Skip to main content
Log in

The Enactive Roots of STEM: Rethinking Educational Design in Mathematics

  • Review Article
  • Published:
Educational Psychology Review Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

New and radically reformative thinking about the enactive and embodied basis of cognition holds out the promise of moving forward age-old debates about whether we learn and how we learn. The radical enactive, embodied view of cognition (REC) poses a direct, and unmitigated, challenge to the trademark assumptions of traditional cognitivist theories of mind—those that characterize cognition as always and everywhere grounded in the manipulation of contentful representations of some kind. REC has had some success in understanding how sports skills and expertise are acquired. But, REC approaches appear to encounter a natural obstacle when it comes to understanding skill acquisition in knowledge-rich, conceptually based domains like the hard sciences and mathematics. This paper offers a proof of concept that REC’s reach can be usefully extended into the domain of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) learning, especially when it comes to understanding the deep roots of such learning. In making this case, this paper has five main parts. The section “Ancient Intellectualism and the REC Challenge” briefly introduces REC and situates it with respect to rival views about the cognitive basis of learning. The “Learning REConceived: from Sports to STEM?” section outlines the substantive contribution REC makes to understanding skill acquisition in the domain of sports and identifies reasons for doubting that it will be possible to apply the same approach to knowledge-rich STEM domains. The “Mathematics as Embodied Practice” section gives the general layout for how to understand mathematics as an embodied practice. The section “The Importance of Attentional Anchors” introduces the concept “attentional anchor” and establishes why attentional anchors are important to educational design in STEM domains like mathematics. Finally, drawing on some exciting new empirical studies, the section “Seeing Attentional Anchors” demonstrates how REC can contribute to understanding the roots of STEM learning and inform its learning design, focusing on the case of mathematics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. For a compelling critique of the need to invoke this Fodorian hypothesis-formation assumption, see Shea’s (2011) commentary on Carey (2011).

  2. Borrowing from Fodor, again, the assumption is that representing as necessarily involves concepts: “To represent (e.g., mentally) Mr. James as a cat is to represent him falling under the concept CAT” (Fodor 2007, p. 105). This line of thought motivates and (apparently) justifies believing in atomistic conceptual primitives. Carruthers (2011) articulates this working assumption well and makes his commitment to it indelibly clear: “many mental states are realized discretely in the brain and possess causally relevant component structure … they possess a discrete existence and are structured out of component concepts” (Carruthers 2011, p. xiv; see the preface of Fodor and Pylyshyn 2015 for similar view and an expanded list of related working assumptions).

  3. Here “content” is understood designating representational content—where, canonically, the notion of representation content assumes the existence of some kind of correctness condition such that the world is taken (“said,” “represented,” or “claimed”) to be in a certain way that it might not be in.

  4. See (Hutto et al. 2014) for an elaborated account of the notion extensive, including how the idea of an extensive mind differs from that of an extended mind.

  5. Other publications explain the critical role that the interval plays in fostering mathematical learning through the MIT-P activity, and in particular its mediating function in students’ micro-actions of adopting the mathematical frames of reference (i.e., the grid and numerals; see (Abrahamson and Trninic 2015); (Abrahamson et al. 2011)) and linking among competing visualizations of the bimanual “green” enactment (Abrahamson et al. 2014).

  6. At the Embodied Design Research Laboratory (EDRL), we are particularly excited by the opportunities created by these new intellectual perspectives and research designs for revisiting and corroborating the construct of reflective abstraction (Piaget 1968). Piaget implicated sensorimotor coordination as a critical achievement in the development of a new conceptual schema. Attentional anchors enable us to underscore Piaget’s revolutionary adoption of structuralism (Piaget 1970) as the systemic alternative to cognitivism.

References

  • Abrahamson, D. (2009). Embodied design: constructing means for constructing meaning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(1), 27–47. [Electronic supplementary material at http://edrl.berkeley.edu/publications/journals/ESM/Abrahamson-ESM/ ]. doi: 10.1007/s10649-008-9137-1

  • Abrahamson, D. (2012a). Discovery reconceived: product before process. For the Learning of Mathematics, 32(1), 8–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D. (2012c). Mathematical Imagery Trainer - Proportion (MIT-P) IPhone/iPad application (Terasoft): iTunes. Retrieved from https://itunes.apple.com/au/app/mathematical-imagery-trainer/id563185943.

