Short Communication
Inferring sense of agency from the quantitative aspect of action outcome

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2013.01.006Get rights and content

Abstract

The sense of agency refers to an experience in which one’s own action causes a change in environment. It is strongly modulated by both the contingency between action and its outcome and the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes. Recent studies have suggested that the action outcome can retrospectively modulate action awareness. We suspect that the sense of agency can also be retrospectively modulated. This study examined whether the quantity of action outcome could influence the sense of agency. The participants’ task was to trigger dot motion in a display and rate the extent to which they could control the initiation of dot motion. Independently of both the temporal contiguity between action and its outcome and the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes, the speed of dot motion as an action’s outcome strongly influenced the sense of agency rating. The present study suggests that the sense of agency stems partly from the inference of action efficiency based on the quantitative aspect of action outcome.

Highlights

► We examined whether the sense of agency can be retrospectively modulated. ► The speed of dot motion as action outcome strongly influenced the sense of agency. ► The sense of agency stems partly from the inference of action efficiency.

Introduction

We often have the experience that our own actions trigger changes in the external world. This experience is called the sense of agency (Haggard & Chambon, 2012). A two-stage model of the sense of agency has been proposed (Synofzik, Vosgerau, & Newen, 2008). At the first stage, a non-conceptual “feeling of agency” occurs on the basis of feed-forward cues, proprioception, and sensory feedback. Previous studies have examined how temporal congruency between an action and its effect contributes to the feeling of agency. For example, a temporally proximate action and its outcome can reduce the strength of neural signals of the outcome (i.e., sensory attenuation: Bays et al., 2005, Blakemore et al., 1999, Blakemore et al., 1998, Kühn et al., 2011). This is because when predicted and actual sensory signals are highly contiguous, they cancel each other out (Wolpert, Ghahramani, & Jordan, 1995). The reduction in neural signals for outcome is interpreted as a causal relation between an action and its outcome, triggering a strong sense of agency (Blakemore et al., 1999). Meanwhile, some studies argue that the mere prediction of the outcome may be sufficient to cause the sensory attenuation (Waszak, Cardoso-Leite, & Hughes, 2012). In addition to the sensory attenuation, subjective compression of a temporal interval between an action and effect occurs when the action and effect are temporally contiguous with each other, which is called intentional binding (Haggard, Clark, & Kalogeras, 2002). Intentional binding is also considered as a sign of the feeling of agency (Desantis, Roussel, & Waszak, 2011). It is strong when temporal contiguity between an action and effect is high (Berberian et al., 2012, Ebert and Wegner, 2010, Haggard et al., 2002). In this way, temporal contiguity between an action and effect plays a significant role in determining the feeling of agency.

At the second stage of the model proposed by Synofzik et al. (2008), the judgment of agency arises on the basis of intentions, prior thoughts, social cues, and contextual cues. The judgment of agency is related to propositional representation of agency. The judgment of agency can be assessed, for example, in a rating task in which participants rate the extent to which they are able to cause a change in the environment. Temporal contiguity between an action and effect affects the judgment of agency (Ebert & Wegner, 2010) or causality rating between action and effect (Shanks, Pearson, & Dickson, 1989). In addition to temporal contiguity, the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes (Sato & Yasuda, 2005) and the consistency between prior thoughts about action and actual action outcome (Desantis et al., 2012, Wegner, 2004, Wegner et al., 2004, Wegner and Wheatley, 1999) are also contributing factors to the judgment of agency.

Recently, a study reported that information presented as an action’s outcome altered the action awareness. Wilke, Synofzik, and Linder (2012) demonstrated that the perceived direction of a pointing action was distorted by presenting pictures with emotional valence: Positive and negative pictures, respectively, attracted and repulsed the subjective pointing direction. These results indicate that the perception of an agent’s own action is retrospectively modulated by the action’s outcome. Action awareness is intimately related to the sense of agency, as shown in studies on intentional binding (Haggard et al., 2002). Thus, we anticipated that an action’s outcome might be able to alter the sense of agency.

The present study examined whether the quantitative aspect of an action’s outcome could alter the sense of agency rating. Imagine a baseball situation. You are a batter. When hitting a home run, you will feel a strong efficiency of your action on the ball. On the other hand, when hitting a pop-up, the feeling of efficiency will be low. We wanted to know whether the sense of agency is modulated by the action efficiency implied by the quantitative aspect of action outcome and if so, how. Participants were asked to press one of four pre-defined keys to trigger dot motion in the display (Fig. 1a). We focused on the effect of the speed of dot motion on the sense of agency, with the prediction that a higher speed of dot motion as action outcome would imply a higher efficiency of action on external environments, and thus lead to a higher sense of agency. Here, the action efficiency refers to the extent to which an agent’s action influences the appearance of the external world, not to the degree of consistency between an executed action and its goal (Gergely & Csibra, 2003). On the other hand, a different sort of prediction could be made for the effect of dot motion speed on the sense of agency. The effect of dot motion speed would be dependent on the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes if the dot motion speed is interpreted under the consistency. Specifically, if prediction about motion direction as the action outcome is inconsistent with actual motion direction (for example, if the participant anticipates the appearance of rightward dot motion though the actual outcome is leftward dot motion), the inconsistency between predicted and actual action outcomes would be accentuated by the increase of dot motion speed, and thus the sense of agency would decrease as the dot motion speed increases when prediction for the action outcome is inconsistent with actual action outcome. We simultaneously manipulated three factors – the temporal contiguity between action and outcome, the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes, and the speed of dot motion − and examined how these factors interactively influenced the sense of agency.

