Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 16 November 2020
Sec. Consciousness Research

A Review on Research and Evaluation Methods for Investigating Self-Transcendence

Updated
  • 1School of Interactive Arts and Technology, Simon Fraser University, Burnaby, BC, Canada
  • 2Department of Psychology, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Milan, Italy
  • 3ATN-P Lab, Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Milan, Italy

Self-transcendence has been characterized as a decrease in self-saliency (ego disillusionment) and increased connection, and has been growing in research interest in the past decade. Several measures have been developed and published with some degree of psychometric validity and reliability. However, to date, there has been no review systematically describing, contrasting, and evaluating the different methodological approaches toward measuring self-transcendence including questionnaires, neurological and physiological measures, and qualitative methods. To address this gap, we conducted a review to describe existing methods of measuring self-transcendence, evaluate the strengths and weaknesses of these methods, and discuss research avenues to advance assessment of self-transcendence, including recommendations for suitability of methods given research contexts.

1. Introduction

Self-transcendence is often defined as decreased self-saliency and increased connection to others and the environment (Yaden et al., 2017a). However, if we look across different disciplines, we find the focus and conceptualization of self-transcendence varies. For example, psychologists often view self-transcendence as involving an elevation or a separation of self from the environment, whereas nursing regards self-transcendence as awareness of one's wholeness in person-environment connections when fragmentation threatens one's well-being (Smith and Liehr, 2014). Self-transcendence seems to be emerging in several disciplines including nursing theory, developmental psychology, gerontology, personality theory and psychiatric genetics, positive psychology, and others (Garcia-Romeu, 2010). Self-transcendence can be regarded as a psychological state, personality trait, developmental process, value orientation, motivation, and worldview (Wong, 2016). Self-transcendence experience is also considered multifaceted in itself, composed of mindfulness, flow, self-transcendent emotions, awe, peak experiences, and mystical experiences (Yaden et al., 2017a). Therefore, depending on the discipline and focus, one will find very different approaches to investigate the phenomenon of self-transcendence (Garcia-Romeu, 2010). Nonetheless, here we will review and describe the research and evaluation methods across different disciplines for investigating self-transcendence by critically discussing, comparing and evaluating them.

Our goals for this paper are to (1) describe existing methods of measuring self-transcendence; (2) evaluate the strengths and weaknesses of these methods; (3) discuss research avenues to advance assessment of self-transcendence, including recommendations for suitability of methods given research contexts.

2. Theories of Self-Transcendence

There exist several theories on self-transcendence, which I divided into three categories: conceptual, phenomenological, and physiological. Each offer a different perspective into understanding self-transcendence since the construct itself is not widely agreed upon.

2.1. Conceptual

Frankl (1966) is perhaps one of the first researchers to go against Freudian models of pleasure seeking and equilibrium as our sole drivers and goals, and toward a model of self-transcendence. Here, Frankl's self-transcendence construct emphasizes focusing on serving others and not on fulfilling one's own potential through constant self-referral. A more contemporary model of self-transcendence uses Frankl's construct and describes it on three levels (Wong, 2016):

1. Seeking ultimate meaning —seeking ultimate ideals of goodness, truth, and beauty;

2. Seeking situational meaning —mindful of present moment with openness, curiosity, and compassion;

3. Seeking one's calling —pursue higher purpose for the greater good.

Another key theory that is relevant to today's conceptualization of self-transcendence is Maslow (1943)'s hierarchy of needs (from bottom to top): physiological (survival) through basic life necessities; safety and security through law and order; belongingness and love through group affiliation; esteem through recognition and achievement; self-actualization through fulfillment of personal potential. Theorists postulate there is a sixth tier need—self-transcendence (Venter, 2017). Maslow wanted to show there is a false dichotomy of thinking of the world as only the self and the environment. One who has self-transcendended no longer relies on others' opinions and is free from culture and social environment. Self-transcendence is being no longer grounded or anchored in one's own culture alone; not exclusively defined by their immediate environment or group. Through self-transcending culture, one can better identify with others. Stellar et al. (2017) and Haidt and Morris (2009) also supported the idea that self-transcendent emotions —compassion, awe, gratitude, appreciation, inspiration, admiration, elevation, and love —are key to positive social functioning and connecting to others. For example, compassion helps to support those in need; gratitude builds commitment to others in need; awe reduces self-importance.

As a way to conceptualize self-transcendence, some have suggested self-transcendence is a measurable personality trait that captures the degree to which an individual feels a part of nature and the universe at large (Cloninger et al., 1993a). Four traits predictive of self-transcendence were neuroticism (negatively correlated), openness to experience (positive), agreeableness (positive), and conscientiousness (positive); another predictor variable was meditation practice (positive) (Levenson et al., 2005). Others have derived components of self-transcendence from the aging process and development across the lifespan, otherwise known as gerotranscendence (Tornstam, 1994). Yaden et al. (2017a) posit the following constructs for self-transcendence: mindfulness, flow, positive emotions (elevation, compassion, admiration, gratitude, love, awe), peak experiences (e.g., comic consciousness, merging with the universe), mystical experiences (e.g., psychedelic), pathological experiences (e.g., schizophrenia, depersonalization disorder). The authors suggest that self-transcendent experiences contain two sub components: reduced self-saliency (annihilation component) and increased connectedness (relational component). Furthermore, blurring the lines between social and spatial (culture and environment) may be a way to increase perceived social connection and, thus, increase well-being. From Yaden et al. (2017a)'s perspective, self-transcendent experiences do not seem to serve any individualistic evolutionary purpose, but might be seen as a way to reinforce cohesive social groups. Additionally, they are also not well-understood in terms of efficacy, contraindications, and implications for therapeutic purpose.

A highly related construct of self-transcendence is meditation, especially those practices that follow the dissolution of self, time, and external reality. As described in Yaden et al. (2017a) and detailed in Vago and Silbersweig (2012), cultivating a state of mindfulness leads to the development of self-other relations that transcends selfish needs, described in the literature as “decentering.” In advanced mindfulness practitioners, this self-other distinction is completely dissolved (Vago and Zeidan, 2016). Dorjee (2016) describes a contemplative practice framework that outlines the progression in shifts of self and reality as states associated with increasing gradients of dereification. Here, the decentering aspect of mindfulness-based practices has been linked to the initial stages of this progression. Schoenberg and Vago (2019) outline a multi-dimensional model of meditation that progresses in five stages: (1) relaxation practices characterized by neuro-visceral processes; (2) concentration practices characterized by focused and diffuse attention; (3) insight practices characterized by ordinary insight that all objects are illusory or constructs of the mind; (4) non-dual practices characterized by dissolution of the self and everything; (5) cumulative practices characterized by unifying compassion and unconditional love as outcomes of the previous four practices. Self-transcendence in particular seems to relate to the latter stages of this model where the ego is dissolved and meditation practitioners experience a feeling of unity. This concept of different developmental-stages in meditation is echoed in Piron (2001)'s paper on meditative depth, which defines meditation not only in type but also in terms of level felt during different times, i.e., a person can experience different degrees of self-transcendence at different times in their life. Piron (2001) refers to Engel (1997)'s Meditation Development Index (MDI) measure that is based on Engel (1997)'s Zen developmental theory of meditation comprised of eight stages: (1) premeditative: meditation as tranquilizer; (2) searching: uncertainty and disorientation; (3) effort: struggle, fight; (4) level of work: calm, regular exercises; (5) support: pleasantness and motivation to continue; (6) being uplifted; (7) resolution; and (8) afterwards. Piron (2001) then furthered MDI by constructing both the Meditation Depth Index (MEDI) and Meditation Depth Questionnaire (MEDEQ) to measure the greatest depth of meditation and meditation itself in a more differentiated way than the index, respectfully.

Millière et al. (2018) approaches self-transcendence as a multidimensional model of altered self-consciousness. They describe self-consciousness as dissolving the sense of self through meditation and the phenomenon occurring from drug-induced ego-dissolution. Here, self-consciousness is organized into two categories of narrative and embodied selfhood. These two aspects are then plotted against six dimensions: (1) a sense of body ownership, (2) awareness of bodily sensations, (3) awareness of spatial self-location, (4) rich phenomenology, (5) access to semantic autobiographical information, and (6) self-related thoughts. A total loss of self is then the absence of these dimensions for both the narrative and embodied self. The authors have emphasized that self-consciousness is not a simple nor uni-dimensional construct, showing that different forms of meditation and psychedelic states are mapped differently to their multidimensional model.

Nursing's conceptual idea of self-transcendence is “the capacity to expand self-boundaries in a variety of ways” (Reed, 2013). In forming this concept of self-transcendence, nurses drew from Neo-Piagetian theories about development in adulthood and later life. The model itself is composed of four basic sets of relationships (Figure 1). First, increased levels of vulnerability, e.g., health event, influence increased levels of self-transcendence. Reed describes vulnerability here as the awareness of one's own mortality, and that self-transcendence arises naturally from this awareness that is often triggered by life crises. Second, self-transcendence relates positively to a sense of well-being and morale but relates negatively to the level of depression. Third, self-transcendence mediates the effects of vulnerability on well-being. Fourth, personal and contextual factors play a role in all three variables, e.g., age, gender, cognitive ability, health status, past significant life events, personal beliefs, family support, and sociopolitical environment. Another conceptual model of self-transcendence from nursing is based on a literature review and uses the process of concept analysis (McCarthy et al., 2018). The antecedents and attributes of self-transcendence are organized into five logically related domains: creativity, relationships, introspection, contemplation, and spirituality (Figure 2).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model of Self-transcendence in the context of nursing described in Reed (2013).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Conceptual model of self-transcendence described in McCarthy et al. (2018). This model was originally used for the Psychoeducational Approach to Transcendence and Health (PATH) program on self-transcendence and well-being in community-dwelling older adults.

2.2. Phenomenological

There is a large body of theoretical literature and empirical research on self-transcendence. However, some of the conceptual models of self-transcendence mentioned above have had their internal validity questioned (MacDonald and Holland, 2002), suggesting that in when reporting self-transcendence an individual might not be able to separate core constructs; and the construct of self-transcendence itself needs further analysis (Akyalcin et al., 2008). There is less research on the process, outcomes, and nature of self-transcendence itself.

Garcia-Romeu et al. (2015) used a grounded theory approach to address shortcomings in the current understanding of self-transcendence in the experiential domain. In terms of context, self-transcendence likely occurs during stressful or challenging times in life, in adulthood, during religious/spiritual practices or social events like concerts/raves, and is catalyzed with psychoactive drugs, spiritual instruction, dance, and prayer. In terms of phenomenology, self-transcendence is described as very physical, whether that be warmth, connectedness, lightness, vibration or shaking, hyper-ventilating, nausea, and vomiting. Perceptually, there is an expansive change in self-boundaries, egolessness, and timelessness. Cognitively and affectively, participants experienced positive affective states (e.g., joy, love, compassion, forgiveness, wonder, and freedom), surrender, vulnerability, and openness. Many people revealed that words and the vehicle of the mind could not really explain their self-transcendent experience because the experience itself transcended such boundaries. In terms of aftermath, the short-term effects consisted of mainly decreased anxiety, increased energy, insight, socialability, and sustained positive affect. Long-term effects were related to enduring transformation and impact, i.e., worldview, self-concept, and value-orientation. The outcomes for aftermath were value re-orientation, increased concern for others, increased positive affect, and disidentification from old patterns of thinking or behavior. The authors recommended self-transcendence needs more research on the pathological manifestations and its potential role in promoting enhanced well-being.

Metzinger (2020) proposed non-dual awareness as a non-conceptual, minimal phenomenal experience. Non-dual awareness or consciousness-as-such is a highly related to self-transcendence, described by Josipovic (2019) as “an empty cognizance, aware and present, but without any thoughts, emotions or perceptions, without a sense of body, space, orientation, time or the usual sense of self” (p. 279). In other words, non-dual awareness is where the self and world are merged into a unified whole or the boundaries of the self are dissolved (Gyamtso, 2001). Non-dual awareness occurs during states of lucidity in sleep and absorption in meditation (Thompson, 2010).

Nour et al. (2016) conceptualize self-transcendence in the same way as Yaden et al. (2017a)—a disappearance of the sense of self. Nour et al. (2016) describe ego consciousness as having two main constructs. First, ego-dissolution as “the associated feeling of increased union with one's surroundings, known as dissolved ego-boundaries.” Second, ego-inflation as “the distinct and largely antithetical experience of unusually elevated self-assuredness and confidence.” Thus, self-transcendence would then be the presence of ego-dissolution and absence of ego-inflation.

2.3. Physiological

Much of the neurophysiological research on self-transcendence relates to meditation and specifically mindfulness meditation, since, according to some, one component of mindfulness is self-transcendence (Vago and Silbersweig, 2012). There is also research on the neural correlates of experiences that are under the umbrella of self-transcendence such as awakening, enlightenment, and mystical experiences. We describe these studies as they relate to self-transcendence but there are always limits when the construct of self-transcendence highly varies across studies. For an overview of neurological and biochemical underpinnings of self-transcendence, see Figures 3, 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Neurological processes relating to self-transcendence. For a review, see (Newberg, 2014; Yaden et al., 2017a,b).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Biochemical processes related to self-transcendence. For a review, see (Yaden et al., 2017a).

In studying awakening experiences, which de Castro (2017) describes as a transcendent experience or “an essential core experience of oneness,” he presents a model that postulates three processing layers: sensory, perceptual, and cognitive. An awakening experience occurs when these three layers are removed. Testing this model is challenging since scientific methods are difficult to apply to phenomena that cannot be reliably and repeatedly produced. Nevertheless, de Castro (2017) sought the specific neurophysiological underpinnings of self-transcendence by comparing his model to existing research. First is the default mode network (DMN), which consists of the medial temporal lobe (declarative or long-term memory), medial prefrontal cortex (analyzing and thinking about attributes of other individuals), posterior cingulate cortex (autobiographical memory), and the ventral precuneus (hub of DMN) as well as parts of the parietal cortex (proprioception) (Smallwood et al., 2012). DMN activity is reduced during contemplative practice, such as meditation (Hasenkamp and Barsalou, 2012; Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2013b; Garrison et al., 2014), as well as in one study showing a video of awe (van Elk and Rotteveel, 2019). This suggests that contemplative practice reduces activity in neural structures involved in cognitive processing. This restraint on the cognitive system in turn amplifies the perceptual and sensory systems. In fact, those brain areas related to perceptual and sensory systems were larger in meditators and cognitive brain areas were smaller (Fox et al., 2014). Specifically, Fox et al. (2014) found these meditation brain areas increased in size: rostrolateral prefrontal cortex (metaawareness and introspection), sensory and insular cortices (body awareness), and anterior, mid-cingulate, and orbitofrontal cortex (emotion regulation). Moreover, these DMN areas were reduced in size: posterior cingulate cortex (self-related thinking), angular gyrus (transfers visual information to Wernicke's area), precuneous (self-awareness), and temporoparietal junction (self-other distinctions). Second, lesion studies have also looked at how self-transcendence is affected pre- and post-surgery and found that damage to the posterior parietal lobe, which is related to body sense and bodily interrelationships, increased self-transcendence (Urgesi et al., 2010; Johnstone et al., 2012). Third, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of inferior parietal lobe, related to body perception and separation of the environment, increases religiousness and spirituality (Crescentini et al., 2016). Other methods for eliciting self-transcendence are direct electric stimulation of brain regions (Blanke et al., 2002, 2004) or transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (Finisguerra et al., 2019). Finally, psilocybin has been found to produce reliable effects indistinguishable from awakening experiences; shows decreased DMN and feelings of oneness. Some researchers have looked at studying individuals who can have transcendent experiences regularly, but this method has its flaws (see Davis and Vago, 2013). Another approach is to develop adequate model test conditions, i.e., better experimental procedures for evoking awakening experiences, such that we can better define the model.

Davis and Vago (2013) outline whether we can determine the specific neural correlates for enlightenment, a term often equated with awakening in Buddhist traditions. The short answer: no. They argue measuring enlightenment is not possible because it is too vaguely defined as a construct. There is too much disagreement, even within Buddhist traditions, on what states and traits define enlightenment over mere concentration. Even if they are agreed upon, people may not agree on whether that state was achieved or not. Even Buddhist teachers do not accept self-reports of meditation experience at face value, but rather assess practice history, the manner and emotional state the report is given, and retrospective observations of behavior. Despite these concerns and challenges, Davis and Vago (2013) offer a potential solution by integrating evidence from neuroimaging with evidence of behavioral transformations specified in particular traditional descriptions of meditation practice. To this end, neuroimaging techniques have shown potential neural correlates of enlightenment. Cessation, referring to cessation of all inward phenomena in meditation, is linked to increased activity in the frontal polar cortex or Brodmann area-10 (higher cognitive functioning) (Koechlin et al., 1999; Ramnani and Owen, 2004). However, there are limitations with fMRI because it relies on generalized linear modeling, which carries some assumptions that a low-resolution fMRI signal, state bleed-over, and subtle states of enlightenment might violate. EEG found gamma band power over the lateral frontal and parietal sites correlated with self-reported clarity in meditation practitioners, suggesting a particular mechanism for increased phenomenal intensity (Lutz et al., 2004).