  • Abrahamson, D., & Lindgren, R. (2014). Embodiment and embodied design. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (2nd ed., pp. 358–376). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., & Sánchez–García, R. (2014). Learning is moving in new ways: an ecological dynamics view on learning across the disciplines. Paper presented at the “Embodied cognition in education” symposium (A. Bakker, M. F. van der Schaaf, S. Shayan, & P. Leseman, Chairs), Freudenthal Institute for Science and Mathematics Education, University of Utrecht, The Netherlands.

  • Abrahamson, D., & Trninic, D. (2015). Bringing forth mathematical concepts: signifying sensorimotor enactment in fields of promoted action. In D. Reid, L. Brown, A. Coles, & M.-D. Lozano (Eds.), Enactivist methodology in mathematics education research [Special issue]. ZDM, 47(2), 295–306.

  • Abrahamson, D., Trninic, D., Gutiérrez, J. F., Huth, J., & Lee, R. G. (2011). Hooks and shifts: a dialectical study of mediated discovery. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 16(1), 55–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., Gutiérrez, J. F., Charoenying, T., Negrete, A. G., & Bumbacher, E. (2012). Fostering hooks and shifts: tutorial tactics for guided mathematical discovery. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 17(1–2), 61–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abrahamson, D., Lee, R. G., Negrete, A. G., Gutiérrez, J. F. (2014). Coordinating visualizations of polysemous action: values added for grounding proportion. In F. Rivera, H. Steinbring, & A. Arcavi (Eds.), Visualization as an epistemological learning tool [Special issue]. ZDM–The international Journal on Mathematics Education, 46(1), 79–93.

