Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 29 September 2015
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Bridging Reading Aloud and Speech Production View all 13 articles

The segment as the minimal planning unit in speech production and reading aloud: evidence and implications

  • 1Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA, USA
  • 2Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, Merced, Merced, CA, USA

Speech production and reading aloud studies have much in common, especially the last stages involved in producing a response. We focus on the minimal planning unit (MPU) in articulation. Although most researchers now assume that the MPU is the syllable, we argue that it is at least as small as the segment based on negative response latencies (i.e., response initiation before presentation of the complete target) and longer initial segment durations in a reading aloud task where the initial segment is primed. We also discuss why such evidence was not found in earlier studies. Next, we rebut arguments that the segment cannot be the MPU by appealing to flexible planning scope whereby planning units of different sizes can be used due to individual differences, as well as stimulus and experimental design differences. We also discuss why negative response latencies do not arise in some situations and why anticipatory coarticulation does not preclude the segment MPU. Finally, we argue that the segment MPU is also important because it provides an alternative explanation of results implicated in the serial vs. parallel processing debate.

In reading aloud and speech production experiments, participants produce a single word utterance as the response and thus the last processing stages—phonological encoding (assigning a segment to a position in a metrical frame), phonetic encoding (retrieving the motor plans required for articulation), and articulation (producing the gestures leading to an acoustic response)—are shared. Moreover, the 2 fields became closer as speech production researchers began to use chronometric measures (Meyer, 1992) and word reading researchers began to use error measures (Kello and Plaut, 2000). Also, models integrating both fields were being proposed (Roelofs, 2004).

One aspect of processing common to both fields is the degree to which processing is incremental. Incremental processing can be manifested in two non-mutually exclusive ways: (1) a segment (i.e., a consonant or vowel) as the minimal planning unit (MPU) (Kawamoto et al., 1998; Kawamoto, 1999), and (2) cascaded processing (Kello et al., 2000; Rapp and Goldrick, 2000). In this review, we consider the MPU.

Some researchers argue that articulation cannot begin before the currently queued word or syllable is fully planned, while others contend that articulation can start with just one segment planned. We begin by reviewing a variety of phonological units that have been proposed as the MPU, but focus on the segment. Next, we summarize more evidence for the segment and rebut arguments against the segment. Finally, we discuss how the segment MPU provides alternative interpretations of results relevant to the serial vs. parallel processing debate in reading aloud.

Possible MPUs

Levelt (1989) initially assumed that the MPU was the phonological word—a stress group that may include multiple words. Under this assumption, the phonological word is completely phonologically encoded before it is sent to the phonetic encoding stage. After all syllables have been phonetically encoded, the motor plan for the entire word is sent to the articulator1.

However, most researchers now assume that the syllable is the MPU (Schriefers and Teruel, 1999; Meyer et al., 2003). Under this assumption, the initial syllable is phonetically encoded after it has been phonologically encoded and then executed by the articulators after the entire word has been phonologically encoded. For Levelt's (1989; Levelt et al., 1999a) model, the syllable plays a unique role because the motor plan determined at the phonetic encoding stage is based on the syllable (either retrieved from a mental syllabary or assembled from the motor plans of individual segments). Levelt's speech production model has been implemented as the WEAVER model (Levelt et al., 1999a).

Models of reading aloud have also been implemented as computational models (Coltheart et al., 2001; Kello and Plaut, 2003; Perry et al., 2007). Unlike speech production models, however, models of reading aloud focus on the mapping from the spelling to a phonological representation and typically base response latency predictions on the time to generate the phonological representation.

Segment as the MPU

Various subsyllabic units have been considered as MPUs including the initial consonant(s) or the initial plosive consonant and following vowel (Frederiksen and Kroll, 1976) and the segment, the unit we focus on in this review (MacKay, 1987; Dell et al., 1993; Kawamoto et al., 1998)2. If the segment is the MPU, then the motor plan for the initial segment can be retrieved and executed as soon as it has been phonologically encoded. Segment motor plans are part of Levelt's (1989) model, but are not intended to be executed individually.