Section snippets

Participants

Fifteen paid adults participated in this experiment. They all reported they had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the ethical committee at Nippon Telegraph and Telephone Corporation (NTT Communication Science Laboratories Ethical Committee). The experiments were conducted according to the principles laid down in the Helsinki Declaration. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

Apparatus

Stimuli were presented on a 21-in. CRT

Results

First, for each condition of each individual, we averaged ratings for each condition across four repetitions (i.e., individual mean). Second, we further calculated group mean by averaging individual mean across all participants. The results are plotted in Fig. 1b. A set of individual mean data for all conditions was submitted to a three-way analysis of variance with the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes, dot motion speed, and action-effect delay as factors. The main

Discussion

The present study demonstrated that the quantitative aspect of action’s outcome modulated the sense of agency rating. Specifically, increasing the speed of dot motion stimuli as action’s effect significantly enhanced the sense of agency, independently of the consistency between predicted and actual action outcomes and action outcome temporal contingency.

Interestingly, increasing dot motion speed enhanced the sense of agency even when the direction of dot motion was inconsistent with the

Acknowledgment

We thank two anonymous reviewers for constructive comments on the earlier version of this study.

References (34)

  • A. Sato et al.

    Illusion of sense of self-agency: discrepancy between the predicted and actual sensory consequences of action modulates the sense of self-agency but not the sense of self-ownership

    Cognition

    (2005)
  • M. Synofzik et al.

    Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2008)
  • F. Waszak et al.

    Action effect anticipation: neurophysiological basis and functional consequences

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2012)
  • C. Wilke et al.

    The valence of action outcomes modulates the perception of one’s actions

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2012)
  • B. Berberian et al.

    Automation technology and sense of control: a window on human agency

    PLoS One

    (2012)
  • S.-J. Blakemore et al.

    Spatiotemporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (1999)
  • S.-J. Blakemore et al.

    Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation

    Nature Neuroscience

    (1998)
  • Cited by (33)

    • Moving targets in space: Movement distance as a predictor for experiences of movement agency

      2022, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      The model assumes that agency is experienced when there is a match between the predicted outcomes and the actual outcomes of a performed action (Blakemore et al., 2002; Blakemore, Wolpert, & Frith, 2000; Wolpert, 1997). The spatial prediction account provides a plausible explanation for movement agency (although see Kawabe, 2013). However, in the paradigm used by Hon et al. (2018), spatial distance is potentially confounded with target/non-target movement compatibility.

    • Musical expertise affects the sense of agency: Intentional binding in expert pianists

      2020, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      The sense of agency is the feeling of causing changes in the external world through the control of one’s own voluntary actions (Haggard, 2017; Kawabe, 2013).

    • Does delay in feedback diminish sense of agency? A review

      2019, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, the full system of the sense of agency may be more complicated and involve multiple processes at different cognitive levels. Recent studies have shown that many other factors besides the comparison mechanism, such as action selection (Chambon & Haggard, 2012; Sidarus, Chambon, & Haggard, 2013), intention (Haggard, 2005; Preston & Newport, 2010; Vinding, Pedersen, & Overgaard, 2013; Wen et al., 2016), effort (Howard, Edwards, & Bayliss, 2016; Minohara et al., 2016), emotion (Gentsch, Weiss, Spengler, Synofzik, & Schütz-Bosbach, 2015; Yoshie & Haggard, 2013, 2017), retrospective inference (Kawabe, 2013; Moore & Haggard, 2008), goal achievement (Aarts, Custers, & Wegner, 2005; Villa, Tidoni, Porciello, & Aglioti, 2018; Wen, Yamashita, & Asama, 2015b), and social interaction (Beyer, Sidarus, Bonicalzi, & Haggard, 2017; Caspar, Christensen, Cleeremans, & Haggard, 2016; Caspar, Cleeremans, & Haggard, 2018) greatly contribute to the sense of agency. Further, the cue integration theory suggests that when people make a judgment of agency, multiple cues from different processes are integrated based on a Bayesian principle (Moore & Fletcher, 2012; Synofzik, Vosgerau, & Lindner, 2009).

    • Multi-scale control influences sense of agency: Investigating intentional binding using event-control approach

      2017, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our voluntary actions influence not only our environment but also the conscious mental state of being the agent of those actions, something that is central to all our conscious experiences (Gallagher, 2007). Sense of agency (SoA) is a complex, multifaceted, phenomenon (Pacherie, 2011) that can be described as the feeling of “I did it” (Engbert, Wohlschläger, & Haggard, 2008), the experience of causing a change in the environment by one’s own action (Kawabe, 2013), the registration that the organism is the initiator of his/her own actions (Synofzik, Vosgerau, & Voss, 2013), and as the feeling of one’s voluntary actions causing external events (Takahata et al., 2012). SoA can be understood as syntax or framework within which several different experiences may be accommodated (Engbert et al., 2008).

    • Sense of agency in continuous action: Assistance-induced performance improvement is self-attributed even with knowledge of assistance

      2017, Consciousness and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although we cannot provide conclusive evidence of the mechanism by which multiple external cues contribute to the sense of agency, the present results at least suggest that there is a case where two external cues are independently used in determining the sense of agency. According to this idea, it is possible that in addition to the combination of performance improvements and knowledge of computer assistance, other combinations of external cues, such as priming of action consequences (Aarts, Custers, & Wegner, 2005), quantitative aspects of action consequences (Kawabe, 2013), and the emotional value of action consequences (Yoshie & Haggard, 2013), might also independently contribute to the sense of agency. In this case, the higher-order inference process for determining the sense of agency could be regarded as the accumulation of effects from multiple external cues.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text