  • Aguilera, M., Bedia, M. G., Santos, B. A., & Barandiaran, X. E. (2013). The situated HKB model: how sensorimotor spatial coupling can alter oscillatory brain dynamics. Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 7(117), 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bamberger, J. (2011). The collaborative invention of meaning: a short history of evolving ideas. Psychology of Music, 39(1), 82–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barab, S., Zuiker, S., Warren, S., Hickey, D., Ingram-Goble, A., Kwon, E.-J., & Herring, S. C. (2007). Situationally embodied curriculum: relating formalisms and contexts. Science Education, 91, 750–782.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolini Bussi, M. G., & Mariotti, M. A. (2008). Semiotic mediation in the mathematics classroom: artefacts and signs after a Vygotskian perspective. In L. D. English, M. G. Bartolini Bussi, G. A. Jones, R. Lesh, & D. Tirosh (Eds.), Handbook of international research in mathematics education (2nd ed., pp. 720–749). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behr, M. J., Harel, G., Post, T., & Lesh, R. (1993). Rational number, ratio, and proportion. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 296–333). NYC: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boutin, A., Blandin, Y., Massen, C., Heuer, H., & Badets, A. (2014). Conscious awareness of action potentiates sensorimotor learning. Cognition, 133(1), 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, M. C., McNeil, N. M., & Glenberg, A. M. (2009). Using concreteness in education: real problems, potential solutions. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 160–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1960). The process of education: a searching discussion of school education opening new paths to learning and teaching. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (2011). Précis of The origin of concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 113–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2011). Opacity of mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chahine, I. C. (2013). The impact of using multiple modalities on students’ acquisition of fractional knowledge: An international study in embodied mathematics across semiotic cultures. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 32(3), 434–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chemero, T. (2009). Radical Embodied Cognitive Science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • Chow, J. Y., Davids, K., Button, C., Shuttleworth, R., Renshaw, I., & Araújo, D. (2007). The role of nonlinear pedagogy in physical education. Review of Educational Research, 77(3), 251–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davids, K. (2012). Learning design for nonlinear dynamical movement systems. The Open Sports Sciences Journal, 5(Suppl. 1), 9–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davids, K., Button, C., & Bennett, S. (2008). Dynamics of skill acquisition. Campaign: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davids, K., Araújo, D., Vilar, L., Renshaw, I., & Pinder, R. A. (2013). An ecological dynamics approach to skill acquisition: Implications for development of talent in sport. Talent Development and Excellence, 5, 21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, P. J., & Hersh, R. (1981). The mathematical experience. Boston: Birkhauser.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Freitas, E., & Sinclair, N. (2012). Diagram, gesture, agency: theorizing embodiment in the mathematics classroom. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 80(1–2), 133–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • diSessa, A. A., & Cobb, P. (2004). Ontological innovation and the role of theory in design experiments. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(1), 77–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H., & Dreyfus, S. (1986). Mind over machine. New York: The Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1999). The challenge of Merleau-Ponty’s phenomenology of embodiment for cognitive science. In G. Weiss & H. F. Haber (Eds.), Perspectives on embodiment: the intersections of nature and culture (pp. 103–120). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duijzer, C. (2015). How perception guides cognition: Insights from embodied interaction with a tablet application for proportions – an eye-tracking study. Utrecht: Utrecht University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrara, F. (2014). How multimodality works in mathematical activity: young children graphing motion. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 12(4), 917–939.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 177–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (2003). Hume variations. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (2007). The revenge of the given. In B. McLaughlin & J. Cohen (Eds.), Contemporary debates in philosophy of mind (pp. 105–116). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A., & Pylyshyn, Z. (2015). Minds without meanings. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Gerofsky, S. (2011). Seeing the graph vs. being the graph: gesture, engagement and awareness in school mathematics. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures (pp. 245–256). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting and knowing: toward an ecological psychology (pp. 67–82). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ginsburg, C. (2010). The intelligence of moving bodies: a somatic view of life and its consequences. Santa Fe: AWAREing Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldstone, R. L., Landy, D. H., & Son, J. Y. (2009). The education of perception. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2(2), 265–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Howison, M., Trninic, D., Reinholz, D., & Abrahamson, D. (2011). The mathematical imagery trainer: from embodied interaction to conceptual learning. In G. Fitzpatrick, C. Gutwin, B. Begole, W. A. Kellogg, & D. Tan (Eds.), Proceedings of the annual meeting of The association for computer machinery special interest group on computer human interaction: “human factors in computing systems” (CHI 2011) (pp. 1989–1998). Vancouver: ACM Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radical enactivism. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. D., & Sánchez-García, R. (2014). Choking RECtified: embodied expertise beyond Dreyfus. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1–23. doi: 10.1007/s11097-014-9380-0

  • Hutto, D. D., Kirchhoff, M. D., Myin, E. (2014). Extensive enactivism: Why keep it all in? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 1–11. DOI: 10.3389/fnhum.2014. 00706.

  • Ingold, T. (2000). The perception of the environment: essays on livelihood, dwelling, and skill (2nd ed.). London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Karplus, R., Pulos, S., & Stage, E. K. (1983). Proportional reasoning of early adolescents. In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of mathematics concepts and processes (pp. 45–89). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1984). Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. American Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative, 246(6), R1000–R1004.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelso, J. A. S., & Engstrøm, D. A. (2006). The complementary nature. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D. (2010). Thinking with external representations. AI & Society, 25, 441–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18(4), 513–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kostrubiec, V., Zanone, P.-G., Fuchs, A., Kelso, J. A. S. (2012). Beyond the blank slate: routes to learning new coordination patterns depend on the intrinsic dynamics of the learner -- experimental evidence and theoretical model. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00222.

  • Laurence, S., & Margolis, E. (2007). Linguistic Determinism and the Innate Basis of Number. In P. Carruthers et al. (eds.), The Innate Mind, vol. 3: Foundations and the Future (Oxford University Press), pp. 139–169.

  • Lee, R. G., Hung, M., Negrete, A. G., Abrahamson, D. (2013). Rationale for a ratio-based conceptualization of slope: results from a design-oriented embodied-cognition domain analysis. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association (Special Interest Group on Research in Mathematics Education), San Francisco, April 27 - May 1.