It is theoretically straightforward to show that the segment is the MPU if phonological or other processes can be shown to affect absolute response latencies and initial segment durations. However, it is methodologically difficult to do so because many initial segments produce little or no acoustic energy.

Problems Due to Acoustic Characteristics of the Initial Segment

The biggest problem is that the initial part of plosive and affricate segments are silent. In fact, for plosives, acoustic energy is not generated until the end of the segment when the second segment begins. Because there is no acoustic energy throughout the entire plosive segment, acoustic latency (response latency based on acoustic onset) conflates response latency and initial segment duration. Moreover, matching the initial segment across conditions isn't a solution because any factor that affects initial segment duration affects acoustic latency.

The conflation of response latency and initial segment duration extends to voiceless affricates and fricatives if voice-keys are used because voice-keys typically miss the low intensity acoustic energy of these segments (Pechmann et al., 1989; Sakuma et al., 1997). In fact, the first 2 segments might be missed if the target begins with /s/ followed by a plosive (Sakuma et al., 1997; Rastle and Davis, 2002).

The problems with using acoustic energy to assess processing difficulty arise because the onset of acoustic energy is arguably the last event occurring during articulation. Two alternatives are to index response latency to the initiation of muscular activity using electromyography (Riès et al., 2012) or movement of speech articulators (lips and jaw) using video (Kawamoto et al., 2008). The latter was used in the experiment described below.

Negative Response Latencies

To demonstrate that the segment is the MPU, the initial segment of a monosyllabic target word was primed in a reading aloud task (Kawamoto et al., 2014, Expt. 2). The initial letter was followed by underscores and presented for either 300 or 600 ms, at which time the underscores were replaced by the remaining letters of the target. The segment MPU predicts that a response can be initiated before the complete target is presented—resulting in a negative response latency—but the syllable MPU does not. The results below are from the 600 ms condition where there is sufficient time for a response to be initiated. Using acoustic onset, 2.5% of the trials had negative latencies measured from onset of the complete target, all beginning with non-plosives. However, using articulatory onset based on movement of the lips and jaw, 26.2% of the trials had negative latencies and these trials included plosives and non-plosives. These negative latencies provide unequivocal evidence for the segment MPU because the initial segment was provided early and measurements were able to detect its initiation early in the course of articulation.

Initial Segment Duration Differences

Additional evidence for the segment MPU is acoustic durations of responses. Duration effects arise because articulation of the current unit is prolonged while the speaker prepares the following unit to be articulated. Processing effects can be manifested as duration effects in different ways (see Kello, 2004), including duration effects measured across the entire word (Damian, 2003)3. However, a duration effect localized to the initial segment is the strongest evidence for the segment as the MPU (Kawamoto et al., 2014).

Although the duration of the initial segment can be measured directly from the acoustic response only for non-plosives, the duration effect for plosives can be determined indirectly. In particular, the difference in the duration of initial plosive segments, ISDP, in different priming conditions corresponds to the difference in acoustic latency for plosive and non-plosive segments, ALP and ALN, respectively:

ISDPISDP=(ALPALN)(ALPALN),and    (1a)
ISDPISDP=(ALPALP)(ALNALN),    (1b)

where double prime and prime denote the 600 ms and 300 ms prime durations, respectively (see also Kawamoto et al., 1998; Kawamoto, 1999).

Alternatively, the initial plosive duration can be determined by using articulatory onset to approximate the beginning of the segment and acoustic onset as the end of the segment. Using both of these approaches, the duration of plosives was also shown to be longer in the 600 ms than in the 300 ms condition due to early initiation of articulation (Kawamoto et al., 2014).

Rebutting Evidence Against Segment MPU

There are many studies demonstrating that a planning unit larger than the initial segment is used for different stimuli under various experimental conditions. These units include the syllable (Cholin et al., 2011), the initial fragment up to and including the first stressed syllable (Sulpizio et al., 2015), the word (Meyer et al., 2003), two phonological words (Damian and Dumay, 2007), and even the clause (Ferreira and Swets, 2002). These results demonstrate that planning units are variable, and can be as large as the clause.