In terms of the Self-Awareness, -Regulation, -Transcendence (S-ART) model, where “T” represents the self-transcendent aspect of mindfulness, there are brain regions for self-specifying (i.e., experiential enactive self and experiential phenomenological self) and self-related processes: the dorsal attentional system and hippocampal-cortical memory system (Vago and Silbersweig, 2012), both components of the DMN. The S-ART framework suggests mindfulness critically involves working memory, efficiency of memory encoding, retrieval, and extinction processes, all aspects of hippocampal and parahippocampal activity, which serve higher order cognitive functions. This relates back to de Castro (2017)'s model of awakening experience, suggesting a common mechanism behind these types of experiences.

One review on the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms that may mediate the effects of self-transcendence comes from Yaden et al. (2017a), who conceptualize self-transcendence as having two major components: loss of self and increased connectedness. In terms of the loss of self, they found superior and inferior parietal functioning is associated with representations of the body's state (i.e., self and other representations) (Newberg et al., 2001), and is decreased when people report having a mystical experience (Azari et al., 2001; Beauregard and Paquette, 2006; Johnstone et al., 2012). Lesions in parietal regions lead to disassociation with the self and out-of-body experiences (Urgesi et al., 2010). Thus, a self/other overlap may also relate to the pro-social qualities of self-transcendence. In terms of the increased connectedness, neurological correlates might be oxytocin and arginine vasopressin, which are associated with social connection (Landgraf and Neumann, 2004; Heinrichs et al., 2009; Grigorenko, 2011). However, the research on these neuropeptides is based on animal models, so the validity of these claims for humans is limited. Researchers also found the vagus nerve is activated during self-transcendent positive emotions such as awe, compassion, gratitude, and love (Keltner, 2009; Kok and Fredrickson, 2010; Thayer et al., 2012; Kok et al., 2013). The vagus nerve is a cranial nerve that interfaces with parasympathetic control of the heart, lungs, and bladder. Since a parasympathetic response is associated with a sedative state, the vagus nerve activation would decrease under “stress” and increase when the body is at rest or in a peaceful state, which could be associated with positive emotions such as self-transcendence.

While we have described a simplified model of the neurological processes related to self-transcendence in Figures 3, 4, a more comprehensive model is suggested by Newberg and Yaden (2018). These authors have put forth a recent review on the brain processes involved with altered-states of consciousness, which they define as a type of self-transcendent experience “associated with intense experiential components and are frequently interpreted in reference to religious and/or spiritual concepts.” Their model shows these major brain areas likely involved in altered-states of consciousness including the prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortex, the thalamus, the parietal lobe, the hippocampus and amygdala, as well as the hypothalamus and autonomic nervous system.

3. Literature Search Methods

We conducted a literature search using ACM, IEEE, and ProQuest databases to identify relevant research articles. We performed additional hand searches of references in the retrieved literature. Our search for studies tapping into the measurement of self-transcendence included articles and empirical work published until September 1, 2019. The search strategy considered only studies published in English. Primary search terms were “self-transcendence,” “mindfulness,” “flow,” “self-transcendent positive emotions,” “awe,” “peak experiences,” “mystical experiences,” “altered states of consciousness and transcendence” plus “measurement,” “assessment,” “questionnaire.” Secondary search terms included self-transcendence and the name of the scale spelled out or abbreviated. We excluded measures that did not fit the definition of self-transcendence as “a feeling of decreased self-saliency and increased connection.”

4. Results: Research/Evaluation Methods for Investigating Self-Transcendence

Methods of investigating self-transcendence can be divided into five major categories: questionnaires and surveys; diary and journal entry; interviews; neurological and physiological measures; and behavioral measures. Next, we will evaluate and discuss these methods with reference to the table in the Supplementary Material.

4.1. Questionnaires and Surveys

Many attempts have been made to capture the subjective nature of self-transcendence through self-report questionnaires. Defining self-transcendence is of utmost importance because, of course, we cannot compare results if researchers are using different definitions. On the other hand, self-transcendence can be looked at as having several facets; studying these different facets could be a way of looking at self-transcendence from multiple lenses, e.g., Yaden et al. (2017a). Next, we briefly describe questionnaires for each of these facets, and we further discuss the validity and reliability in section 5.2.

4.1.1. Mindfulness Questionnaires

Mindfulness questionnaires can be divided into two different categories: state and trait. State mindfulness is a fluid or short-term mindset that is flexible in how you perceive the world; it is linked to a concurrent experience or transient mood. Trait mindfulness, on the other hand, is a more permanent facet of personality, most likely linked to genetics. A state, when repeatedly elicited, can often result in a trait change (Kiken et al., 2015). Most mindfulness questionnaires are aimed at assessing trait mindfulness: Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) (Brown and Ryan, 2003), Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI) (Buchheld et al., 2001), Kentucky Inventory of Mindfulness Skills (KIMS) (Baer et al., 2004), Cognitive and Affective Mindfulness Scale (CAMS-R) (Feldman et al., 2006), Southampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ) (Chadwick et al., 2008), and Five-Facet Mindfulness Scale (FFMQ) (Baer et al., 2006). Capturing one's ability to get into a state is challenging, and there are different approaches. One approach is by using several facets to capture mindfulness. FFMQ and KIMS both have multiple facets to measure mindfulness, and report good internal consistencies, α = 0.89 and 0.76 − −0.91 respectfully. However, despite their perceived comprehensiveness, researchers have found the multiple sub-scales provide redundant information and the time to complete the surveys is long. MAAS, FMI, and CAMS-R all purposefully have one total score because the researchers believe mindfulness is comprised of many things that cannot be separated out into sub-scales. MAAS is the mostly widely used mindfulness scale, with an internal consistency of 0.82, test-retest reliability of 0.81, and adequate convergent and discriminant validity. FMI is the second most widely used mindfulness scale, but unlike MAAS it includes a focus on curiosity as a part of the definition of mindfulness; FMI has an internal consistency of 0.93. CAMS-R is the shortest holistic measure of mindfulness with only 12 items. Internal consistencies ranged from 0.74 − 0.80. Lastly, SMQ is different from all the other scales because it measures a mindful approach to distressing thoughts and images. SMQ has an internal consistency α = 0.89; significant correlation with MAAS r = 0.57.

There exist only two mindfulness questionnaires that aim to measure state mindfulness: State Mindfulness Scale (SMS) (Tanay and Bernstein, 2013) and Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS) (Lau et al., 2006). TMS contains two sub scales, curiosity and decentering, with internal consistencies of 0.93 and 0.91, respectively. SMS has also shown a high reliability of α = 0.95. While TMS seems to be more widely used, it is intended for meditators, so languages used may not be accessible to novices. SMS, on the other hand, was designed to be used with all levels of mindfulness meditation and does not use jargon terms.

Another questionnaire that aims to measure meditation regardless of practice type, i.e., mindfulness or otherwise, is the Meditation Depth Questionnaire (MEDEQ) (Piron, 2001). MEDEQ is widely used because of its meditation practice agnostic quality and has good convergent validity (0.64 − 0.93) as well as high internal consistency (α = 0.92).

4.1.2. Flow Questionnaires

Flow seems to be assessed mostly in the context of either games or physical activity. While many designers and developers aspire to flow, few actually have a scientifically valid and reliable way to measure it. Csikszentmihalyi (1990) started to develop the Flow Scale using semi-structured interviews, but that scale did not seem to have high reliability estimates. Jackson and Marsh (1996) have developed two separate scales to measure flow, based off of Csikszentmihalyi's flow theory: Flow State Scale (FSS) (Jackson and Marsh, 1996), Flow State Scale 2 (FSS-2) and Dispositional Flow Scale 2 (DFS-2) (Jackson and Eklund, 2002; Hamari and Koivisto, 2014). These scales are very similar in the questions they ask and their theoretical underpinnings, with similarly strong content and construct validity and reliability estimates ranging from 0.81 to 0.90. The difference is in the application: FSS measures flow experienced within a particular event and DFS measures the frequency of flow experiences in a given event in general.

4.1.3. Self-Transcendent Positive Emotions and Awe Questionnaires

Positive emotions might be assessed using popular emotion scales such as the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (Watson et al., 1988), Self Assessment Manikin Scale (SAM scale) (Bradley and Lang, 1994), or International Affective Picture System (IAPS) (Lang et al., 1997). However, these do not focus on the specific positive emotions relating to self-transcendence, namely elevation, compassion, admiration, gratitude, love, and awe. There exist three scales that attempt to capture these emotions: Inclusion of Other in Self Scale (IOS) (Aron et al., 1992), Dispositional Positive Emotion Scales (DPES) (Shiota et al., 2006), and Modified Differential Emotions Scale (mDES) (Fredrickson et al., 2003). IOS uses a set of Venn Diagrams to assess self-other overlap with good test-retest reliability (r = 0.83 overall), and good convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity. DPES assesses seven different positive emotions that are closely related to self-transcendence, with good reliability estimate ranging from 0.75 to 0.92. However, DPES does not assess positive emotions directly, but more people's dispositions toward positive emotions. mDES measures 20 discrete emotions, including positive emotions amusement, awe, contentment, gratitude, hope, love, pride, sexual desire, joy, interest, surprise and eight negative emotions. mDES can be used as a reliable tool for the assessment of positive (α = 0.79) and negative (α = 0.69) emotions for a specific time frame, e.g., the past 24 h. Thus, mDES is a retrospective, rather than present acute state, method.

Awe is a specific self-transcendent positive emotion, and it is only relatively recently that researchers have attempted to create scales to capture awe. There are five awe questionnaires: The Nature of Awe Questionnaire (NAQ) (Shiota et al., 2007), Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S) (Yaden et al., 2018), Situational Awe Scale (SAS) (Krenzer, 2018), Awe and the Small-self (AS) (Piff et al., 2015), and Gratitude/Awe Scale (GrAw-7) (Büssing et al., 2018). Small-self is conceptualized by Piff et al. (2015) as “a relative diminishment of the individual self and its interests vis-a‘-vis something perceived to be more vast and powerful than oneself” (p. 2). NAQ and AS are the most widely used, mostly because AWE-S, SAS, and GrAw-7 have only been developed recently. NAQ is a reliable measure of the small-self (α = 0.82), but AS does not seem to have any studies mentioning reliability estimates. AS neglects the additional content suggested by treatments of awe conducted outside of psychology (e.g., admiration mixed with wonder and fear; sublime), whereas SAS reflects psychological, philosophical, and religious perspectives. Both SAS and AWE-S claim to be more robust measures of awe compared to NAQ and AS. AWE-S shows strong internal consistency for each of its six factors: altered time perception (α = 0.91), self-diminishment (α = 0.89), connectedness (α = 0.87), vastness (α = 0.85), physical sensations (α = 0.81), and need for accommodation (α = 0.80) (Yaden et al., 2018). AWE-S also demonstrated strong reliability (α = 0.93). A paper on validating the SAS showed good convergent validity compared to DPES-awe (Shiota et al., 2006) and AS (Piff et al., 2015); additionally there was adequate construct validity among the four factors: connection (α = 0.84), oppression (α = 0.83), chills (α = 0.81), and diminished self (α = 0.69) (Krenzer, 2018). The authors of SAS point out that while SAS and AWE-S share a similar factor structure, it differs in that AWE-S includes additional factors that did not emerge in SAS (i.e., perceived vastness and need for accommodation) and SAS includes a negatively valenced sub-scale of awe that may capture a broader range of contexts. GrAw-7 is an extended version of the 3-item Gratitude/Awe subscale of the Spiritual Practices (SpREUK-P) scale. It is intended to measure dispositional gratitude/awe as a trait, namely feelings of gratitude, reverence/awe, and experiencing the beauty in life. GrAw-7 is strongly correlated with the perception of the sacred in life (Daily Spiritual Experience Scale) in religious persons, but it can also be used in secular populations. One validation study showed it had good internal consistency (α = 0.82) (Büssing et al., 2018); however, no other study has confirmed this.

4.1.4. Peak and Mystical Experience Questionnaires

Mystical experience, as conceptualized by Stace (1960) and summarized by Robertson (1962), is “the intuition of oneness with the ultimate spiritual reality” (p. 180). Some of the earliest research on self-transcendence aimed to objectively measure peak and mystical experiences in the context of peak performance, sport, religion and spirituality, hallucinogenic experiences. These scales are still relevant and used today. Two scales look at peak experiences: Peak Scale (PS) (Mathes, 1982) and Experience Questionnaire (EQ) (Privette and Bundrick, 1987). PS measures tendencies for peak experience, and not assessing peak experience themselves, whereas EQ explores the phenomenon of peak experience. Both PS (α = 0.94) and EQ (α = 0.70) have adequate reliability.

There are five scales that attempt to measure mystical experience: Mysticism Scale (MS) (Hood, 1975), Daily Spiritual Experience Scale (DSES) (Underwood and Teresi, 2002), Spiritual Transcendence Scale (Spirit-TS) (Piedmont, 1999), Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ) (MacLean et al., 2012), and States of Consciousness Questionnaire (SOCQ) (Griffiths et al., 2006). Spirit-TS assesses mysticism as a personality trait with adequate reliability (α = 0.86). Spirit-TS generalizes to a wide range of faith traditions. MS and DSES both are used within the context of religious and spiritual experience, and report sufficient construct validity and internal consistency (α = 0.93). However, they may miss different noetic qualities of mystical experience because of their focus. SOCQ is similar to MS, expect more for hallucinogenic experiences. MEQ also assesses hallucinogenic effects but in laboratory settings. SOCQ contains 43 items from the MEQ, but adds distractor items to ensure whether a participant had a “complete” mystical experience or not. Both scales have shown good internal validity and reliability.

4.1.5. Altered States of Consciousness and Transcendence Questionnaires

The questionnaires we have described so far have focused on specific constructs of self-transcendence. Here, we describe and discuss questionnaires that attempt to look at altered states of consciousness and transcendence generally. Three scales assess the occurrence of the phenomena on different consciousness dimensions: Phenomenology of Consciousness Inventory (PCI) (Pekala and Levine, 1982), Altered States of Consciousness Rating Scale (OAV) (Bodmer et al., 1994; Studerus et al., 2010), 5-Dimension Altered States of Consciousness (5D-ASC) (Dittrich, 1998; Dittrich et al., 2010), and 11-Dimension Altered States of Consciousness (11D-ASC) (Studerus et al., 2010). All of these scales are very lengthy (53 to 94 items) and have acceptable levels of reliability. They have been used mostly with understanding experiences from taking hallucinogens such as psilocybin. The consciousness dimensions themselves are different across these scales. 11D-ASC has better discriminant and convergent validity scores compared to 5D-ASC, but has lower reliability. The original OAV is shorter than the 5D-ASC with comparable reliability. The new OAV may provide a better fit, and lower order scales do have sufficient validity and reliability (42-items).

Two scales measure self-transcendence as non-dual awareness: Non-dual Awareness Dimensional Assessment-Trait (NADA-T) and Non-dual Awareness Dimensional Assessment-State (NADA-S) (Hanley et al., 2018). Both NADA-T and NADA-S are 13 items long with good reliability (r = 0.93). While, they have not been used widely in assessing self-transcendence since they are relatively new scales, they promise to capture aspects of self-transcendence that other measures do not. For example, TMS captures the decentering aspect of self-transcendence, while NADA measures the form and formless absorption (i.e., relational self-transcendence) and experiential emptiness of self (i.e., annihilational self-transcendence) (Hanley et al., 2018). Another consciousness related measure of self-transcendence is Ego-Dissolution Inventory (EDI)(Nour et al., 2016). EDI is 16 items that assess the associated feeling of increased union with one's surroundings through two constructs: ego-dissolution and ego-inflation. It has sufficient construct validity (ρ = 0.735), and is primarily used in measuring ego-dissolution in psychedelic experiences.

