosing embryos without one or more central human functional capabilities.

  13. “All these capabilities are implicit in the idea of a life worthy of human dignity (Nussbaum 2006, p. 70).” “Any child born into a species has the dignity relevant to that species, whether or not it seems to have the “basic capabilities” relevant to that species. For that reason, it should also have all the capabilities relevant to the species […] (Nussbaum 2006, p. 347)”.

  14. Emphasis added.

  15. e.g. autosomal recessive deafness 1A (DFNB1A) (Barashkov et al. 2011).

  16. See e.g. Clarke and van Amerom 2008.

  17. Assuming there is a genetic marker for homosexuality.

References

  • Ali, A., and K.M. Cheng. 1985. Early egg production in genetically blind (rc/rc) chickens in comparison with sighted (Rc/rc) controls. Poultry Science 64: 789–794.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Allred, S. 2009. Reframing Asperger syndrome: Lessons from other challenges to the diagnostic and statistical manual and ICIDH approaches. Disability and Society 24(3): 343–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barashkov, N.A., L.U. Dzhemileva, S.A. Fedorova, F.M. Teryutin, O.L. Posukh, E.E. Fedotova, S.L. Lobov, and E.K. Khusnutdinova. 2011. Autosomal recessive deafness 1A (DFNB1A) in Yakut population isolate in Eastern Siberia: extensive accumulation of the splice site mutation IVS1 + 1G > A in GJB2 gene as a result of founder effect. Journal of Human Genetics 56(9): 631–639.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barnbaum, Deborah.R. 2008. Ethics of autism: Among them, but not of them. Bloomington, IN, USA: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J., and E. Clubley. 2001. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31(1): 5–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • S. Baron-Cohen 2002a. Is Asperger Syndrome Necessarily Viewed as a Disability? Focus on autism and other developmental disabilities 2002; 17(3): 186–191.

  • Baron-Cohen, S. 2002. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(6): 253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., S. Wheelwright, A. Burtenshaw, and E. Hobson. 2007. Mathematical talent is linked to autism. Human Nature 18: 125–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., E. Ashwin, C. Ashwin, T. Tavassoli, and B. Chakrabarti. 2010. Talent in autism: hyper- systemizing, hyper-attention to detail and sensory hyper-sensitivity. In Autism and talent, ed. Francesca. Happé, and Uta. Frith. Oxford: Oxford University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Lombardo, M.V., Auyeung, B., Ashwin, E., Chakrabarti, B., and R. Knickmeyer. 2011. Why are autism spectrum conditions more prevalent in males? PLoS Biol 9(6):e1001081. Epub 2011 Jun 14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. 2011. The science of evil. New York: Basic books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brühwiler, C., and P. Blatchford. 2011. Effects of class size and adaptive teaching competency on classroom processes and academic outcome. Learning & Instruction 21(1): 95–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D.B. 2010. Advances and challenges in the genetics of autism. Focus 8(3): 339–349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casanova, M.F., A. El-Baz, E. Vanbogaert, P. Narahari, and A. Switala. 2010. A topographic study of minicolumnar core width by lamina comparison between autistic subjects and controls: possible minicolumnar disruption due to an anatomical element in-common to multiple laminae. Brain Pathology 20(2): 451–458.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, J., and G. van Amerom. 2008. Asperger’s syndrome. Social Work in Health Care 46(3): 85–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Collins, S., B. Forkman, H.H. Kristensen, P. Sandöe, and P.M. Hocking. 2011. Investigating the importance of vision in poultry: Comparing the behavior of blind and sighted chickens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 133: 60–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, D.S. 1997. Genetic dilemmas and the child’s right to an open future. Hastings Center Report 27(2): 7–15.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, M. 2004. Autism and creativity: is there a link between Autism in men and exceptional ability?. New York: Brunner-Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fung, C.H.M. 2009. Asperger’s and musical creativity: The case of Erik Satie. Personality and Individual Differences 46: 775–783.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia-Falgueras, A., and D.F. Swaab. 2010. Sexual hormones and the brain: an essential alliance for sexual identity and sexual orientation. Endocrinology Development 17: 22–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glover, J. 2006. Choosing children; genes, disability, and design. Oxford: Clarendon press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Grandin, T. 2006. Thinking in pictures: My life with autism, Expanded Edition. Westminster, Maryland: Knopf Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenspan, P. 2004. Emotions, rationality and mind/body. In: Thinking about feeling; contemporary philosophers on emotions, Ed. by R.C. Solomon. Oxford/Oxford University Press.

  • Habermas, J. 2003. The future of human nature. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacking, I. 2009. Autistic autobiography. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364: 1467–1473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., and P. Vital. 2010. What aspects of autism predispose to talent? In Autism and talent, ed. Francesca. Happé, and Uta. Frith. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaarsma, P., and S. Welin. 2012. Autism as a natural human variation: reflections on the claims of the neurodiversity movement. Health Care Analysis 20: 20–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruijver, F.P., Zhou, J.N., Pool, C.W., Hofman, M.A., Gooren, L.J. and D.F. Swaab. 2000. Male-to-female transsexuals have female neuron numbers in a limbic nucleus. The Journal of clinical endocrinology and metabolism 2000; 85(5): 2034–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lajos, S., U. Zsolt, D. Fogd, and P. Czobor. 2011. Dysfunctional core beliefs, perceived parenting behavior and psychopathology in gender identity disorder: A comparison of male-to-female, female-to-male transsexual and nontranssexual control subjects. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 42: 38–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luders, E., Sánchez, F.J., Gaser, C., Toga, A.W., Narr, K.L., Hamilton, L.S., and E. Vilain. 2009. Regional gray matter variation in male-to-female transsexualism. Neuroimage 15; 46(4):904–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, M. 2004 Emotions as Judgments of value and importance. In: Thinking about feeling; contemporary philosophers on emotions, Ed. by R.C. Solomon. Oxford/Oxford University Press.

  • Nussbaum, M. 2006. Frontiers of justice; disability, nationality, species membership. Harvard: Harvard University press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum, M. 2011. Creating capabilities: The human development approach. Cumberland, RI, USA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., E.J. Sonuga-Barke, and J. Castle. 2010. Investigating the impact of early institutional deprivation on development: background and research strategy of the English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) study. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 75(1): 1–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sacks, O. 1989. Seeing voices. A journey into the world of the deaf. Berkeley and Los Angeles, CA, USA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savulescu, J., and G. Kahane. 2009. The moral obligation to create children with the best chance of the best life. Bioethics 23: 274–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, E.L., and M.F. Casanova. 2010. Autism or autisms? Finding the lowest common denominator. Boletín de la Asociación Médica de Puerto Rico 2010; 102(4): 17–24.

Download references

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the anonymous reviewers for valuable comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pier Jaarsma.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Jaarsma, P., Welin, S. Human capabilities, mild autism, deafness and the morality of embryo selection. Med Health Care and Philos 16, 817–824 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-013-9464-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11019-013-9464-6

Keywords

Navigation