We argue that the segment remains viable as the MPU because the planning unit varies by individuals, as well as stimuli and experimental design. Two different scenarios can arise with a variable planning scope. In one scenario, an effect can be found assuming a smaller unit than the putative MPU. For example, Damian (2003) found longer word durations when the initial segment was primed as predicted by the segment but not the syllable MPU, but only when a deadline was imposed. In the other scenario, a smaller unit might yield no effect as predicted. For example, monosyllabic words are named as quickly as bisyllabic words when presented in the same block as predicted by syllable and segment MPUs (Meyer et al., 2003; Damian et al., 2010), but more quickly when presented in different blocks as predicted by the word MPU (Meyer et al., 2003). Therefore, the planning unit was ostensibly larger in some studies without finding any effect for the smaller MPU. We further note that a smaller MPU does not always predict shorter latencies; longer latencies can arise if there is competition between different initial syllables (e.g., in assigning stress, Sulpizio et al., 2015) or segments (e.g., in mapping a letter or letters to a phoneme as discussed below).

Another argument is that anticipatory coarticulation precludes the segment MPU because knowledge of upcoming segments is required during articulation and because it is ubiquitous (Levelt et al., 1999b; Rastle et al., 2000). However, Kawamoto and Liu (2007) found that anticipatory coarticulation is not ubiquitous. They had participants utter one member of a minimal pair (still-stool, spill-spool, still-spill, or stool-spool) and found that there was anticipatory coarticulation of the vowel on the initial segment when the vowels were identical, but not when the vowels were different. Moreover, the long interval between articulatory onset and acoustic onset when the initial segment alone is primed (Kawamoto et al., 2014) can be interpreted as coarticulatory effects of the initial segment on the preceding null phoneme.

Implications of the Segment as the MPU

Determining that the segment is the MPU is important in its own right, but it is also important because it provides an alternative account of results in other debates such as whether phonological encoding is purely parallel or has a sequential component. We examine a length effect and a position effect, effects that would be considered straightforward for sequential reading models to account for (e.g., Perry et al., 2007). However, we argue that current sequential models cannot account for the entire pattern of results, but that purely parallel models can if the segment is the MPU and if acoustic characteristics of the initial segment are considered.

Onset Complexity Effect

Researchers have examined whether words with a simple onset consisting of a single consonant have shorter or longer naming latencies than words with a complex onset consisting of two or more consonants. An early study by Frederiksen and Kroll (1976) found that when length was controlled, words with simple onsets had shorter naming latencies than words with complex onsets. However, interpreting these results is complicated by two acoustic characteristics of simple and complex onsets. First, complex onsets in English can only begin with plosives or voiceless fricatives, and many complex onsets beginning with /s/ are followed by a plosive. Second, segments have a shorter duration in a complex onset than in a simple onset (Klatt, 1974; Rastle and Davis, 2002).

Kawamoto and Kello (1999) reexamined the onset complexity effect for monosyllabic targets beginning with /s/. (Fillers beginning with plosives were also included.) In one experiment the second consonant of the complex onset was a plosive, and in another it was a non-plosive. Using measures of acoustic latency based on marking digitized responses, they found that targets with complex onsets had shorter acoustic latencies than targets with simple onsets despite being longer in length. They hypothesized that the inconsistency in their results and Frederiksen and Kroll's (1976) results was due to how acoustic latency was determined. This hypothesis was confirmed by Rastle and Davis (2002) who replicated Kawamoto and Kello's (1999, Expt. 2) results when acoustic latency was based on hand-marking digitized responses, but who found no effect when an integrator voice-key was used, and an opposite effect when a simple voice-key was used (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Acoustic latencies of words with simple vs. complex onsets beginning with /s/ followed by a plosive determined in different ways.