Other scales look at self-transcendence through different lenses: Self-transcendence Scale (STS) (Reed et al., 1989), Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) (Cloninger et al., 1993b), Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI) (Levenson et al., 2005), the Self Expansiveness Level Form (SELF) (Friedman, 1983), and the Portrait Values Questionnaire Revised-RR-Self-transcendence Subscale (PVQ-RR-ST) (Schwartz, 2012). STS, TCI, SELF, and PVQ-RR-ST measure self-transcendence as a trait, and not a state. STS is used widely in nursing studies and has been adapted for use with adolescent, adult, and older adult populations. Reliability ranges from 0.80 to 0.88, with test-retest reliability of 0.95. SELF is designed to assess self-expansiveness, which has been operationalized as three distinct levels based on a spatial-temporal cartography of self-concept: personal, middle, and transpersonal. A validation study found SELF to have good internal consistency (α = 0.66 − 0.81) and test-retest reliability (r = 0.8–0.83) (MacDonald et al., 1994). TCI has gone through several iterations. The original has 226 items with True/False answers and the revised TCI has 240 items using a 5-point Likert scale. Self-transcendence is a sub scale with 33 True/False items (TCI) or 26 5-point Likert scale items (TCI-R). SELF and STS are significantly shorter (15 items) but may not capture the subtle nuances of qualitative experience. TCI is comprehensive, but is not inclusive of religious and spiritual components of self-transcendence. ASTI measures transcendence as a developmental process that is more lifespan inclusive, and is used in studies assessing wisdom since self-transcendence is considered a high level of psychological development. Its reliability is satisfactory (α = 0.83), and moderate internal consistency of .66. PVQ-RR-ST is a sub-scale of the PVQ. PVQ is an alternative to the Schwartz Value Survey (SVS) that measures the ten basic values of persons not educated in Western schools that emphasize abstract, context-free thinking. It can be used with children as well as adult populations, and has good internal consistency (α = 0.76 − 0.85). Overall, the state scales seem to capture more the experience of self-transcendence, but have not been generalized across different experiences of self-transcendence. The trait and developmental scales of self-transcendence are good for assessing people's tendencies for self-transcendence, but may not be suitable for short-term measures.

4.2. Diary and Journal Entry

Diary studies, in general, are a research method for collecting information on participants' lives. Specifically, researchers are usually most interested in the behaviors, thoughts, and feelings of participants over time. Diary methods have been around for a long time, but modern diary methods are systematic and often highly structured. I will review three different diary methods: Narrative Recall, Diary Entries, and Experience Sampling.

Narrative Recall for studying emotion is when participants would recall and write about a personal experience involving the theoretically defined, prototypical elicitor of each emotion. Narrative Recall has been used in emotion research for decades as a well-validated measure (Shaver et al., 1987), however Griskevicius et al. (2010) further developed the method by recognizing that using the emotion word as a prompt was biasing participants and decreasing validity. With Griskevicius et al. (2010)'s method, participants are not asked to write about an “emotion word” so that the definition is not constrained by researchers; participants can give rich descriptions of positive emotions. Narrative recall has been used to capture both the state and trait experience of awe (Shiota et al., 2007; Piff et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2018; Zhao et al., 2018), and has been used in classifying emotions generally including self-transcendent emotions of love, compassion, amazement, wonderment, awe, and elation (Shaver et al., 1987). However, this method is very time consuming to both conduct and analyze. Diary Entries are long-term written accounts that are suitable for studying the long-term effect of an event, when the event cannot be studied in a laboratory, and when researchers want to know how participants reflect about an experience through time (Bolger et al., 2003). This method can give researchers new insight into a phenomenon and participants can give rich, thick descriptions. For example, researchers have used diary entries to study mindfulness, dreams, and mystical experiences (Hall and Van de Castle, 1966; Sokel, 1978; Cangas et al., 2008; Zhu et al., 2017). However, these accounts can be distorted, very selective and not representative of the whole experience (Janssens et al., 2018). For salient activities, they are less subject to retrospective bias than are interview data. Experience Sampling was one of the first psychology methods for studying emotions (Csikszentmihalyi et al., 1977) as a way to provide a valid instrument to describe variations in self-reports of mental processes. Participants report (e.g., write notes or type in an app) what they were experiencing just before the prompt of a pager or similar signaling device. The signals are typically given at random times to minimize instrumentation effects based on the expectation of a page at a particular time. The experience sampling method has been used in studying flow in different contexts: self-expansion in couples (Graham, 2008), experience at work (Fullagar and Kelloway, 2009), peak experience in the Grand Canyon (Panter, 2017), during sport (Jackson, 2000), and in virtual reality (Gaggioli, 2012). This method has also been used in studying different forms of focused attention or mindfulness (Easterlin and Cardea, 1998; Abuhamdeh and Csikszentmihalyi, 2012; Smallwood et al., 2012). Experience Sampling reports acceptable levels of internal consistency and test-retest reliability, but researchers recommend using other measures to corroborate the results and ensure validity (Csikszentmihalyi and Larson, 2014).

4.3. Interviews

Contemporary research on consciousness started with functionalist psychologists, phenomenology in philosophy, and the Gestalt approach. However, the popularity of behaviorism eclipsed this research until the 1960s with increased interest of altered states of consciousness (Cardeña and Pekala, 2014). Many scientists dismissed introspective methods claiming they were unreliable, biased, and un-objective. Others conceded that no science is observer-free because we always view science from a certain sociocultural and psychological perspective. Different interviewing methods of assessing introspective experience, such as self-transcendence, can be phenomenological, concurrent, or retrospective.

4.3.1. Phenomenological Interviews

The interpretive framework in phenomenology is essentially postmodern: human experience is complex, experienced subjectively, and has meaning. Many articles on phenomenology research are focused on how the data are analyzed rather than how its obtained (Moustakas, 1994). Giorgi (1985), a phenomenology researcher, stated that questions should be broad and open ended so the participant can express their view point extensively and limit interviewer bias. However, he failed to describe how to proceed in the interview after stating those initial generalist questions. Bevan (2014) describes a three-part guide to phenomenological interviewing to help better consistency and address criticism of approach being too open ended and biased: contextualization, apprehending the phenomenon, and clarification. Phenomenological interviewing can provide thick, rich descriptions where the essence of experience or phenomenon emerges from participants rather than existing theory or research. Moreover, it does not impose existing theoretical models and is very comprehensive. However, it may be too structured for some researchers, an understanding of assumptions is required, philosophical ideas are abstract, participants need to be carefully chosen, finding participants may be difficult if the phenomena is very specific and rare, bracketing personal experiences is needed, and it is very time consuming. Phenomenological methods have been used to measure constructs related to self-transcendence such as lucid dreaming (Kitson et al., 2018), peak experiences (Panzarella, 1980), wonder (Gallagher et al., 2015), altered states of consciousness in float tanks (Kjellgren et al., 2004), meditation (Downey and Cohen, 2018); but also has been used to measure self-transcendence itself in women with breast cancer (Coward, 1990, 1991), gay men and women with AIDS (Coward and Lewis, 1993; Coward, 1995), and in healthy populations (Coward, 1996). Microphenomenology is a method for exploring lived experience very finely or for a singular event (Petitmengin, 2006), with a much more narrow view than phenomenology. This method is also comprehensive, with a rigorous technique, high level of reliability, and fine degree of granularity. However, the interviewer needs to be highly trained in the interviewing technique to ensure reliability. Microphenomenology has been used to study awe (Quesnel et al., 2018; Stepanova et al., 2019), meditative experiences (Petitmengin et al., 2017; Prpa et al., 2018; Przyrembel and Singer, 2018), and altered states of consciousness caused by DMT—a fast-acting tryptamine psychedelic (Timmermann et al., 2019). Both phenomenology and microphenomenology can be useful in describing self-transcendence, but which one you use depends on if you want to study the experience generally and across life times (phenomenology), or if you want to look at one specific experience in great detail (microphenomenology).

4.3.2. Concurrent Interviews

Concurrent methods have the advantage of gathering rich data while the experience is happening, which significantly reduces memory bias that comes from retrospective reports. The drawbacks include being not easily quantifiable, it may not be a comprehensive account, there is a chance of reactivity during the process, and some experiences may not translate well because of temporal and representational limitations of language and translation between thought and experience. However, these methods seem to have good concurrent validity. Thinking Out Loud (Ericsson and Simon, 1980; Watson, 2009) is one method where participants verbally report what they are experiencing while they are experiencing it. This method can be helpful for real-time experiential reports, but can be intrusive or disruptive of the experience while it happens. Researchers have used the Thinking Out Loud method to understand experiences of mindfulness (Aslan et al., 2016), transformative experience in the context of diabetes (Paterson et al., 1999), and altered states of consciousness (Marcusson-Clavertz and Cardeña, 2011). Cued-recall Debrief (Omodei and McLennan, 1994), in contrast, allows the participant to first have the experience while they are recorded from the firs- person perspective, and then are asked to verbally report their thoughts and feelings while watching the replay. The major advantage of this is not disrupting the experience itself; this is especially important for a transcendent experience where disrupting the participant to ask questions would most likely negatively impact the experience. These methods have been used in several Human-Computer Interaction (HCI) and Psychology studies of emotion (see Supplementary Material for details). For example, the cued-recall debrief method has been used to assess real-time emotions (Bruun and Ahm, 2015; Bruun et al., 2016), affective responses (Bentley et al., 2005), and decision making (Omodei and McLennan, 1994). While this method seems promising in assessing states of self-transcendence in real-time, only a few studies have actually managed to implement this measure for self-transcendence specifically (Quesnel et al., 2018; Stepanova et al., 2019).

4.3.3. Retrospective Interviews

Retrospective interviews are used when interviewing the participant during the experience is not possible or impractical. Often this is done with self-transcendent experiences that have already happened to the participant. The most common type of retrospective interview for self-transcendence is the semi-structured interview (Kvale and Brinkmann, 2015). The goal is to capture rich, detailed nuanced answers that uncover subjective differences and specificities of the interviewee. Participants verbally respond to questions that were created beforehand. Normally all of the questions are asked, and similar wording is used from interviewee to interviewee. However, sometimes questions may not always follow the exact order. While this allows more freedom to explore self-transcendence, which may also provide new insight, this method is time consuming and the quality of data largely depends on the interviewer's skills. Rather than open interviewing, which has virtually no structure, semi-structured interviews are pertinent when the researcher is beginning the investigation with a fairly clear focus rather than a general focus, so that they can address more specific issues. Structured interviews might be too restrictive for exploring a phenomenon like self-transcendence. Another form of retrospective interviewing is Retrospective Reports; Maslow (1959) provided a guide to investigating peak experiences where participants are asked to describe the most wonderful or joyful experience of their lives, how they felt, and if and how it affected their lives through in-depth interviews or open-ended surveys (15–30 min). Similar to semi-structured interviews, there are usually only a few pre-determined questions. However, retrospective reports include a coding scheme and member checking in the process to improve reliability and validity. Both semi-structured interviews and retrospective reports can provide rich detail and a comprehensive view on self-transcendence. However, both suffer from similar drawbacks of memory bias.

4.4. Neurological and Physiological Measures

There are a variety of physiological measures that tap into self-transcendent experiences. Many studies involve meditation, which is related to self-transcendence, but not much research is on self-transcendence in particular. There is a challenge with physiological measures mapping to specific states and emotions because our bodies are complex and the same physiological response (e.g., increased heart rate) may serve multiple functions and correlate with different states depending on other factors. That said, we can still use physiological measures as yet another lens into understanding self-transcendence. We will discuss two types of measures: neurological and non-neurological (which we will simply call physiological).

4.4.1. Neurological Measures

Neurological changes in the brain can be observed through several techniques, each with advantages and disadvantages. There are two types of neurological techniques: direct and indirect measures. Direct measures look at neural activity of the brain itself, while indirect measures look at other bodily changes that are related to direct brain activity. Direct measures include brain lesions, Electroencephalography (EEG), and Magnetoencephalograph (MEG). Indirect measures are Positron Emission Tomography (PET), Single Photon Emission Computed Tomography (SPECT), Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI), Functional Magnetic Imaging (fMRI), and Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS).

Brain lesions are one of the oldest neurological methods for investigating the link between brain function and behavior (Broca, 1861). Participants have portions of their brain removed and a comparison is made between performance before and after the lesion and consequent deficits are noted. One can also study participants whose brains have been damaged through natural means. Typically, this method is now only used when participants need surgery on that particular part of the brain, since it would be unethical to run a true experiment. While this method shows us insight into the causally necessary function of brain structures, it is extremely invasive, often not generalizable, and assumes that discrete anatomical modules deal with different cognitive functions. Studies have shown lesions to the parietal lobe increase the propensity for self-transcendence (Urgesi et al., 2010). Another direct measure of brain function is EEG, which directly measures brain electrical activity just under the scalp via electrodes placed on the skin (Berger, 1929). This method is widely used to study experiences such as meditation and hallucinogens because it is non-invasive, portable, relatively low-cost, and provides good temporal resolution. Different meditation types may correspond with different brain waves: focused attention (beta: 13–30 Hz; gamma: 30–50 Hz), open monitoring (theta: 4–8 Hz), and automatic self-transcending (alpha-1: 8–10 Hz) (Cahn et al., 2012); although neuroelectric correlates of meditation have not yet been firmly established and should be considered with caution (Fox and Cahn, 2018). This method is good for recording real-time measures of brain activity, and several consumer EEG products make it accessible to researchers. However, there are several drawbacks to EEG. First, electric conductivity may vary widely from person to person and also over time, due to the natural conductivities of other tissues such as brain matter, blood, and bones. Because of this, it is sometimes unclear exactly which region of the brain is emitting a signal. Also, it can be difficult to determine the source of the underlying signal since EEG only records signals directly below the scalp. It also requires a reference electrode, which may not always be reliable. EEG has been used to measure meditation-induced altered states of consciousness (Lehmann et al., 2001; Aftanas and Golocheikine, 2002), mystical experience (Tenke et al., 2017), and near death experience during meditation (Beauregard et al., 2009). EEG has also been used to measure related constructs of self-transcendence including brain states during transcendental meditation (Banquet, 1973; Yamamoto et al., 2006), yogic meditation and trance (Das and Gastaut, 1955), and mindfulness meditation (Travis and Arenander, 1994; Kerr et al., 2013). The popularization of mindfulness meditation in Western culture and the development of consumer EEGs has stemmed several meditation-related digital experiences and apps that claim to measure mindfulness (Hinterberger, 2011; Choo and May, 2014; Prpa et al., 2015; Amores et al., 2016; Gervais et al., 2016; Kosunen et al., 2016; Gu and Frasson, 2017; Antle et al., 2018). A more recent technique that is similar to EEG is MEG, which maps brain activity by recording magnetic fields produced by electrical currents occurring naturally in the brain, using very sensitive magnetometers (Cohen, 1968). Here, participants may be positioned on a movable examination table or seated in a comfortable chair within a room that shields out any electric and magnetic noise that could interfere with the exam. They will be positioned within the stationary helmet that contains the MEG detectors placed on the head. MEG has been used to measure brain activity during transcendental meditation (Yamamoto et al., 2006), mindfulness meditation (Kerr et al., 2013; Wong et al., 2015), and mindfulness-induced selflessness (Dor-Ziderman et al., 2013) and altered states of consciousness (Berkovich-Ohana et al., 2013a). Compared to EEG, MEG has improved spatial resolution because magnetic fields are less distorted by bone (skull). Moreover, activity is localized with more accuracy compared to EEG and is reference free. However, MEG is not as good as fMRI at localizing activity. Some other drawbacks are that MEG needs specialized shielding to eliminate the magnetic interference, it requires highly sensitive instrumentation, and it is not portable. Both MEG and EEG meet high standards of reliability and validity.