  • Liao, C., & Masters, R. S. (2001). Analogy learning: a means to implicit motor learning. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 307–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loader, P. (2012). The epistemic/pragmatic dichotomy. Philosophical Explorations: An International Journal for the Philosophy of Mind and Action, 15(2), 219–232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marghetis, T., & Núñez, R. l. (2013). The motion behind the symbols: a vital role for dynamism in the conceptualization of limits and continuity in expert mathematics. Topics in Cognitive Science. doi:10.1111/tops.12013.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R. (2003). Three conjectures concerning the relationship between body activity and understanding mathematics. In R. Nemirovsky, M. Borba, N. A. Pateman, B. J. Dougherty, & J. T. Zilliox (Eds.), Proceedings of the twenty seventh annual meeting of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol. 1 (pp. 105–109). Honolulu: OH: Eric Clearinghouse for Science, Mathematics, and Environmental Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, K. M., & Ranganathan, R. (2010). Instructions as constraints in motor skill acquisition. In I. Renshaw, K. Davids, & G. J. P. Savelsbergh (Eds.), Motor learning in practice: a constraints-led approach (pp. 17–32). Florence: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. E., Edwards, L. D., & Matos, J. F. (1999). Embodied cognition as grounding for situatedness and context in mathematics education. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 39, 45–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, D. R. (1994). The world on paper. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrick, C. J., & Martin, T. (2011). Hands up, know body move: learning mathematics through embodied actions. Austin: University of Texas at Austin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1968). Genetic epistemology (E. Duckworth, trans.). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1970). Structuralism (C. Maschler, trans.). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., & Szeminska, A. (1960). The child’s conception of geometry (E. A. Lunzer, trans.) (E. A. Lunzer, trans.). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Presmeg, N. C. (2006). Research on visualization in learning and teaching mathematics: emergence from psychology. In A. Gutiérrez & P. Boero (Eds.), Handbook of research on the psychology of mathematics education: past, present, and future (pp. 205–235). Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the mind: concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2013). Three key concepts of the theory of objectification: knowledge, knowing, and learning. In L. Radford (Ed.), Theory of objectification: knowledge, knowing, and learning [Special issue]. REDIMAT - Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 2(1), 7–44.

  • Reed, E. S., & Bril, B. (1996). The primacy of action in development. In M. L. Latash & M. T. Turvey (Eds.), Dexterity and its development (pp. 431–451). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinholz, D., Trninic, D., Howison, M., & Abrahamson, D. (2010). It’s not easy being green: embodied artifacts and the guided emergence of mathematical meaning. In P. Brosnan & D. Erchick (Eds.), Proceedings of the thirty-second annual meeting of the north-american chapter of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (PME-NA 32) (Vol. VI, Ch. 18: technology (pp. 1488–1496). Columbus: PME-NA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Renshaw, I., Chow, J. Y., Davids, K., & Hammond, J. (2010). A constraints-led perspective for understanding skill acquisition and game play. Physical Education & Sport Pedagogy, 15(2), 117–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rick, J. (2012). Proportion: a tablet app for collaborative learning. In H. Schelhowe (Ed.), Proceedings of the 11th Annual Interaction Design and Children Conference (IDC 2012) (Vol. “Demo Papers”, pp. 316–319). Bremen, Germany: ACM-IDC.

  • Roth, W.-M. (2010). Incarnation: radicalizing the embodiment of mathematics. For the Learning of Mathematics, 30(2), 8–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2015). Excess of graphical thinking: movement, mathematics and flow. For the Learning of Mathematics, 35, 1–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W-M., & Thom, J. S. (2009). Bodily experience and mathematical conceptions: from classical views to a phenomenological reconceptualization. In L. Radford, L. Edwards, & F. Arzarello (Eds.), Gestures and multimodality in the construction of mathematical meaning [Special issue]. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 175–189.