Although the difference in results due to the method of measuring acoustic latency reported by Rastle and Davis has been widely recognized, the theoretical implication of the onset complexity effect has not. In particular, the partially sequential DRC (Coltheart et al., 2001) and CDP+ (Perry et al., 2007) models cannot account for the results (see Table 1) because the sequential rule route of these models processes the input from the beginning to the end of the word 1 letter or 1 grapheme at a time, respectively, independently of other letters and graphemes in the input. Thus, processing takes longer when the input has more letters and graphemes.

However, Kawamoto and Kello argued that the onset complexity result could be accounted for assuming parallel processing if the segment is the MPU. In particular, the initial consonant can be almost any consonant if the 2nd letter is a vowel as it is for simple onsets, but is almost always /s/ if the second segment of a complex onset is a plosive or a nasal consonant. If processing is parallel, the first segment is still being processed when the second segment is being processed. If information about the second segment can influence processing of the first segment, then the initial /s/ of a complex onset followed by a plosive or a nasal consonant should be encoded before the /s/ of a simple onset. Thus, for the segment MPU, articulation can be initiated earlier for targets with complex rather than simple onsets.

Regularity by Position of Regularity Interaction

Monosyllabic English words with irregular pronunciations have longer acoustic latencies than matched words with regular pronunciations. This regularity effect diminishes as the position of the irregular grapheme moves from left to right (Roberts et al., 2003). The authors argue that sequential models such as the DRC model could account for the data, but purely parallel models could not.

However, all the models considered by Roberts and colleagues assume that the MPU is the syllable (or word). Kawamoto et al. (1998) argued that purely parallel models could account for the regularity by position of regularity interaction if plosivity of the initial segment is taken into account and if the segment is the MPU. As illustrated in Figure 1, when the irregular grapheme is at position 1, targets beginning with plosives as well as non-plosives manifest the regularity effect because phonation cannot begin until the initial segment reaches threshold. When the irregular grapheme is at position 2, the 2nd segment takes longer to reach threshold for irregular graphemes than for regular graphemes. However, acoustic latency is longer only for irregular targets beginning with plosives; no effect of regularity is predicted for targets beginning with non-plosives. This interaction of plosivity and regularity for targets with the irregular grapheme at position 2 has been found (Cortese, 1998; Kawamoto et al., 1998). Finally, when the irregular grapheme is at position 3, only targets beginning with an /s/ followed by a plosive might manifest an effect, but only if a voice-key is used. On this account, the regularity effect diminishes from left to right because the proportion of stimuli that manifest an effect that can be detected acoustically diminishes from left to right. Roberts et al. (2003) rejected the account proposed by Kawamoto et al. (1998) because coarticulation was argued to be ubiquitous and thus the segment could not be the MPU. However, the coarticulation argument has been rebutted (see above). Moreover, Roberts and colleagues never provided an account of the regularity by plosivity interaction.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Time course of encoding and articulation for regular and irregular words. The top of the figure shows the putative time-course of phonological encoding of monosyllabic words (1 regular word and 3 irregular words) that are 4 segments long (each segment labeled S1, S2, S3, or S4) assuming a parallel encoding scheme. For the 3 irregular words, the irregular segment which occurs at position 1, 2, or 3, is indicated by an “*.” On each segment's time-course of phonological encoding, the white triangle depicts the increase in activation of the correct segment, and the base of the triangle on the right side of the triangle depicts when that segment reaches threshold. Below the sets of time-courses of encoding at the bottom of the figure are the time-courses of articulation on the same time-scale as the phonological encoding assuming the segment as the planning unit (i.e., the criterion to initiate articulation). The vertical bar corresponds to the point in time when the 1st segment reaches threshold based on the time-course of encoding above, with the white and gray rectangles corresponding to to the duration of the 1st and 2nd segments. The “n” and “p” at the left and right edges of the white rectangle corresponds to the acoustic onset for non-plosive and plosive initial segments, respectively.