In terms of indirect measures of brain function, PET illustrates where neural firing is taking place by injecting a small amount of radioactive tracer and taking a picture of the cerebral blood flow (Phelps et al., 1975). This method is important for understanding the role of various neurotransmitters in cognition, such as glutamate and GABA (Newberg et al., 2001). PET's advantages include little artifacts in the scan, high spatial resolution, and quick scan times (30 s). The disadvantages are that we can only locate generalized areas of brain activity and not specific locations, it is expensive, invasive, and not suitable for children or vulnerable populations because of the radioactive elements. PET has been used to measure meditation-induced altered states of consciousness (Herzog et al., 1990; Lou et al., 1999; Kjaer et al., 2002), spiritual experiences (Borg et al., 2003), mindful awareness (Karlsson et al., 2011; Hakamata et al., 2013), and psilocybin-induced altered states of consciousness (Vollenweider et al., 1997; Gouzoulis-Mayfrank et al., 1999). Similar to PET is SPECT, which uses a radioactive compound and gamma camera to render a 3D map of a person's body (including brain) (Kuhl and Edwards, 1963). While SPECT is relatively inexpensive compared to PET, it does have longer imaging times and has a high rate of false positives. SPECT can be used to complement any gamma imaging study, where a true 3D representation can be helpful, e.g., tumor imaging, infection (leukocyte) imaging, thyroid imaging or bone scan. It can be used to provide information about localized function in internal organs, such as functional cardiac or brain imaging. SPECT has been used to show how meditation-induced peak experience activates the prefrontal cortex (Newberg et al., 2001). It has also been used to measure brain states during religious and mystical states (D'Aquili and Newberg, 1993), hallucinogenic experiences from mescaline (Hermle et al., 1998), and ayahuasca (Riba et al., 2006; Sanches et al., 2016). Another indirect measure of brain functioning is MRI, which measures the change in blood flow related to energy level in brain cells by typically using the blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) contrast, (Lauterbur, 1973). Participants are subjected to a powerful magnet that aligns the hydrogen nuclei of water atoms inside of their brain and can determine changes in the amount of brain tissue (structural MRI) or in the amount of blood flow (functional MRI). Typically, the more active a particular brain structure, the more blood flow it receives. Functional MRI (fMRI) uses the same basic principles as structural MRI, except that the former measures metabolic activity around anatomical structures whereas the latter takes 3D images of the anatomical structures themselves. Like MRI, fMRI has been used to quantify the volume of particular brain structures. fMRI can be used to produce activation maps showing which parts of the brain are involved in a particular mental process, whereas MRI only shows the structures. This means that MRI can be used to measure changes in volume or make comparisons between different subjects, e.g., meditation (reviews: Gotink et al., 2016; Afonso et al., 2020). fMRI has many advantages: non-invasive; poses little health risk; usable for all ages, including in-utero; wide spread availability; relatively low cost per scan; good spatial resolution; and better temporal resolution than other indirect neuroimaging methods, although not as good as EEG. The disadvantages are that fMRI is expensive, not portable, very noisy, and cannot be used to evaluate neurotransmitter systems. The noise aspect is particularly troublesome for studying self-transcendence because it can be difficult to get into this state when a giant, loud machine is distracting the participant. A study with transcendental meditators showed that the noise from fMRI strongly influenced both subjective and neurophysiological responses during meditation practice, calling into question the fMRI results (Travis et al., 2020). There is no agreed-upon average value for fMRI reliability. There are so many factors spread out across so many levels of influence that it is almost impossible to summarize the reliability of fMRI with a single value. fMRI is one of the more widely used methods of brain imaging for self-transcendence and its constructs. Researchers have used fMRI to capture brain regions that are active during meditation (Lazar et al., 2000; Wang et al., 2011), spiritual experiences (Miller et al., 2019), near death experiences (Beauregard et al., 2009), admiration and compassion (Immordino-Yang et al., 2009), awe experiences (van Elk et al., 2019), and drug-induced altered states of consciousness with DMT (Daumann et al., 2010), psilocybin (Lebedev et al., 2015; Carhart-Harris et al., 2018), LSD (Carhart-Harris et al., 2016; Tagliazucchi et al., 2016), and ayahuasca (de Araujo et al., 2012; Palhano-Fontes et al., 2015). fNIRS is a non-invasive and safe optical technique that uses light emitting diodes or laser diodes to measure human cerebral cortex oxygenation changes in response to certain stimuli/tasks. Compared to fMRI, fNIRS is silent and tolerable to movement artifacts, it measures oxygenated hemoglobin (O_2Hb) as well as deoxygenated hemoglobin (HHb), it allows long-time continuous measurements as well as repeated measures in a short time span, and it has a higher temporal resolution. Some of fNIRS's limitations are that it has poor spatial resolution, little standardization, and can have unstable accuracy between sessions. Overall, fNIRS is good for when you want a small, wearable device that is movement robust, especially for neuromonitoring and neurorehabilitation. In the context of self-transcendence, fNIRS has been used to measure positive emotions such as awe, gratitude, and love (Hu et al., 2019), brain states during mindfulness meditation (Gundel et al., 2018), sustained attention meditation (Zheng et al., 2019), and drug-induced altered states of consciousness with psilocybin (Scholkmann et al., 2019).

4.4.2. Physiological Measures

There are many techniques for measuring physiological processes related to self-transcendence. Psychophysiological measures use indices of bodily responses that reflect variation in psychological states (Potter and Bolls, 2012). Unlike direct measures of the brain, psychophysiological correlations are not causal, but are instruments in which to test theories. Psychophysiological techniques use specific tools for measuring the physiological response: GooseCam for philoerection (goosebumps) (Benedek et al., 2010); Pneumograph for respiration (Marey, 1878); Electrodemograph (EDG) for skin electrical activity (Vigouroux, 1879); Photoplethysmograph (PPG) for blood flow changes (Hertzman, 1937); Electrocardiograph (ECG/EKG) for heart rate and heart rate variability (HRV) (Einthoven, 1895); Electromyograph (EMG) for muscle electrical activity (Hardyck et al., 1966). Each of these psychophysiological instruments allows measuring specific characteristics of self-transcendence that would otherwise not be captured in self-reports (Bartholow and Bolls, 2013). For example, a study of awe showed a mixed valence by means of sEMG and a unique physiological pattern resembling a freezing or dissociation-like response (Chirico et al., 2017).

GooseCam investigates goosebumps as a response to strong emotional experiences by using a high-quality camera to record the surface of the skin. Goosebumps have been linked to constructs of self-transcendence such as peak experiences and awe (Grewe et al., 2009). Several research groups have used goosecam to measure instances of awe, e.g., (Benedek et al., 2010; Sumpf et al., 2015; Wassiliwizky et al., 2017; Quesnel and Riecke, 2018). This method is shown to be robust to variations in skin characteristics, but there is reduced internal validity because this method does not distinguish between the subjective sensation of chills and the objective phenomenon of visible piloerection. Pneumograph is one of the earliest physiological measures that records velocity and force of chest movements during respiration. Participants wear one or more straps around their chest and abdomen, which measures the expansion and contraction (i.e., volume) of their breathing. Respiration rate has been shown to correlate with level of mindfulness, i.e., slower breathing indicates higher degree of mindfulness (Ahani et al., 2014; Wielgosz et al., 2016), while showing mixed response to peak experiences and self-transcendent emotions, i.e., indicating both physiological arousal and physiological calming effects (Mori and Iwanaga, 2017; Clayton et al., 2019). While this is a relatively easy method to use, movement artifacts can influence the results. EDG measures skin electrical activity directly (skin conductance and skin potential) and indirectly (skin resistance) using electrodes placed over the digits or hand and wrist. As sweat increases in the ducts, the resistance at the skin goes down, resulting in higher levels of recorded skin conductance. Skin conductance is a reliable measure of emotional arousal that reflects the level of activation within the emotional and motivational systems. Skin conductance has been used as a measure of awe (Grewe et al., 2009; Chirico et al., 2017), peak experiences (Mori and Iwanaga, 2017), self-transcendent positive emotions (Shiota et al., 2011), and self-transcendence generally (Clayton et al., 2019). One thing to keep in mind when using EDG is that it only measures emotional arousal, not valence. PPG measures the relative blood flow through a digit sensor attached by a Velcro band to the fingers or to the temple to monitor the temporal artery. PPG signal is strong and robust, but a huge range in individual differences and indirect measures of self-transcendence makes reliability challenging. A more direct measure of heart activity is ECG/EKG, which measures the electrical activity of the heart by using electrodes placed on the skin. ECG is more accurate than PPG in measuring HRV, however wearing electrodes on the torso, wrists, or legs can feel more invasive, especially if the participant has lots of hair. Heart activity in general is innervated by both the central and peripheral nervous system, so it can be difficult to determine the nature of the feeling, i.e., whether it is positive or negative. Another drawback of using HRV is that one must account for baselines and individual differences. HR and HRV have been used as measure of self-transcendence (Clayton et al., 2019), awe (Grewe et al., 2009; Sumpf et al., 2015; Wassiliwizky et al., 2017), peak experiences (Mori and Iwanaga, 2017), mindfulness (May et al., 2016), and self-transcendent positive emotions (Shiota et al., 2011). Finally, EMG, i.e., surface EMG, measures the electrical signal associated with muscle activity. In terms of emotion research, this is typically done by placing electrodes on specific facial muscles involved in the valence of emotional processing: corrugator supercilii, orbicularis oculi, and zygomaticus major. Facial EMG is a reliable and valid method for measuring emotional valence (Potter and Bolls, 2012). fEMG has been used to measure self-transcendent emotions (Clayton et al., 2019), awe (Chirico et al., 2017), and flow (Cheron, 2016). EMG can be a useful diagnostic tool, the electrodes are easy and quick to apply with minimal discomfort. Yet, adipose tissue (fat) can affect EMG recordings, compliant skin is needed for accurate readings, and muscle cross talk can occur.

All of these physiological measures can provide an objective view of self-transcendence because it is hard to fake physiological data. That said, the Hawthorne effect can threaten internal validity, i.e., people behave differently when they know that they are being observed. There is also an external validity threat when using a convenience sample, which is not representative enough of the population, because there is a huge variation in individual differences and a large, representative sample is needed to account for them. Correct sensor placement is also critical because wrong placement will result in measuring the incorrect bodily response. Most transcendence related research that uses physiological measures is actually measuring arousal and valence generally, and not a discrete emotion. This is because our bodily reactions are complicated and intertwined. However, there are some researchers trying to measure discrete emotions, such as awe, gratitude, and love (Koelstra et al., 2012; Shiota et al., 2017).

4.5. Behavioral Measures

Behavioral measures are overt actions and reactions that are observed and recorded, exclusive of self-reported behavior. Behavioral measures do not suffer the same reliability and validity threats that introspective measures do. However, self-transcendence is generally considered a psychological phenomenon, so studying it from a behaviorist perspective is difficult. Proxy measures look at aspects thought to be related to self-transcendence, such as pro-sociality and empathy, e.g., (Van Lange et al., 1997; Saroglou et al., 2008; Rosenberg et al., 2013; Piff et al., 2015; Quesnel et al., 2018). Other proxy measures include body size estimation (van Elk et al., 2016) and time perception (Rudd et al., 2012), where a smaller size estimation and slower time perception were correlated to self-transcendence. Although, a recent paper found experimentally induced awe does not affect implicit and explicit time perception (van Elk and Rotteveel, 2019). The main method of studying self-transcendence behavior is observation, where the researcher watches (i.e., observes) behavior as it organically and spontaneously unfolds in a natural environment. No one really knows when the method of observation for scientific study dates back to. In modern times, it is used in grounded theory and ethnographic approaches (Creswell, 2012). Participants are either aware (overt) or not (covert) they are being studied. Observations can be in a controlled setting, in a natural environment, or the researcher can join in as part of the group. Observational research is used when other data collection procedures, such as surveys, questionnaires, etc. are not effective or adequate. They are also used when the goal is to evaluate an ongoing behavior process, event, or situation; or when there are physical outcomes that can be readily seen. For example, gestures, interactions, voice qualities, and facial features like eye gaze, pupil dilation, and blinking can indicate an affective state (Sharma and Gedeon, 2012). Typically, these behaviors are coded and analyzed across data sources. Observations can explain meaning and context, which is important for self-transcendence. However, they can be viewed as too subjective, the Hawthorne effect can affect validity, and results depend on the researcher's role. Overt observation may affect validity of findings, and covert may be unethical, have a high potential for role conflict, and may not tell the whole story. This method is also very time consuming and, in the case of self-transcendence, should probably be used in conjunction with other methods rather than relying on a single data source. Some researchers have used observation in studying awe (Quesnel et al., 2018), mindfulness (Gervais et al., 2016; van Rooij et al., 2016; Antle et al., 2018), self-transcendence in women with breast cancer (Coward, 1990), and ego dissolution after taking ayahuasca (Uthaug et al., 2018).

5. Discussion

5.1. Conceptual Challenges of Self-Transcendence Methods

5.1.1. Different Underlying Theories for Different Measures

Generally, there are three views of self-transcendence: physiological (neurobiological mechanisms), phenomenological (experiential), and conceptual (traits, values). Each of these theories offer a different perspective in which to define self-transcendence, which in turn impacts how one measures self-transcendence.

Physiological measures of self-transcendence aim to objectively capture states of self-transcendence. For example, people report having a mystical experience when there is decreased activation in parietal functioning (Azari et al., 2001; Beauregard and Paquette, 2006; Johnstone et al., 2012). That said, even seemingly objective measures are not directly capturing self-transcendence as a whole but are instead measuring correlates or outcomes of self-transcendence, e.g., increased heart rate.

Phenomenological measures of self-transcendence aim to capture the lived experience of self-transcendence as a whole and highly individualized phenomenon, e.g., through ground theory interviews (Garcia-Romeu et al., 2015). Whereas physiological measures are reductionist, phenomenological measures of self-transcendence seek to understand the context in which self-transcendence occurs, the physical sensations, and the perceptual, cognitive and affective experiences.

On the other hand, conceptual measures of self-transcendence consider a wider range of social, developmental, and personality facets of self-transcendence that physiological and phenomenological measures do not capture. Still, conceptual measures should be used with caution since they are each developed from a specific theoretical lens. For example, self-transcendence as a personality trait in Nursing is derived from Neo-Piagetian theories. Moreover, when looking at the mindfulness facet of self-transcendence, these measures are derived from either Buddhist psychology (e.g., FMI) or with a pragmatic approach (e.g., KIMS) (Sauer et al., 2013). There are other conceptually based measures of self-transcendence that view it as a state, not a personality trait, e.g., TCI (Cloninger et al., 1993b).

5.1.2. Unclear Constructs and Facets of Self-Transcendence

Koenig (2008) postulates that the term “spirituality” is often conflated with general good mental well-being or health. This is problematic both because the construct should be distinct and because this definition deviates from the original meaning of the word, i.e., religious or secular person living by a set of ethical and moral values and meanings. Self-transcendence can also suffer from this same issue. For example, several measures only consider the positive aspects of self-transcendence and exclude the “dark side,” including absorption, suggestibility, and dissociation, which can have psychotic outcomes (MacDonald and Holland, 2002).

The term “self-transcendence” is so broadly defined in the literature that it can encompass a wide range of constructs that each contain multiple facets. For example, mindfulness is considered a construct of self-transcendence (Yaden et al., 2017a), and mindfulness itself has several facets, e.g., FFMQ (Baer et al., 2006). With this broad and widely encompassing definition, it is difficult to derive a measure that is specific to self-transcendence and at the same time encompasses all its aspects.

5.1.3. Direct Measures vs. Outcomes and Correlates of Self-Transcendence

Self-transcendence in itself can be difficult to capture and measure, especially because re-creating the right conditions for a self-transcendence to occur is challenging. Some methods ask the participant to recall a time when they had a self-transcendent experience, e.g., AWE-S (Yaden et al., 2018) or EQ (Privette and Bundrick, 1987), while others try to evoke the experience, e.g., micro-phenomenology (Petitmengin, 2006). In order to avoid having to reproduce self-transcendence or better tap into a previous experience, researchers have looked at proxy measures. For example, self-transcendence is reported to be correlated with self-diminishment (van Elk and Rotteveel, 2019) and pro-social tendencies (Carlo and Randall, 2002). These proxy measures can be powerful tools when direct measures cannot be used or are nor feasible. However, researchers need to be clear on what exactly they measure—self-transcendence or its outcome.

5.2. Empirical Support of Self-Transcendence Methods

5.2.1. Internal Consistency Reliability

One measure of internal consistency (reliability) is Cronbach's alpha. α ≥ 0.70 is considered acceptable in most social science research situations. We found that the questionnaires all had sufficient alpha. However, the proxy measures were still lacking measures of internal consistency, with the exception of the Prosocial Tendencies Measure (PTM) (Carlo and Randall, 2002) that showed high internal consistency of α = 0.92. The differences in high and low alpha scores could be the difference in narrow and broad scope. Questionnaires measuring specific constructs of self-transcendence will have higher alpha coefficients than those measuring self-transcendence more broadly. Additionally, those questionnaires with a higher number of items will have higher alphas. As we can see in Table 1, the more broad measures of altered-states of consciousness and self-transcendence, positive emotions, as well as proxy measures have lower alphas than the specific measures of awe, flow, mystical or peak experiences, and mindfulness.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Reliability coefficients of internal consistency measured with Cronbach's alpha and test-retest measured with Pearson's r, and number of items for questionnaires that measure general self-transcendence (left) and specific constructs of self-transcendence (right).

5.2.2. Test-Retest Reliability

Test-retest reliability determines the consistency of a test taken multiple times, often denoted as r. Often, r ≥ 0.7 is the accepted value, but this will depend on the time between test and retest, the length of the test, what is being measured, and the characteristics of the sample. We found that test-retest reliability has been infrequently evaluated in measures of self-transcendence. Approximately half of all the questionnaires tested for test-retest reliability (see Table 1). Of those, most questionnaires reported adequate test-retest reliability. Those with low test-retest reliability may be due to the nature of the test; trait measures of self-transcendence will be more stable over time compared to state measures. Test-retest reliability is an important psychometric property for establishing stability of these measures over time. We suggest more validation studies are needed to establish the stability of the construct.