  • Rotman, B. (2000). Mathematics as sign: writing, imagining, counting. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmandt-Besserat, D. E. (1992). Before writing Vol. 1: from counting to cuneiform. Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, B., Bumbacher, E., & Blikstein, P. (2015). Discovery versus direct instruction: learning outcomes of two pedagogical models using tangible interface. In T. Koschmann, P. Häkkinen, & P. Tchounikine (Eds.), Exploring the material conditions of learning: opportunities and challenges for CSCL,“ the proceedings of the Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL) conference. Gothenburg: ISLS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, D. L., & Martin, T. (2006). Distributed learning and mutual adaptation. In Harnad, S., & Dror, I. E. (Eds.), Distributed cognition [Special issue]. Pragmatics & Cognition, 14(2), 313–332.

  • Sfard, A. (1994). Reification as the birth of metaphor. For the Learning of Mathematics, 14(1), 44–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sfard, A. (2002). The interplay of intimations and implementations: generating new discourse with new symbolic tools. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 11(2&3), 319–357.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shayan, S., Abrahamson, D., Bakker, A., Duijzer, C., & van der Schaaf, M. (2015). The emergence of proportional reasoning from embodied interaction with a tablet application: an eye-tracking study. In L. Gómez Chova, A. López Martínez, & I. Candel Torres (Eds.), Proceedings of the 9th international technology, education, and development conference (INTED 2015) (pp. 5732–5741). Madrid: IATED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shea, N. (2011). Acquiring a new concept is not explicable-by-content. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34(3), 148–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2006). Microgenetic analyses of learning. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (6 ed., Vol. 2, cognition, perception, and language (pp. 464–510). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, N., & Gol Tabaghi, S. (2010). Drawing space: mathematicians’ kinetic conceptions of eigenvectors. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 74(3), 223–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., & Gasser, M. (2005). The development of embodied cognition: six lessons from babies. Artificial Life, 11, 13–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starra, A., Libertus, M. E., & Brannona, E. M. (2013). Number sense in infancy predicts mathematical abilities in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(45), 18116–18120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, R., & Hall, R. (1998). Disciplined perception: learning to see in technoscience. In M. Lampert & M. L. Blunk (Eds.), Talking mathematics in school: studies of teaching and learning (pp. 107–149). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoate, I., & Wulf, G. (2011). Does the attentional focus adapted by swimmers affect their performance? Journal of Sport science & Coaching, 6(1), 99–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, A. L., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: grounded theory procedures and techniques. Newbury Park: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theories. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Theoretical models of human development, Vol. 1, pp. 258–312). Hoboken: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, E. (2007). Mind in life: biology, phenomenology, and the sciences of mind. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trninic, D., & Abrahamson, D. (2012). Embodied artifacts and conceptual performances. In K. Thompson, M. J. Jacobson, & P. Reimann (Eds.), Proceedings of the 10th international conference of the learning sciences: future of learning (ICLS 2012) (Vol. 1 (pp. 283–290). Sydney: University of Sydney / ISLS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Uttal, D. H., & O’Doherty, K. (2008). Comprehending and learning from visual representations: a developmental approach. In J. Gilbert, M. Reiner, & M. Nakhleh (Eds.), Visualization: theory and practice in science education (pp. 53–72). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • van Dijk, L., Withagen, R., & Bongers, R. M. (2015). Information without content: a Gibsonian reply to enactivists’ worries. Cognition, 134, 210–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2010). From addition to multiplication … and back. The development of students’ additive and multiplicative reasoning skills. Cognition and Instruction, 28(3), 360–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Thompson, E. & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

  • von Glasersfeld, E. (1983). Learning as constructive activity. In J. C. Bergeron & N. Herscovics (Eds.), Proceedings of the 5th annual meeting of the North American group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol. 1, pp. 41–69). Montreal: PME-NA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittmann, M. C., Flood, V. J., & Black, K. E. (2013). Algebraic manipulation as motion within a landscape. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 82(2), 169–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G., & Su, J. (2007). An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy in beginners and experts. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 384–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zarghami, M., Saemi, E., & Fathi, I. (2012). External focus of attention enhances discus throwing performance. Kinesiology, 44(1), 47–51.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael D. Kirchhoff.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hutto, D.D., Kirchhoff, M.D. & Abrahamson, D. The Enactive Roots of STEM: Rethinking Educational Design in Mathematics. Educ Psychol Rev 27, 371–389 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9326-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9326-2

Keywords

Navigation