Cortese (1998) also reported simulations based on serial and parallel models showing that targets beginning with plosive as well as non-plosive targets predicted a regularity effect at position 2, but not the interaction. We argue that models fail to predict the plosivity by regularity interaction at position 2 because the naming latency predictions assume that the MPU is the syllable (or word) and that the dependent measure is acoustic latency. If the MPU is the segment, sequential and parallel models should account for the plosivity by regularity interaction at position 2. Thus, the crucial distinction is not whether processing is serial or parallel, but whether the MPU is the segment or the syllable.

Final Remarks

The segment MPU suggests that written word processing can be highly incremental, with the degree of incrementality varying across individuals and with stimulus and task demands. More importantly, articulatory, and acoustic effects implied by the segment MPU also affect assumptions about earlier encoding stages.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

1. ^Dell et al. (1993) use a simple recurrent network to produce segments sequentially without a stage that encodes segments to slots in a phonological frame.

2. ^Some models (e.g., Dell et al., 1993) have been implemented as simple recurrent connectionist networks that generate each segment (or its features) after a fixed number of processing cycles without any buffering.

3. ^If the effect is localized to the initial segment, measuring the duration of the entire word would miss the effect for targets beginning with plosives.

References

Cholin, J., Dell, G. S., and Levelt, W. J. M. (2011). Planning and articulation in incremental word production: syllable-frequency effects in English. J. Exp. Psychol. 37, 109–122. doi: 10.1037/a0021322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., and Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual-route cascade model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 108, 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, M. J. (1998). Revisiting serial position effects in reading. J. Mem. Lang. 39, 652–665. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2603

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damian, M. F. (2003). Articulatory duration in single-word speech production. J. Exp. Psychol. 29, 416–431. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.29.3.416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damian, M. F., Bowers, J. S., Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., and Spalek, K. (2010). Does word length affect speech onset latencies when producing single words? J. Exp. Psychol. 36, 892–205. doi: 10.1037/a0019446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damian, M. F., and Dumay, N. (2007). Time pressure and phonological advance planning in spoken production. J. Mem. Lang. 57, 195–209. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell, G. S., Juliano, C., and Govindjee, A. (1993). A theory of frame constraints in phonological speech errors. Cogn. Sci. 17, 149–195. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1702_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, F., and Swets, B. (2002). How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums. J. Mem. Lang. 46, 57–84. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2797

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frederiksen, J. R., and Kroll, J. F. (1976). Spelling and sound: approaches to the internal lexicon. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2, 361–379. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.2.3.361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawamoto, A. H. (1999). Incremental encoding and incremental articulation in speech production: evidence based on response latency and initial segment duration. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 48–49. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99331770

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawamoto, A. H., and Kello, C. T. (1999). Effect of onset cluster complexity in speeded naming: a test of rule-based approaches. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 25, 361–375. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.25.2.361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawamoto, A. H., Kello, C. T., Jones, R. M., and Bame, K. (1998). Initial phoneme versus whole word criterion to initiate pronunciation: evidence based on response latency and initial phoneme duration. J. Exp. Psychol. 24, 862–885. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.862

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawamoto, A. H., and Liu, A. (2007). “A unified account of duration and coarticulatory effects in speech production,” in Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society, eds D. S. McNamara and G. Trafton (Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society), 1151–1156.

Google Scholar

Kawamoto, A. H., Liu, Q., Lee, R. J., and Grebe, P. R. (2014). The segment as the minimal planning unit in speech production: evidence based on absolute response latencies. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 67, 2340–2359. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.927892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kawamoto, A. H., Liu, Q., Mura, K., and Sanchez, A. (2008). Articulatory preparation in the delayed naming task. J. Mem. Lang. 58, 347–365. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.06.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kello, C. T. (2004). Control over the time course of cognition in the tempo-naming task. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 30, 942–955. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.30.5.942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kello, C. T., and Plaut, D. C. (2000). Strategic control in word reading: evidence from speeded responding in the tempo-naming task. J. Exp. Psychol. 26, 719–750. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.3.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kello, C. T., and Plaut, D. C. (2003). Strategic control over rate of processing in word reading: a computational investigation. J. Mem. Lang. 48, 207–232. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00512-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kello, C. T., Plaut, D. C., and MacWhinney, B. (2000). The task-dependence of staged versus cascade processing: an empirical and computational study of Stroop interference in speech production. J. Exp. Psychol. 129, 340–360. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.129.3.340

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klatt, D. (1974). The duration of [s] in English words. J. Speech Hear. Res. 17, 51–63. doi: 10.1044/jshr.1701.51

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. Cambridge, MA: MITPress.

Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A. S. (1999a). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 1–38. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99001776

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A. S. (1999b). Multiple perspectives on word production. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 61–75. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99451775

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacKay, D. G. (1987). The Organization of Perception and Action: A Theory of Action and Other Cognitive Skills. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

Google Scholar

Meyer, A. (1992). Investigation of phonological encoding through speech error analyses: achievements, limitations, and alternatives. Cognition 42, 181–211. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90043-H

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, A., Roelofs, A., and Levelt, W. J. M. (2003). Word length effects in object naming: the role of a response criterion. J. Mem. Lang. 48, 131–147. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00509-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pechmann, T., Reetz, H., and Zerbst, D. (1989). Kritik einer Meβmethode: Zur Ungenaugket von Voice-key Messungen [Critique of a method of measurement: on the unreliability of voice-key measurements.]. Sprac. Kognit. 8, 65–71.

Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., and Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: the CDP+ model of reading aloud. Psychol. Rev. 114, 273–315. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.273

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapp, B., and Goldrick, M. (2000). Discreteness and interactivity in spoken word production. Psychol. Rev. 107, 460–499. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.107.3.460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rastle, K., and Davis, M. H. (2002). On the complexities of measuring naming. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 28, 307–314. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.28.2.307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rastle, K., Harrington, J., Coltheart, M., and Palethorpe, S. (2000). Reading aloud begins when the computation of phonology is complete. J. Exp. Psychol. 26, 1178–1191. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.26.3.1178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riès, S., Legous, T., Burle, B., Alario, F., and Malfait, N. (2012). Why does picture naming take longer than word reading? The contribution of articulatory processes. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 19, 955–961. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0287-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roberts, M. A., Rastle, K., Coltheart, M., and Besner, D. (2003). When parallel processing in visual word recognition is not enough: new evidence from naming. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 10, 405–414. doi: 10.3758/BF03196499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, A. (2004). Seriality of phonological encoding in naming objects and reading their names. Mem. Cogn. 32, 212–222. doi: 10.3758/BF03196853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakuma, N., Fushimi, T., and Tatsumi, I. (1997). Onseiha no shisatsu ni yoru kana no ondokusenji no sokutei: ondokusenji wa gotton no choonho ni ori okiku kotonaru [Naming latency measurements of kana based on inspection of voice waveforms: naming latency varies greatly depending on the manner of articulation of the initial phoneme.]. Shinkeishinrigaku 13, 126–136.

Schriefers, H., and Teruel, E. (1999). Phonological facilitation in the production of two-word utterances. Eur. J. Cogn. Psychol. 11, 17–50. doi: 10.1080/713752301

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sulpizio, S., Spinelli, G., and Burani, C. (2015). Stress affects articulatory planning in reading aloud. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 41, 453–461. doi: 10.1037/a0038714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: absolute latency, segment duration, serial vs. parallel encoding

Citation: Kawamoto AH, Liu Q and Kello CT (2015) The segment as the minimal planning unit in speech production and reading aloud: evidence and implications. Front. Psychol. 6:1457. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01457

Received: 21 July 2015; Accepted: 11 September 2015;
Published: 29 September 2015.

Edited by:

Simone Sulpizio, University of Trento, Italy

Reviewed by:

Markus F. Damian, University of Bristol, UK
Cristina Burani, Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies, CNR, Italy

Copyright © 2015 Kawamoto, Liu and Kello. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alan H. Kawamoto, Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz, 1159 High St., Santa Cruz, CA 95064, USA, ahk@ucsc.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.