5.2.3. Validity

We considered four types of validity: construct, content, face, and criterion.

The questionnaires of self-transcendence that report adequate validity as the following: PCI, STS, SELF, TCI, ASTI, IOS, PTM, MEDEQ, AWE-S, SAS, DFS-2, EQ, MS, MEQ, SOCQ, MAAS, and TMS. Those that specifically mention construct validity, which can be a measure of overall validity, are PCI, STS, AWE-S, DFS-2, EQ, MS, MEQ, and MAAS.

Diary and journal entry methods overall have good construct validity and they have been used in emotion research for decades as a well-validated measure. Interview measures have good validity when rigorous technique is applied and they can also bring richness to the data that would not otherwise have been captured in a questionnaire or physiological measure. Observations too can have strong validity and bring an in-depth understanding when they are natural.

Neurological measures of self-transcendence vary in accuracy, and not many have been specifically used to measure self-transcendence, but have been used to measure mindfulness meditation and self-transcendent emotions, including decreased self-saliency and increased connection (see Figure 3). If considering self-transcendence as an emotional state, then neuroimaging can be limited since there is little evidence that emotions are correlated directly to one specific brain region (Lindquist et al., 2012). As we can see in Figure 3, self-transcendence spans multiple brain regions and networks, making it difficult for some neuroimaging techniques with low spatial resolution to accurately measure self-transcendence. For example, EEG and MEG have poor spatial resolution while having a relatively high temporal resolution compared to fMRI. Thus, capturing the specific brain areas of self-transcendence would be more accurate with fMRI. However, capturing self-transcendence in a large noisy machine where the participant is supine also comes with its own challenges.

Physiological measures of self-transcendence are also not straightforward because their indices are often simply emotional arousal and valence; it is difficult to correlate a single physiological response with a complex emotion such as self-transcendence. That said, when combined with other measures that provide ground truth, such as interviews, they can be good indicators that a change has occurred from a normal state of consciousness to an altered one. One research group showed that one can represent subjective experiences of emotion as categorical somatotopic maps (Nummenmaa et al., 2014). While physiological measures can accurately measure arousal, those seeking to find a measure of self-transcendence should use these physiological measures with caution. It is the opinion of some researchers that there is no physiological “finger print” of any emotion and that our efforts to measure emotions should be directed “to observe, map, and better understand the breadth, nature, and function of this variation in emotion categories” (Siegel et al., 2018, p. 36).

5.3. Recommendations

Having reviewed the literature on measures of self-transcendence, we have some recommendations on which measure to use to asses self-transcendence. That said, the answer depends on the research context and purpose. Since self-transcendence is broadly defined and contains many constructs, we suggest taking a multi-method approach and triangulating the results for increased validity. One existing methodology that seemed most aligned with capturing the nature of self-transcendence is neurophenomenology—the synthesis of neuroimaging to provide rich empirical data (e.g., EEG, MRI, PET) and valid descriptions of first-person subjective experience (e.g., phenomenology). Neurophenomenology was first introduced by Laughlin et al. (1992), and Varela (1996), and developed by Maturana and Varela (1991), Varela (1996), Thompson (2010). For the phenomenological component, Husserl (1983) developed a method of bracketing knowledge in order to focus on the experience itself and provide an unbiased account. Varela used this interview method to obtain reliable descriptions of first-person experience. Another interpretation of neurophenomenology is the use of micro-phenomenology—a qualitative, second-person interview for the research of first-person accounts of lived experiences. Micro-phenomenology was first introduced by Pierre Vermersch as “entretien d'explicitation” (explication interview) (Vermersch, 1994) and further developed by Petitmengin (2006); it is now becoming a central element of Varela's neurophenomenology. Taking from neurophenomenology, we suggest two broad approaches of assessing and investigating self-transcendence: (1) in-the-moment and (2) as a value, developmental process, or personality trait.

5.3.1. Assessing and Investigating Self-Transcendence in the Moment

We propose combining some of the following methods for investigating self-transcendence in-the-moment, which we will elaborate on subsequently:

Questionnaires and Surveys

• Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S);

• Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS);

• Non-dual Awareness Dimensional Assessment Scale-State (NADA-S);

• Inclusion of Other in Self Scale (IOS);

• Dispositional Positive Emotion Scales (DPES);

• Mysticism Scale (MS)

Observation

• Observation

Interviews

• Micro-phenomenology

Neurological and Physiological Measures

• Electroencephalography (EEG);

• Function Near-Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS);

• Electrodemograph (EDG);

• Facial Electromyograph (EMG)

Diary and Journal Entry

• Diary Entry

Questionnaires and Surveys

From the list of questionnaires previously described, we suggest the Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S), Toronto Mindfulness Scale (TMS), Non-dual Awareness Dimensional Assessment Scale-State (NADA-S), Inclusion of Other in Self Scale (IOS), Dispositional Positive Emotion Scales (DPES), and Mysticism Scale (MS). To measure state self-transcendence, specifically the decreased self-salience component, we propose using the AWE-S, TMS, and NADA-S. There are very few questionnaires that measure state qualities to begin with, and what exist currently are often not well-validated. AWE-S, although new, already has some validation studies to support it and likewise for TMS and NADA-S. All of these scales measure a specific aspect related to self-transcendence and may not be all encompassing of the experience, but perhaps with both they will fill in the gaps until we have a better state measure for self-transcendence. Lastly, IOS might be useful in measuring the connectedness aspect of self-transcendence. This measure is unique in that it uses pictures instead of language, which might be useful for capturing the ineffable aspects of self-transcendence. IOS, TMS, and AWE-S are good for assessing “at the present moment,” so it might be useful to do pre- and post-measures of these three scales to see if there is a change. Although DPES and MS are not explicitly measuring self-transcendence in the moment, past life experience can play a huge role in both if self-transcendence occurs and how it is experienced. DPES can provide an assessment of participants' pre-disposition to self-transcendence and MS can provide a brief history of past self-transcendent experiences. With these two measures, one might better control for individual differences. Both of these questionnaires have good reliability and validity, and are widely used. Other questionnaires that assess pre-disposition or traits are not as inclusive for all types of self-transcendence, are not well-validated, or are very long and redundant.

Observation

Although psychometric tests can be helpful in understanding complex inner states such as self-transcendence, they often fail to capture the rich, qualitative experiential aspects. Observation methods are useful in filling this gap to see what people experienced both leading up to and during a self-transcendent experience. We suggest a setting where the participant has consented to be recorded but the researcher is not present so the participant can experience self-transcendence without the Hawthorne effect and at the same time researchers can more ethically collect their data. An example might be in a natural environment such as a meditation retreat. A lab could suffice and bring more control to the study, but we recommend creating a welcome and warm space rather than the usual cold and sterile conditions of a lab since mindset and setting are important.

Interviews

Since there are so many facets to explore with self-transcendence, a rich qualitative description would nicely complement the quantitative questionnaires and provide detail and context to the observations. We suggest using the micro-phenomenology method in conjunction with neurological and physiological measures we will describe in the next section. Micro-phenomenology seems to be a good fit for studying self-transcendence in this case because it allows the participant to fully experience self-transcendence without disturbing them with think-aloud methods. It also attempts to avoid memory bias by situating the participant back in the experience they just had through evocation. In contrast, a phenomenological interview seems more fitting to describe multiple experiences over time, whereas micro-phenomenology is about one specific event. Researchers still need to be cautious when using this method since the definition of self-transcendence is still quite broad. That is, there is a risk of missing a critical component while looking too deeply at one component of self-transcendence. Other retrospective interviewing methods seem more flawed and prone to memory bias compared to micro-phenomenology. Cued-recall debrief tries to address the issue of memory bias by showing a first-person recording of the experience during the interview, yet does not have the evocation element that makes micro-phenomenology potentially more valid.

Neurological and Physiological Measures

We would recommend to select some, or if feasible all, of the following neurological and physiological measures in conjunction with micro-phenomenology: EEG, fNIRS, EDG, and facial EMG. Most neurological measures are indirectly measuring something or are very expensive and not accessible, e.g., fMRI and MEG. Of all the neurological measures, EEG and fNIRS seem to be the best methods in this case because they are non-invasive, relatively inexpensive, portable, and are complementary measures of brain activity. EEG is already extensively used in studying related experiences such as meditation, so it seems like a natural extension to study self-transcendence. However, because of EEG's limited spatial resolution, it would be better to use fNIRS, which has a higher spatial resolution, in conjunction with EEG to achieve a more accurate measure of brain activity. In order to corroborate these measure of brain activity, we suggest using both EDG and facial EMG to measure emotional arousal and valence, respectfully. Both are required to get an accurate representation of the participant's affective state. Other physiological measures could be used to further corroborative the results. Still, it is preferable to not have to subject participants to too many electrodes as this can be uncomfortable, unnecessary, and maybe not all that more helpful.

Diary and Journal Entry

The last measure we would consider for investigating self-transcendence is diary entry. Self-transcendence can be a powerful and profound experience, but the real benefits continue well after the initial experience. With self-transcendence, one hopes to have a general sense of increased well-being, pro-sociality, and positive social values. Therefore, having participants complete diary entries would show how they reflect about the experience through time and show if and how a self-transcendent experience made a lasting and significant impact on their lives.

5.3.2. Assessing and Investigating Self-Transcendence as a Value, Developmental Process, or Personality Trait

In this section, we suggest methods that are appropriate for measuring self-transcendence as a value, developmental process, or personality trait. We propose the following methods for investigating self-transcendence in their respective contexts based the their validity, reliability, suitability, and weighing the advantages and disadvantages:

Value

• Portrait Values Questionnaire Revised-RR-Self-transcendence Subscale (PVQ-RR-ST)

• Semi-structured interviews

Developmental Process

• Self-transcendence Scale (STS)

• Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI)

• Diary Entries

• Retrospective Interviews

Personality Trait

• Dispositional Positive Emotion Scales (DPES)

• Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI)

• Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS)

• Dispositional Flow Scale 2 (DFS-2)

• Gratitude/Awe Scale (GrAw-7)

• Spiritual Transcendence Scale (Spirit-TS)

• Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)

• Computed Tomography (CT)

• Experience Sampling

• Semi-structured Interviews

Value

Values are generally described as stable, broad goals that are important to us in life (Schwartz, 2012). Values express a person's motivations which may or may not be reflected in behavior. Thus, behavioral, observational, or physiological measures would not be suitable for measuring self-transcendence as a value. PVQ-RR-ST appears to be the only questionnaire measuring self-transcendence as a value. Here, values relate to universalism and benevolence, specifically measuring acceptance, appreciation, and understanding of the welfare of others (Schwartz, 2012). Semi-structured interviews can be used to capture rich, detailed nuanced answers that uncover subjective differences and specificities of the interviewee. Other interview methods we reviewed were either focused on self-transcendence as experiential, e.g., phenomenology or retrospective, or were about self-transcendence in-the-present-moment, e.g., microphenomenology, think-aloud -protocol, and cued recall debrief. Diary and journal methods may provide some context to why a participant may value self-transcendence or not.

Developmental Process

Reed (2013)'s theory of self-transcendence aligns with Frankl and Maslow's developmental stages that need to be fulfilled in order to have a sense of purpose in life. Reed's theory has been primarily used in older adult populations where self-transcendence plays an integral role in healing and in dignified acceptance of the end of life. From this theory, Reed et al. (1989) developed the Self-transcendence Scale (STS) to identify intrapersonal, interpersonal, transpersonal, and temporal experiences characteristic of later life, which reflect expanded boundaries of self. The Adult Self-Transcendence Inventory (ASTI) is another questionnaire to measure transcendence as a developmental process that is more lifespan inclusive (Levenson et al., 2005). Between the two, we recommend the STS because it is more widely used, it has higher internal consistency, and it reports good construct validity; whereas the ASTI has questionable test-retest reliability and construct validity.

We also recommend diary entries in this case because they obtain rich, longitudinal narratives of individuals' lives that can be a window into how self-transcendence develops over time. Retrospective interviews can also help to understand how self-transcendence as a developmental process changes over time.

Personality Trait

Personality traits are defined as descriptions of people in terms of relatively stable patterns of behavior, thoughts, and emotions, and are therefore summaries of an individual's responses and behaviors (McCrae and Costa, 2003). In order to assess self-transcendence as a personality trait, one should consider the following questionnaires that measure an individual's behavioral, cognitive, and emotional tendencies. The following all have good reliability and encompass both the broad and specific construct of self-transcendence, depending on what you are most interested in. Moreover, we found these questionnaires tended to span across different populations and faiths. That said, if one is interested in a specific context of self-transcendence, e.g., in the Christian faith or involving hallucinogenics, then it would be worth considering measures specifically with those in mind.

For general measures of self-transcendence as a trait, we recommend the Dispositional Positive Emotion Scales (DPES) that assess seven different positive emotions: joy, contentment, pride, love, compassion, amusement, and awe (Shiota et al., 2006). The second measure is Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) (Cloninger et al., 1993b). In terms of measuring specific constructs of self-transcendence, the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) measures general tendency to be attentive to and aware of present moment experience in daily life (Brown and Ryan, 2003). Second, the Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2) measures the frequency of flow experiences in chosen physical activity in general (Jackson and Eklund, 2002). Third, Gratitude/Awe Scale (GrAw-7) measures feelings of gratitude, reverence/awe, and experiencing the beauty in life (Büssing et al., 2018). Lastly, Spiritual Transcendence Scale (Spirit-TS) captures a personal tendency to turn toward a larger, objective perspective of reality than one's personal life (Piedmont, 1999).

When considering self-transcendence as a trait, rather than a state, the type of brain imaging used should reflect that goal. Self-transcendence as a state would require measuring brain activity, and thus would need a functional imaging method such as EEG, fMRI, PET, SPECT, or MEG. On the other hand, measuring self-transcendence as a trait can use either a functional imaging method such as EEG or a structural imaging method such as computerized tomography (CT) or MRI. As Cahn and Polich (2006) point out in their review paper on neuroimaging studies of meditation states and traits, EEG can be used to measure changes in brainwave amplitude in comparisons such as resting vs. meditating or beginner vs. expert meditators. In CT scanning, a computer is used to construct images of the brain (or other parts of the body) from a series of x-ray scans at multiple angles, giving much better resolution than standard x-rays. MRI scans, on the other hand, not only provide higher-definition images than CT scans, but they can also show sagittal and coronal sections of the brain, not just the axial sections to which CT scans are limited. While EEG measures coherence between brain activity or spectral values in different frequency ranges, MRI focuses on activations in different brain areas (Raffone and Srinivasan, 2010). Therefore, it appears the best way to measure self-transcendence as a trait is through a combined method of EEG and MRI, which several studies of meditation have already done (Cahn and Polich, 2006).

Since traits look at patterns of behavior, thoughts, and emotions, experience sampling can help to describe patterns of an individual's daily experience, to evaluate the common experience of situations, and to study the dynamics of emotions and other subjective states. Semi-structured interviews can also help determine patterns; although observation would directly measure behavior, it would be very time consuming to see stable patterns of behavior that are warranted for personality traits.

5.4. Future Directions

After reviewing the current measures of self-transcendence, we have recommended a multi-method approach with a list of different measures researchers should consider based on their validity, reliability, and suitability. We recommend this multi-method approach since self-transcendence is currently defined in many ways depending on the theoretical lens. As such, we divided our recommendations according to how one conceptualizes self-transcendence, namely as a state, trait, value, or developmental process. We recognize that we have recommended a long list of methods, and due to limited resources many researchers hoping to study self-transcendence may not have the means to use all of the measures we have recommended. In this case, we would still strongly suggest taking a multi-method approach and select the few measures that would be most relevant and accessible to the study. For example, questionnaires are a well-validated method of assessing some aspects of self-transcendence, and triangulating that data with a physiological measure and qualitative interviews can increase confidence in the findings. If one were to select only one type of measure, we might suggest taking a qualitative approach since at its core self-transcendence is a subjective phenomena that cannot be reduced to physiological and neurological correlates. Already, there are researchers taking a multi-method approach to measuring self-transcendence or one of its constructs and we might take these studies as examples of how to move forward in the field. Reinerman-Jones et al. (2013) used questionnaires, EEG, ECG, fNIRS, and phenomenological interviews to investigate awe and wonder in a simulated space travel scenario. Zhu et al. (2017) used a package of probes in the form of photos, diaries, and cards together with questionnaires, ECG, and semi-structured interviews to assess mindfulness.

One of the major challenges in measuring self-transcendence is in capturing it while it is happening. Recent developments in virtual reality (VR) have provided an opportunity to create realistic experiences that can support a self-transcendence. Thus, moving forward, we might use this technology as a way to further explore self-transcendence. Quesnel et al. (2018) used VR to help elicit awe and wonder during a journey through a forest, under water, and space to finally gaze upon the Earth. Quesnel and Riecke (2018) were able to elicit awe in four different Google Earth VR environments. Chirico et al. (2018) found that VR can effectively elicit feelings of awe, and that emotions elicited by virtual and natural conditions were not significantly different (Chirico and Gaggioli, 2019). With the development of VR headsets with integrated physiological and neurological sensors, it seems possible for VR to both help elicit self-transcendence and at the same time measure participant's bodily responses. Moreover, the use of first-person recordings of the virtual scene can be used for cued-recall debrief interviews.

Another challenge we identified in our review is that self-transcendence is measured differently depending on the theoretical lens used to define it. Self-transcendence as an emotional state seems to be the most widely used according to our review. One of the critiques of this view comes from embodied appraisal theory, which states that emotions are perceptions of bodily changes according to somatic theories of emotion (James, 1884). Here, emotions register changes in our bodies, but do not represent bodily changes (Prinz, 2004). Instead of response-dependent properties, emotions represent core relational themes (Lazarus and Lazarus, 1991), i.e., emotions have meaning. According to this theory, we can use physiological signals that correlate with self-transcendence, but self-transcendence, like any emotion, does not represent a change in body physiology. Self-transcendence does represent core relational themes, which inform us about our relationship with the world, embody our convictions, and factor intelligibly into our decisions. Although not everyone would agree with Prinz's embodied appraisal theory of emotions, e.g., Pineda (2015), future research on measuring self-transcendence should carefully consider what measures are actually correlations or proxies of self-transcendence rather than self-transcendence itself. Qualitative experience seems to be the key to measuring and understanding self-transcendence directly.

6. Conclusion

Within this paper, we described and reviewed measurement approaches to self-transcendence. Based on our review, we provided recommendations and suitability of methods given different research contexts. We want to reiterate that there is no one answer to the question “how does one measure self-transcendence.” The optimal choice needs to be determined by the researcher's objectives and the research questions themselves. We have seen that the construct of self-transcendence itself varies greatly depending on theoretical groundings. Whatever the theoretical framing, we suggest that researchers employ a mixed-methods approach in order to embrace the enactive quality of self-transcendence with empirical rigor.

Author Contributions

AK, AG, and BR contributed to the initial planning and topic of the review. AC provided guidance on review conceptualization. AK performed the screening and eligibility process and conducted a qualitative research synthesis of the data. AC provided feedback and additional research synthesis. AK wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Open access publication fees funded by Simon Fraser University.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.547687/full#supplementary-material

References

Abuhamdeh, S., and Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2012). Attentional involvement and intrinsic motivation. Motivat. Emot. 36, 257–267. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9252-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Afonso, R., Kraft, I., Aratanha, M., and Kozasa, E. (2020). Neural correlates of meditation: a review of structural and functional MRI studies. Front. Biosci. 12, 92–115. doi: 10.2741/s542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aftanas, L. I., and Golocheikine, S. A. (2002). Non-linear dynamic complexity of the human EEG during meditation. Neurosci. Lett. 330, 143–146. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(02)00745-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ahani, A., Wahbeh, H., Nezamfar, H., Miller, M., Erdogmus, D., and Oken, B. (2014). Quantitative change of EEG and respiration signals during mindfulness meditation. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 11, 1–11. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-11-87

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Akyalcin, E., Greenway, P., and Milne, L. (2008). Measuring transcendence: extracting core constructs. J. Transpers. Psychol. 40, 41–59.

Google Scholar

Amores, J., Benavides, X., and Maes, P. (2016). “PsychicVR: increasing mindfulness by using virtual reality and brain computer interfaces,” in Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI EA '16 (New York, NY: ACM), 2. doi: 10.1145/2851581.2889442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Antle, A. N., Chesick, L., and Mclaren, E.-S. (2018). Opening up the design space of neurofeedback brain-computer interfaces for children. ACM Trans. Comput. Hum. Interact. 24, 1–33. doi: 10.1145/3131607

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aron, A., Aron, E. N., and Smollan, D. (1992). Inclusion of other in the self scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 63, 596–612. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslan, I., Burkhardt, H., Kraus, J., and Andre, E. (2016). “Hold my heart and breathe with me: tangible somaesthetic designs,” in Proceedings of the 9th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, NordiCHI '16 (New York, NY: ACM), 92:1–92:6. doi: 10.1145/2971485.2996727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Azari, N. P., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G., Niedeggen, M., Hefter, H., Tellmann, L., et al. (2001). Neural correlates of religious experience. Eur. J. Neurosci. 13, 1649–1652. doi: 10.1046/j.0953-816x.2001.01527.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., and Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment 11, 191–206. doi: 10.1177/1073191104268029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J., and Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Banquet, J.-P. (1973). Spectral analysis of the EEG in meditation. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 35, 143–151. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(73)90170-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartholow, B. D., and Bolls, P. (2013). “Media psychophysiology: the brain and beyond,” in The Oxford Handbook of Media Psychology (Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195398809.013.0027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauregard, M., Courtemanche, J., and Paquette, V. (2009). Brain activity in near-death experiencers during a meditative state. Resuscitation 80, 1006–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.resuscitation.2009.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauregard, M., and Paquette, V. (2006). Neural correlates of a mystical experience in carmelite nuns. Neurosci. Lett. 405, 186–190. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedek, M., Wilfling, B., Lukas-Wolfbauer, R., Katzur, B. H., and Kaernbach, C. (2010). Objective and continuous measurement of piloerection. Psychophysiology 47, 989–993. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01003.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bentley, T., Johnston, L., and von Baggo, K. (2005). “Evaluation using cued-recall debrief to elicit information about a user's affective experiences,” in Proceedings of the 17th Australia Conference on Computer-Human Interaction: Citizens Online: Considerations for Today and the Future, OZCHI '05 (Narrabundah, ACT: Computer-Human Interaction Special Interest Group (CHISIG) of Australia), 1–10.

Google Scholar

Berger, H. (1929). Electroencephalogram in humans. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 87, 527–570. doi: 10.1007/BF01797193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Dor-Ziderman, Y., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013a). Alterations in the sense of time, space, and body in the mindfulness-trained brain: a neurophenomenologically-guided MEG study. Front. Psychol. 4:912. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013b). Studying the default mode and its mindfulness-induced changes using EEG functional connectivity. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 1616–1624. doi: 10.1093/scan/nst153

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bevan, M. T. (2014). A method of phenomenological interviewing. Qual. Health Res. 24, 136–144. doi: 10.1177/1049732313519710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., and Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain 127, 243–258. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T., and Seeck, M. (2002). Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature 419, 269–270. doi: 10.1038/419269a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bodmer, I., Dittrich, A., and Lamparter, D. (1994). Außergewöhnliche bewußtseinszustände–ihre gemeinsame struktur und messung. Welten Bewußtseins 3, 45–58.

Bolger, N., Davis, A., and Rafaeli, E. (2003). Diary methods: capturing life as it is lived. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 579–616. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, J., Andre, B., Soderstrom, H., and Farde, L. (2003). The serotonin system and spiritual experiences. Am. J. Psychiatry 160, 1965–1969. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.1965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, M. M., and Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuring emotion: the self-assessment manikin and the semantic differential. J. Behav. Therapy Exp. Psychiatry 25, 49–59. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(94)90063-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Broca, P. (1861). Remarques sur le siège de la faculté du langage articulé, suivies d'une observation d'aphémie (perte de la parole). Bulletin et Memoires de la Societe anatomique de Paris 6, 330–357.

Google Scholar

Brown, K. W., and Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84:822. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruun, A., and Ahm, S. (2015). “Mind the gap! Comparing retrospective and concurrent ratings of emotion in user experience evaluation,” in Human-Computer Interaction-INTERACT 2015, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, eds J. Abascal, S. Barbosa, M. Fetter, T. Gross, P. Palanque, and M. Winckler (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 237–254. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-22701-617

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruun, A., Law, E. L.-C., Heintz, M., and Eriksen, P. S. (2016). “Asserting real-time emotions through cued-recall: is it valid?” in Proceedings of the 9th Nordic Conference on Human-Computer Interaction, NordiCHI '16 (New York, NY: ACM), 37:1–37:10. doi: 10.1145/2971485.2971516

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchheld, N., Grossman, P., and Walach, H. (2001). Measuring mindfulness in insight meditation (Vipassana) and meditationbased psychotherapy: the development of the Freiburg Mindfulness Inventory (FMI). J. Medit. Medit. Res. 1, 11–34.

Google Scholar

Büssing, A., Recchia, D. R., and Baumann, K. (2018). Validation of the gratitude/awe questionnaire and its association with disposition of gratefulness. Religions 9:117. doi: 10.3390/rel9040117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., and Polich, J. (2012). Event-related delta, theta, alpha and gamma correlates to auditory oddball processing during vipassana meditation. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 100–111. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cahn, B. R., and Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol. Bull. 132:180. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cangas, A. J., Sass, L. A., and Pérez-Álvarez, M. (2008). From the visions of saint Teresa of jesus to the voices of schizophrenia. Philos. Psychiatry Psychol. 15, 239–250. doi: 10.1353/ppp.0.0187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardeña, E., and Pekala, R. J. (2014). “Researching states of consciousness and anomalous experiences,” in Varieties of Anomalous Experience: Examining the Scientific Evidence, 2nd Edn., eds E. Carde, S. J. Lynn, and S. Krippner (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 21–56. doi: 10.1037/14258-002

CrossRef Full Text

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Bolstridge, M., Day, C. M. J., Rucker, J., Watts, R., Erritzoe, D. E., et al. (2018). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: six-month follow-up. Psychopharmacology 235, 399–408. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4771-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carhart-Harris, R. L., Muthukumaraswamy, S., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., Droog, W., Murphy, K., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 4853–4858. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1518377113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlo, G., and Randall, B. A. (2002). The development of a measure of prosocial behaviors for late adolescents. J. Youth Adolesc. 31, 31–44. doi: 10.1023/A:1014033032440

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chadwick, P., Hember, M., Symes, J., Peters, E., Kuipers, E., and Dagnan, D. (2008). Responding mindfully to unpleasant thoughts and images: reliability and validity of the Southampton mindfulness questionnaire (SMQ). Brit. J. Clin. Psychol. 47, 451–455. doi: 10.1348/014466508X314891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cheron, G. (2016). How to measure the psychological “flow”? A neuroscience perspective. Front. Psychol. 7:1823. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chirico, A., Cipresso, P., Yaden, D. B., Biassoni, F., Riva, G., and Gaggioli, A. (2017). Effectiveness of immersive videos in inducing awe: an experimental study. Sci. Rep. 7:1218. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-01242-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chirico, A., Ferrise, F., Cordella, L., and Gaggioli, A. (2018). Designing awe in virtual reality: an experimental study. Front. Psychol. 8:2351. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chirico, A., and Gaggioli, A. (2019). When virtual feels real: comparing emotional responses and presence in virtual and natural environments. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Network. 22, 220–226. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0393

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choo, A., and May, A. (2014). “Virtual mindfulness meditation virtual reality and electroencephalography for health gamification,” in 2014 IEEE Games, Media, Entertainment (GEM) (Toronto, ON), 1–3. doi: 10.1109/GEM.2014.7048076

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clayton, R. B., Raney, A. A., Oliver, M. B., Neumann, D., Janicke-Bowles, S. H., and Dale, K. R. (2019). Feeling transcendent? Measuring psychophysiological responses to self-transcendent media content. Media Psychol. 1, 1–26. doi: 10.1080/15213269.2019.1700135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C., Svrakic, D., and Przybeck, T. (1993a). “A psychobiological model of temperament and character,” in The Development of Psychiatry and Its Complexity 50, 975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cloninger, C. R., Svrakic, D. M., and Przybeck, T. R. (1993b). A psychobiological model of temperament and character. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 50, 975–990. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820240059008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, D. (1968). Magnetoencephalography: evidence of magnetic fields produced by alpha-rhythm currents. Science 161, 784–786. doi: 10.1126/science.161.3843.784

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coward, D. D. (1990). The lived experience of self-transcendence in women with advanced breast cancer. Nurs. Sci. Q. 3, 162–169. doi: 10.1177/089431849000300408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coward, D. D. (1991). Self-transcendence and emotional well-being in women with advanced breast cancer. Oncol. Nurs. Forum 18, 857–863.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Coward, D. D. (1995). The lived experience of self-transcendence in women with AIDS. J. Obstetr. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 24, 314–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.1995.tb02482.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coward, D. D. (1996). Self-transcendence and correlates in a healthy population. Nurs. Res. 45:116. doi: 10.1097/00006199-199603000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coward, D. D., and Lewis, F. M. (1993). The lived experience of self-transcendence in gay men with AIDS. Oncol. Nurs. Forum 20, 1363–1368.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Crescentini, C., Chittaro, L., Capurso, V., Sioni, R., and Fabbro, F. (2016). Psychological and physiological responses to stressful situations in immersive virtual reality: differences between users who practice mindfulness meditation and controls. Comput. Hum. Behav. 59(Suppl C), 304–316. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creswell, J. W. (2012). Qualitative Inquiry and Research Design: Choosing Among Five Approaches. SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience. New York, NY: Harper Perennial Modern Classics.

Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M., and Larson, R. (2014). “Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method,” in Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology (Dordrecht: Springer), 35–54. doi: 10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8_3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., and Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activity and experience. J. Youth Adolesc. 6, 281–294. doi: 10.1007/BF02138940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D'Aquili, E. G., and Newberg, A. (1993). Religious and mystical states: a neuropsychological model. Zygon 28, 177–200. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9744.1993.tb01026.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Das, N., and Gastaut, H. (1955). Variations dans l'activité électrique du cerveau, du coeur et des muscles squelettiques pen-|dant la méditation et la transe du yoga. EEG Clin. Neurophysiol. 55, 211–219.

Daumann, J., Wagner, D., Heekeren, K., Neukirch, A., Thiel, C., and Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E. (2010). Neuronal correlates of visual and auditory alertness in the DMT and ketamine model of psychosis. J. Psychopharmacol. 24, 1515–1524. doi: 10.1177/0269881109103227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. H., and Vago, D. R. (2013). Can enlightenment be traced to specific neural correlates, cognition, or behavior? No, and (a qualified) Yes. Front. Psychol. 4:870. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00870

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Araujo, D. B., Ribeiro, S., Cecchi, G. A., Carvalho, F. M., Sanchez, T. A., Pinto, J. P., et al. (2012). Seeing with the eyes shut: Neural basis of enhanced imagery following ayahuasca ingestion. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 2550–2560. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21381

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Castro, J. M. (2017). A model of enlightened/mystical/awakened experience. Psychol. Religion Spiritual. 9, 34–45. doi: 10.1037/rel0000037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dittrich, A. (1998). The standardized psychometric assessment of altered states of consciousness (ASCs) in humans. Pharmacopsychiatry 31, 80–84. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dittrich, A., Lamparter, D., and Maurer, M. (2010). 5d-ASC: Questionnaire for the Assessment of Altered States of Consciousness. A Short Introduction. Zurich: PSIN PLUS.

Dorjee, D. (2016). Defining contemplative science: the metacognitive self-regulatory capacity of the mind, context of meditation practice and modes of existential awareness. Front. Psychol. 7:1788. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dor-Ziderman, Y., Berkovich-Ohana, A., Glicksohn, J., and Goldstein, A. (2013). Mindfulness-induced selflessness: a MEG neurophenomenological study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:582. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Downey, L. L., and Cohen, M. S. (2018). Virtual worlds and well-being: meditating with sanctuarium. Int. J. Virtual Augment. Reality 2, 14–31. doi: 10.4018/IJVAR.2018010102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Easterlin, B. L., and Cardeña, E. (1998). Cognitive and emotional differences between short- and long-term vipassana meditators. Imaginat. Cogn. Pers. 18, 69–81. doi: 10.2190/21GX-R4TD-XMD4-6P2W

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einthoven, W. (1895). Ueber die Form des menschlichen Electrocardiogramms. Eur. J. Physiol. 60, 101–123. doi: 10.1007/BF01662582

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, K. (1997). Meditation. 1.

Ericsson, K. A., and Simon, H. A. (1980). Verbal reports as data. Psychol. Rev. 87, 215–251. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, G., Hayes, A., Kumar, S., Greeson, J., and Laurenceau, J.-P. (2006). Mindfulness and emotion regulation: the development and initial validation of the cognitive and affective mindfulness scale-revised (CAMS-R). J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 29:177. doi: 10.1007/s10862-006-9035-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finisguerra, A., Crescentini, C., and Urgesi, C. (2019). Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation affects implicit spiritual self-representations. Neuroscience 412, 144–159. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2019.05.059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K. C., and Cahn, B. R. (2018). Meditation and the brain in health and disease. Medit. Brain. doi: 10.31231/osf.io/m2sez

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., et al. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 43, 48–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1966). Self-transcendence as a human phenomenon. J. Humanist. Psychol. 6, 97–106. doi: 10.1177/002216786600600201

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., and Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 84:365. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friedman, H. L. (1983). The self-expansive level form: a conceptualization and measurement of a transpersonal construct. J. Transpers. Psychol. 15:37.

Google Scholar

Fullagar, C. J., and Kelloway, E. K. (2009). Flow at work: an experience sampling approach. J. Occupat. Organ. Psychol. 82, 595–615. doi: 10.1348/096317908X357903

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaggioli, A. (2012). Quality of experience in real and virtual environments: some suggestions for the development of positive technologies. Stud. Health Technol. Informat. 181, 177–181. doi: 10.3233/978-1-61499-121-2-177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallagher, S., Reinerman-Jones, L., Janz, B., Bockelman, P., and Trempler, J. (2015). “The phenomenology of unprecedented experience: ontological and cognitive wonder,” in A Neurophenomenology of Awe and Wonder (London: Springer), 115–129. doi: 10.1057/9781137496058_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garcia-Romeu, A. (2010). Self-transcendence as a measurable transpersonal construct. J. Transpers. Psychol. 42, 26–47.

Google Scholar

Garcia-Romeu, A., Himelstein, S. P., and Kaminker, J. (2015). Self-transcendent experience: a grounded theory study. Qual. Res. 15, 633–654. doi: 10.1177/1468794114550679

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrison, K. A., Scheinost, D., Constable, R. T., and Brewer, J. A. (2014). Bold signal and functional connectivity associated with loving kindness meditation. Brain Behav. 4, 337–347. doi: 10.1002/brb3.219

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gervais, R., Roo, J. S., Frey, J., and Hachet, M. (2016). Introspectibles: tangible interaction to foster introspection. arXiv:1603.04581. doi: 10.1145/2839462.2839468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giorgi, A. (1985). “Sketch of a psychological phenomenological method,” in Phenomenology and Psychological Research, ed A. Giorgi (Pittsburgh, PA: Duquesne University Press), 8–22.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gotink, R. A., Meijboom, R., Vernooij, M. W., Smits, M., and Hunink, M. M. (2016). 8-week mindfulness based stress reduction induces brain changes similar to traditional long-term meditation practice-a systematic review. Brain Cogn. 108, 32–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., Schreckenberger, M., Sabri, O., Arning, C., Thelen, B., Spitzer, M., et al. (1999). Neurometabolic effects of psilocybin, 3, 4-methylenedioxyethylamphetamine (MDE) and d-methamphetamine in healthy volunteers: a double-blind, placebo-controlled pet study with [18f] FDG. Neuropsychopharmacology 20, 565–581. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00089-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J. M. (2008). Self-expansion and flow in couples' momentary experiences: An experience sampling study. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95:679. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grewe, O., Kopiez, R., and Altenmüüller, E. (2009). The chill parameter: goose bumps and shivers as promising measures in emotion research. Music Percept. 27, 61–74. doi: 10.1525/mp.2009.27.1.61

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R. R., Richards, W. A., McCann, U., and Jesse, R. (2006). Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology 187, 268–283. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0457-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigorenko, E. L. (2011). “Closeness of all kinds: the role of oxytocin and vasopressin in the physiology of spiritual and religious behavior,” in Thriving and Spirituality Among Youth: Research Perspectives and Future Possibilities, eds A. E. A. Warren, R. M. Lerner, and E. Phelps (John Wiley & Sons Inc), 33–60.

Google Scholar

Griskevicius, V., Shiota, M. N., and Neufeld, S. L. (2010). Influence of different positive emotions on persuasion processing: a functional evolutionary approach. Emotion 10:190. doi: 10.1037/a0018421

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gu, G., and Frasson, C. (2017). “Virtual sophrologist: a virtual reality neurofeedback relaxation training system,” in Brain Function Assessment in Learning, Lecture Notes in Computer Science, eds C. Frasson and G. Kostopoulos (Cham: Springer), 176–185. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-67615-9_16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gundel, F., von Spee, J., Schneider, S., Haeussinger, F. B., Hautzinger, M., Erb, M., et al. (2018). Meditation and the brain-neuronal correlates of mindfulness as assessed with near-infrared spectroscopy. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 271, 24–33. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gyamtso, R. K. T. (2001). Progressive Stages of Meditation on Emptiness. Auckland, NZ: Zhyisil Chokyi Ghatsal Publications.

Haidt, J., and Morris, J. P. (2009). Finding the self in self-transcendent emotions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 7687–7688. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0903076106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hakamata, Y., Iwase, M., Kato, T., Senda, K., and Inada, T. (2013). The neural correlates of mindful awareness: a possible buffering effect on anxiety-related reduction in subgenual anterior cingulate cortex activity. PLoS ONE 8):e75526. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0075526

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hall, C. S., and Van de Castle, R. L. (1966). The Content Analysis of Dreams. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Google Scholar

Hamari, J., and Koivisto, J. (2014). Measuring flow in gamification: dispositional flow scale-2. Comput. Hum. Behav. 40, 133–143. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanley, A. W., Nakamura, Y., and Garland, E. L. (2018). The nondual awareness dimensional assessment (NADA): new tools to assess nondual traits and states of consciousness occurring within and beyond the context of meditation. Psychol. Assess. 30:1625. doi: 10.1037/pas0000615

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardyck, C. D., Petrinovich, L. F., and Ellsworth, D. W. (1966). Feedback of speech muscle activity during silent reading: rapid extinction. Science 154, 1467–1468. doi: 10.1126/science.154.3755.1467

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasenkamp, W., and Barsalou, L. W. (2012). Effects of meditation experience on functional connectivity of distributed brain networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:38. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heinrichs, M., von Dawans, B., and Domes, G. (2009). Oxytocin, vasopressin, and human social behavior. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 30, 548–557. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hermle, L., Gouzoulis-Mayfrank, E., and Spitzer, M. (1998). Blood flow and cerebral laterality in the mescaline model of psychosis. Pharmacopsychiatry 31, 85–91. doi: 10.1055/s-2007-979352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hertzman, A. B. (1937). Photoelectric plethysmography of the fingers and toes in man. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 37, 529–534. doi: 10.3181/00379727-37-9630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herzog, H., Lele, V. R., Kuwert, T., Langen, K.-J., Kops, E. R., and Feinendegen, L. E. (1990). Changed pattern of regional glucose metabolism during yoga meditative relaxation. Neuropsychobiology 23, 182–187. doi: 10.1159/000119450

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinterberger, T. (2011). The sensorium: a multimodal neurofeedback environment. Adv. Hum. Comp. Int. 2011, 3:1–3:10. doi: 10.1155/2011/724204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hood, R. W. (1975). The construction and preliminary validation of a measure of reported mystical experience. J. Sci. Study Religion 14, 29–41. doi: 10.2307/1384454

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, X., Zhuang, C., Wang, F., Liu, Y.-J., Im, C.-H., and Zhang, D. (2019). fnirs evidence for recognizably different positive emotions. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 13:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Husserl, E. (1983). Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy: First Book: General Introduction to a Pure Phenomenology, Vol. 2. Springer Science & Business Media. doi: 10.1007/978-94-009-7445-6

CrossRef Full Text

Immordino-Yang, M. H., McColl, A., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. (2009). Neural correlates of admiration and compassion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106, 8021–8026. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0810363106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A. (2000). Joy, fun, and flow state in sport. Emot. Sport 6, 135–155. doi: 10.5040/9781492596233.ch-006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in physical activity: the flow state scale-2 and dispositional flow scale-2. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 24, 133–150. doi: 10.1123/jsep.24.2.133

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jackson, S. A., and Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: the flow state scale. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 18, 17–35. doi: 10.1123/jsep.18.1.17

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

James, W. (1884). What is an emotion? Mind 9, 188–205. doi: 10.1093/mind/os-IX.34.188

CrossRef Full Text

Janssens, K. A., Bos, E. H., Rosmalen, J. G., Wichers, M. C., and Riese, H. (2018). A qualitative approach to guide choices for designing a diary study. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 18, 1–12. doi: 10.1186/s12874-018-0579-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnstone, B., Bodling, A., Cohen, D., Christ, S. E., and Wegrzyn, A. (2012). Right parietal lobe-related “selflessness” as the neuropsychological basis of spiritual transcendence. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 22, 267–284. doi: 10.1080/10508619.2012.657524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josipovic, Z. (2019). Nondual awareness: consciousness-as-such as non-representational reflexivity. Prog. Brain Res. 244, 273–298. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.10.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karlsson, H., Hirvonen, J., Salminen, J., and Hietala, J. (2011). No association between serotonin 5-ht 1a receptors and spirituality among patients with major depressive disorders or healthy volunteers. Mol. Psychiatry 16, 282–285. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keltner, D. (2009). Born to be Good: The Science of a Meaningful Life. WW Norton & Company.

Kerr, C. E., Sacchet, M. D., Lazar, S. W., Moore, C. I., and Jones, S. R. (2013). Mindfulness starts with the body: somatosensory attention and top-down modulation of cortical alpha rhythms in mindfulness meditation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:12. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiken, L. G., Garland, E. L., Bluth, K., Palsson, O. S., and Gaylord, S. A. (2015). From a state to a trait: trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Pers. Individ. Diff. 81, 41–46. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kitson, A., Schiphorst, T., and Riecke, B. E. (2018). “Are you dreaming?: A phenomenological study on understanding lucid dreams as a tool for introspection in virtual reality,” in Proceedings of the 2018 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (ACM), 343. doi: 10.1145/3173574.3173917

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjaer, T. W., Bertelsen, C., Piccini, P., Brooks, D., Alving, J., and Lou, H. C. (2002). Increased dopamine tone during meditation-induced change of consciousness. Cogn. Brain Res. 13, 255–259. doi: 10.1016/S0926-6410(01)00106-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kjellgren, A., Sundequist, U., Sundholm, U., Norlander, T., and Archer, T. (2004). Altered consciousness in flotation-rest and chamber-rest: experience of experimental pain and subjective stress. Soc. Behav. Pers. 32, 103–115. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2004.32.2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koechlin, E., Basso, G., Pietrini, P., Panzer, S., and Grafman, J. (1999). The role of the anterior prefrontal cortex in human cognition. Nature 399:148. doi: 10.1038/20178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koelstra, S., Muhl, C., Soleymani, M., Lee, J.-S., Yazdani, A., Ebrahimi, T., et al. (2012). DEAP: a database for emotion analysis; using physiological signals. IEEE Trans. Affect. Comput. 3, 18–31. doi: 10.1109/T-AFFC.2011.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, H. G. (2008). Concerns about measuring “spirituality” in research. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 196, 349–355. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31816ff796

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, B. E., Coffey, K. A., Cohn, M. A., Catalino, L. I., Vacharkulksemsuk, T., Algoe, S. B., et al. (2013). How positive emotions build physical health: perceived positive social connections account for the upward spiral between positive emotions and vagal tone. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1123–1132. doi: 10.1177/0956797612470827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, B. E., and Fredrickson, B. L. (2010). Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol. Psychol. 85, 432–436. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosunen, I., Salminen, M., Jarvela, S., Ruonala, A., Ravaja, N., and Jacucci, G. (2016). “RelaWorld: neuroadaptive and immersive virtual reality meditation system,” in IUI 2016 (Sonoma, CA: ACM Press), 208–217. doi: 10.1145/2856767.2856796

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krenzer, W. (2018). Assessing the Experience of Awe: Validating the Situational Awe Scale, Doctoral. College of Science and Health Theses and Dissertations.

Google Scholar

Kuhl, D. E., and Edwards, R. Q. (1963). Image separation radioisotope scanning. Radiology 80, 653–662. doi: 10.1148/80.4.653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kvale, S., and Brinkmann, S. (2015). InterViews : Learning the Craft of Qualitative Research Interviewing/Svend Brinkmann. Los Angeles, CA: Aalborg University; Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Landgraf, R., and Neumann, I. D. (2004). Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: a dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 25, 150–176. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2004.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., and Cuthbert, B. N. (1997). International Affective Picture System (IAPS): Technical Manual and Affective Ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, 39–58.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lau, M. A., Bishop, S. R., Segal, Z. V., Buis, T., Anderson, N. D., Carlson, L., et al. (2006). The Toronto mindfulness scale: development and validation. J. Clin. Psychol. 62, 1445–1467. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laughlin, C. D. Jr., McManus, J., and d'Aquili, E. G. (1992). Brain, Symbol & Experience: Toward a Neurophenomenology of Human Consciousness. Columbia University Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Lauterbur, P. (1973). Image formation by induced local interactions: examples employing nuclear magnetic resonance. Nature 242:190. doi: 10.1038/242190a0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazar, S. W., Bush, G., Gollub, R. L., Fricchione, G. L., Khalsa, G., and Benson, H. (2000). Functional brain mapping of the relaxation response and meditation. Neuroreport 11:1581. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200005150-00042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lazarus, R. S., and Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford University Press.

Lebedev, A. V., Lövdén, M., Rosenthal, G., Feilding, A., Nutt, D. J., and Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2015). Finding the self by losing the self: Neural correlates of ego-dissolution under psilocybin. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 3137–3153. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22833

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Achermann, P., Jeanmonod, D., Gianotti, L. R. R., and Pizzagalli, D. (2001). Brain sources of EEG gamma frequency during volitionally meditation-induced, altered states of consciousness, and experience of the self. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 108, 111–121. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(01)00116-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levenson, M. R., Jennings, P. A., Aldwin, C. M., and Shiraishi, R. W. (2005). Self-transcendence: conceptualization and measurement. Int. J. Aging Hum. Dev. 60, 127–143. doi: 10.2190/XRXM-FYRA-7U0X-GRC0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., and Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review. Behav. Brain Sci. 35:121. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X11000446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lou, H. C., Kjaer, T. W., Friberg, L., Wildschiodtz, G., Holm, S., and Nowak, M. (1999). A 15o-H2o PET study of meditation and the resting state of normal consciousness. Human Brain Mapp. 7, 98–105. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1999)7:2<98::AID-HBM3>3.0.CO;2-M

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutz, A., Greischar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B., Ricard, M., and Davidson, R. J. (2004). Long-term meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental practice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 101, 16369–16373. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0407401101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, D. A., and Holland, D. (2002). Examination of the psychometric properties of the temperament and character inventory self-transcendence dimension. Pers. Individ. Diff. 32, 1013–1027. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00107-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacDonald, D. A., Tsagarakis, C. I., and Holland, C. J. (1994). Validation of a measure of transpersonal self-concept and its relationship to Jungian and five-factor model conceptions of personality. J. Transpers. Psychol. 26, 175–175.

Google Scholar

MacLean, K. A., Leoutsakos, J.-M. S., Johnson, M. W., and Griffiths, R. R. (2012). Factor analysis of the mystical experience questionnaire: a study of experiences occasioned by the Hallucinogen Psilocybin. J. Sci. Study Relig. 51, 721–737. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5906.2012.01685.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marcusson-Clavertz, D., and Cardeña, E. (2011). Hypnotizability, alterations in consciousness, and other variables as predictors of performance in a Ganzfeld PSI task. J. Parapsychol. 75:235.

Google Scholar

Marey, E. J. (1878). La Méthode Graphique dans les Sciences Experimentales et Principalement en Physiologie et en Médecine. Paris: G. Masson.

Google Scholar

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychol. Rev. 50:370. doi: 10.1037/h0054346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maslow, A. H. (1959). Cognition of being in the peak experiences. J. Genet. Psychol. 94, 43–66. doi: 10.1080/00221325.1959.10532434

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathes, E. W. (1982). Peak experience tendencies: scale development and theory testing. J. Humanist. Psychol. 22, 92–108. doi: 10.1177/0022167882223011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maturana, H. R., and Varela, F. J. (1991). Autopoiesis and Cognition: The Realization of the Living, Vol. 42. Springer Science & Business Media.

Google Scholar

May, R. W., Bamber, M., Seibert, G. S., Sanchez-Gonzalez, M. A., Leonard, J. T., Salsbury, R. A., et al. (2016). Understanding the physiology of mindfulness: aortic hemodynamics and heart rate variability. Stress 19, 168–174. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2016.1146669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCarthy, V. L., Hall, L. A., Crawford, T. N., and Connelly, J. (2018). Facilitating self-transcendence: an intervention to enhance well-being in late life. Western J. Nurs. Res. 40, 854–873. doi: 10.1177/0193945917690731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCrae, R. R., and Costa, P. T. (2003). Personality in Adulthood: A Five-Factor Theory Perspective. Guilford Press. doi: 10.4324/9780203428412

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Metzinger, T. (2020). Minimal phenomenal experience. Philos. Mind Sci. 1, 1–44. doi: 10.33735/phimisci.2020.I.46

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miller, L., Balodis, I. M., McClintock, C. H., Xu, J., Lacadie, C. M., Sinha, R., et al. (2019). Neural correlates of personalized spiritual experiences. Cereb. Cortex 29, 2331–2338. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milliére, R., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Roseman, L., Trautwein, F.-M., and Berkovich-Ohana, A. (2018). Psychedelics, meditation, and self-consciousness. Front. Psychol. 9:1475. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, K., and Iwanaga, M. (2017). Two types of peak emotional responses to music: the psychophysiology of chills and tears. Sci. Rep. 7:46063. doi: 10.1038/srep46063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moustakas, C. (1994). Phenomenological Research Methods. London: Sage. doi: 10.4135/9781412995658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newberg, A. (2014). The neuroscientific study of spiritual practices. Front. Psychol. 5:215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., Pourdehnad, M., Santanna, J., and d'Aquili, E. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: a preliminary spect study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 106, 113–122. doi: 10.1016/S0925-4927(01)00074-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Newberg, A. B., and Yaden, D. B. (2018). A neurotheological perspective on altered states of consciousness. J. Conscious. Stud. 25, 202–225.

Google Scholar

Nour, M. M., Evans, L., Nutt, D., and Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2016). Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: validation of the ego-dissolution inventory (EDI). Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:269. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R., and Hietanen, J. K. (2014). Bodily maps of emotions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 111, 646–651. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1321664111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Omodei, M. M., and McLennan, J. (1994). Studying complex decision making in natural settings: using a head-mounted video camera to study competitive orienteering. Percept. Motor Skills 79(3_Suppl), 1411–1425. doi: 10.2466/pms.1994.79.3f.1411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Palhano-Fontes, F., Andrade, K. C., Tofoli, L. F., Santos, A. C., Crippa, J. A. S., Hallak, J. E., et al. (2015). The psychedelic state induced by ayahuasca modulates the activity and connectivity of the default mode network. PLoS ONE 10:e0118143. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0118143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Panter, A. J. (2017). Flow States and Peak Experiences in the Grand Canyon: A Heuristic Exploration. The University of the Rockies.

Google Scholar

Panzarella, R. (1980). The phenomenology of aesthetic peak experiences. J. Humanist. Psychol. 20, 69–85. doi: 10.1177/002216788002000105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paterson, B., Thorne, S., Crawford, J., and Tarko, M. (1999). Living with diabetes as a transformational experience. Qual. Health Res. 9, 786–802. doi: 10.1177/104973299129122289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekala, R. J., and Levine, R. L. (1982). Quantifying states of consciousness via an empirical-phenomenological approach. Imaginat. Cogn. Pers. 2, 51–71. doi: 10.2190/2D3H-CKP8-DYNH-4KV3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petitmengin, C. (2006). Describing one's subjective experience in the second person: an interview method for the science of consciousness. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 5, 229–269. doi: 10.1007/s11097-006-9022-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petitmengin, C., Van Beek, M., Bitbol, M., Nissou, J.-M., and Roepstorff, A. (2017). What is it like to meditate?: Methods and issues for a micro-phenomenological description of meditative experience. J. Conscious. Stud. 24, 170–198.

Google Scholar

Phelps, M. E., Hoffman, E. J., Mullani, N. A., and Ter-Pogossian, M. M. (1975). Application of annihilation coincidence detection to transaxial reconstruction tomography. J. Nuclear Med.16:210.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Does spirituality represent the sixth factor of personality? Spiritual transcendence and the five-factor model. J. Pers. 67, 985–1013. doi: 10.1111/1467-6494.00080

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piff, P. K., Dietze, P., Feinberg, M., Stancato, D. M., and Keltner, D. (2015). Awe, the small self, and prosocial behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 108, 883–899. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pineda, D. (2015). Emotions, appraisals, and embodied appraisals. Crítica Revista Hispanoamericana de Filosofía 47, 3–30. doi: 10.22201/iifs.18704905e.2015.494

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piron, H. (2001). The meditation depth index (MEDI) and the meditation depth questionnaire (MEDEQ). J. Medit. Medit. Res. 1, 69–92.

Potter, R. F., and Bolls, P. D. (2012). Psychophysiological Measurement and Meaning: Cognitive and Emotional Processing of Media. New York, NY: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203181027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, J. J. (2004). Gut Reactions: A Perceptual Theory of Emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Privette, G., and Bundrick, C. M. (1987). Measurement of experience: construct and content validity of the experience questionnaire. Percept. Motor Skills 65, 315–332. doi: 10.2466/pms.1987.65.1.315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prpa, M., Cochrane, K., and Riecke, B. E. (2015). “Hacking alternatives in 21st century: designing a bio-responsive virtual environment for stress reduction,” in Pervasive Computing Paradigms for Mental Health, Communications in Computer and Information Science (Cham: Springer), 34–39. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-32270-4_4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prpa, M., Tatar, K., Francoise, J., Riecke, B., Schiphorst, T., and Pasquier, P. (2018). “Attending to breath: exploring how the cues in a virtual environment guide the attention to breath and shape the quality of experience to support mindfulness,” in Proceedings of the 2018 Designing Interactive Systems Conference, DIS '18 (New York, NY: ACM), 71–84. doi: 10.1145/3196709.3196765

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Przyrembel, M., and Singer, T. (2018). Experiencing meditation-evidence for differential effects of three contemplative mental practices in micro-phenomenological interviews. Conscious. Cogn. 62, 82–101. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quesnel, D., and Riecke, B. E. (2018). Are you awed yet? How virtual reality gives us awe and goose bumps. Front. Psychol. 9:2158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quesnel, D., Stepanova, E., Aguilar, I., Pennefather, P., and Riecke, B. E. (2018). “Creating AWE: artistic and scientific practices in research-based design for exploring a profound immersive installation,” in IEEE Games, Entertainment, & Media Techniques Conference (IEEE GEM) (Galway), 200–207. doi: 10.1109/GEM.2018.8516463

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raffone, A., and Srinivasan, N. (2010). The exploration of meditation in the neuroscience of attention and consciousness. Cogn. Proc. 11, 1–7. doi: 10.1007/s10339-009-0354-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramnani, N., and Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5:184. doi: 10.1038/nrn1343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, P. G. (2013). “Theory of self-transcendence,” in Middle Range Theory for Nursing 3. doi: 10.1891/9780826195524.0006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reed, P. G., Boyd, M., and Buckwalter, K. C. (1989). Mental health of older adults. Western J. Nurs. Res. 11, 143–163. doi: 10.1177/019394598901100202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinerman-Jones, L., Sollins, B., Gallagher, S., and Janz, B. (2013). Neurophenomenology: an integrated approach to exploring awe and wonder1. S. Afr. J. Philos. 32, 295–309. doi: 10.1080/02580136.2013.867397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riba, J., Romero, S., Grasa, E., Mena, E., Carrió, I., and Barbanoj, M. J. (2006). Increased frontal and paralimbic activation following ayahuasca, the pan-amazonian inebriant. Psychopharmacology 186, 93–98. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0358-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, L. (1962). Mysticism and philosophy. By W. T. Stace. (Macmillan & Co. Ltd. Pp. 349. Price 21s.). Philosophy 37, 179–182. doi: 10.1017/S0031819100036883

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenberg, R. S., Baughman, S. L., and Bailenson, J. N. (2013). Virtual superheroes: using superpowers in virtual reality to encourage prosocial behavior. PLoS ONE 8:e55003. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., and Aaker, J. (2012). Awe expands people's perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being. Psychol. Sci. 23, 1130–1136. doi: 10.1177/0956797612438731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanches, R. F., de Lima Osório, F., dos Santos, R. G., Macedo, L. R., Maia-de Oliveira, J. P., Wichert-Ana, L., et al. (2016). Antidepressant effects of a single dose of ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: a spect study. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 36, 77–81. doi: 10.1097/JCP.0000000000000436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saroglou, V., Buxant, C., and Tilquin, J. (2008). Positive emotions as leading to religion and spirituality. J. Positive Psychol. 3, 165–173. doi: 10.1080/17439760801998737

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauer, S., Walach, H., Schmidt, S., Hinterberger, T., Lynch, S., Büssing, A., et al. (2013). Assessment of mindfulness: review on state of the art. Mindfulness 4, 3–17. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schoenberg, P. L., and Vago, D. R. (2019). Mapping meditative states and stages with electrophysiology: concepts, classifications, and methods. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 28, 211–217. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholkmann, F., Holper, L., Preller, K. H., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2019). Effects of psilocybin on functional connectivity measured with fnirs: insights from a single-subject pilot study. Matters 5:e201911000002.

Google Scholar

Schwartz, S. H. (2012). An overview of the Schwartz theory of basic values. Online Read. Psychol. Cult. 2, 2307–2919. doi: 10.9707/2307-0919.1116

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, N., and Gedeon, T. (2012). Objective measures, sensors and computational techniques for stress recognition and classification: a survey. Comput. Methods Prog. Biomed. 108, 1287–1301. doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2012.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., and O'connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: further exploration of a prototype approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 52:1061. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Campos, B., Oveis, C., Hertenstein, M. J., Simon-Thomas, E., and Keltner, D. (2017). Beyond happiness: building a science of discrete positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 72:617. doi: 10.1037/a0040456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., and John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style. J. Positive Psychol. 1, 61–71. doi: 10.1080/17439760500510833

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., and Mossman, A. (2007). The nature of awe: elicitors, appraisals, and effects on self-concept. Cogn. Emot. 21, 944–963. doi: 10.1080/02699930600923668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiota, M. N., Neufeld, S. L., Yeung, W. H., Moser, S. E., and Perea, E. F. (2011). Feeling good: autonomic nervous system responding in five positive emotions. Emotion 11:1368. doi: 10.1037/a0024278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., et al. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychol. Bull. 144:343. doi: 10.1037/bul0000128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smallwood, J., Brown, K., Baird, B., and Schooler, J. W. (2012). Cooperation between the default mode network and the frontal-parietal network in the production of an internal train of thought. Brain Res. 1428, 60–70. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.03.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, M. J., and Liehr, P. R., editors (2014). Middle Range Theory for Nursing, 3rd Edn. New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1891/9780826195524

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sokel, W. H. (1978). Kafka's poetics of the inner self. Modern Austrian Literature 11, 37–58.

Google Scholar

Stace, W. T. (1960). Mysticism and Philosophy. New York, NY: MacMillan Press.

Google Scholar

Stellar, J. E., Gordon, A. M., Piff, P. K., Cordaro, D., Anderson, C. L., Bai, Y., et al. (2017). Self-transcendent emotions and their social functions: compassion, gratitude, and awe bind us to others through prosociality. Emot. Rev. 9, 200–207. doi: 10.1177/1754073916684557

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stepanova, E. R., Quesnel, D., and Riecke, B. E. (2019). Understanding AWE: can a virtual journey, inspired by the overview effect, lead to an increased sense of interconnectedness? Front. Digit. Humanit. 6:9. doi: 10.3389/fdigh.2019.00009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Studerus, E., Gamma, A., and Vollenweider, F. X. (2010). Psychometric evaluation of the altered states of consciousness rating scale (OAV). PLoS ONE 5:e12412. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0012412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sumpf, M., Jentschke, S., and Koelsch, S. (2015). Effects of aesthetic chills on a cardiac signature of emotionality. PLoS ONE 10:e0130117. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0130117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tagliazucchi, E., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., Orban, C., Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Murphy, K., et al. (2016). Increased global functional connectivity correlates with LSD-induced ego dissolution. Curr. Biol. 26, 1043–1050. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tanay, G., and Bernstein, A. (2013). State Mindfulness Scale (SMS): development and initial validation. Psychol. Assess. 25, 1286–1299. doi: 10.1037/a0034044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tenke, C. E., Kayser, J., Svob, C., Miller, L., Alvarenga, J. E., Abraham, K., et al. (2017). Association of posterior EEG alpha with prioritization of religion or spirituality: a replication and extension at 20-year follow-up. Biol. Psychol. 124, 79–86. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Åhs, F., Fredrikson, M., Sollers, J. J. III., and Wager, T. D. (2012). A meta-analysis of heart rate variability and neuroimaging studies: implications for heart rate variability as a marker of stress and health. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 36, 747–756. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, E. (2010). Mind in Life. Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Timmermann, C., Roseman, L., Schartner, M., Milliere, R., Williams, L., Erritzoe, D., et al. (2019). Neural correlates of the DMT experience as assessed via multivariate EEG. bioRxiv 9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Tornstam (1994) 1 203–225. doi: 10.1101/706283.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tornstam, L. (1994). Gerotranscendence-a theoretical and empirical exploration.

Travis, F., Nash, J., Parim, N., and Cohen, B. H. (2020). Does the MRI/fMRI procedure itself confound the results of meditation research? An evaluation of subjective and neurophysiological measures of tm practitioners in a simulated MRI environment. Front. Psychol. 11:728. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00728

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Underwood, L. G., and Teresi, J. A. (2002). The daily spiritual experience scale: development, theoretical description, reliability, exploratory factor analysis, and preliminary construct validity using health-related data. Ann. Behav. Med. 24, 22–33. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2401_04

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Urgesi, C., Aglioti, S. M., Skrap, M., and Fabbro, F. (2010). The spiritual brain: selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence. Neuron 65, 309–319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Uthaug, M., Van Oorsouw, K., Kuypers, K., Van Boxtel, M., Broers, N., Mason, N., et al. (2018). Sub-acute and long-term effects of ayahuasca on affect and cognitive thinking style and their association with ego dissolution. Psychopharmacology 235, 2979–2989. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4988-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vago, D. R., and Silbersweig, D. A. (2012). Self-awareness, self-regulation, and self-transcendence (S-ART): a framework for understanding the neurobiological mechanisms of mindfulness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:296. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vago, D. R., and Zeidan, F. (2016). The brain on silent: mind wandering, mindful awareness, and states of mental tranquility. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1373:96. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13171

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., Arciniegas Gomez, M. A., van der Zwaag, W., van Schie, H. T., and Sauter, D. (2019). The neural correlates of the awe experience: reduced default mode network activity during feelings of awe. Hum. Brain Mapp. 40, 3561–3574. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., Karinen, A., Specker, E., Stamkou, E., and Baas, M. (2016). “Standing in awe”: the effects of awe on body perception and the relation with absorption. Collabra Psychol. 2, 1–16. doi: 10.1525/collabra.36

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Elk, M., and Rotteveel, M. (2019). Experimentally induced awe does not affect implicit and explicit time perception. Attent. Percept. Psychophys. 82, 926–937. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01924-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Lange, P. A. M., De Bruin, E. M. N., Otten, W., and Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: theory and preliminary evidence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 73, 733–746. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.73.4.733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Rooij, M., Lobel, A., Harris, O., Smit, N., and Granic, I. (2016). “DEEP: a biofeedback virtual reality game for children at-risk for anxiety,” in Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference Extended Abstracts on Human Factors in Computing Systems, CHI EA '16 (New York, NY: ACM), 1989–1997. doi: 10.1145/2851581.2892452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F. J. (1996). Neurophenomenology: a methodological remedy for the hard problem. J. Conscious. Stud. 3, 330–349.

Google Scholar

Venter, H. J. (2017). Self-transcendence: Maslow's answer to cultural closeness. J. Innovat. Manage. 4, 3–7. doi: 10.24840/2183-0606_004.004_0002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vermersch, P. (1994). L'entretien D'explicitation, Vol. 1. ESF Sciences Humaines.

Vigouroux, R. (1879). Sur le role de la resistance electrique des tissues dans l'electro-diagnostic. Comptes Rendus Societe de Biologie 31, 336–339.

Vollenweider, F. X., Leenders, K. L., Scharfetter, C., Maguire, P., Stadelmann, O., and Angst, J. (1997). Positron emission tomography and fluorodeoxyglucose studies of metabolic hyperfrontality and psychopathology in the psilocybin model of psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacology 16, 357–372. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(96)00246-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, D. J. J., Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Wintering, N., Pluta, J., Khalsa, D. S., et al. (2011). Cerebral blood flow changes associated with different meditation practices and perceived depth of meditation. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 191, 60–67. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wassiliwizky, E., Jacobsen, T., Heinrich, J., Schneiderbauer, M., and Menninghaus, W. (2017). Tears falling on goosebumps: co-occurrence of emotional lacrimation and emotional piloerection indicates a psychophysiological climax in emotional arousal. Front. Psychol. 8:41. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., and Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 54:1063. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Watson, J. B. (2009). Is thinking merely the action of language mechanisms? Brit. J. Psychol. 100, 169–180. doi: 10.1348/000712608X336095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wielgosz, J., Schuyler, B. S., Lutz, A., and Davidson, R. J. (2016). Long-term mindfulness training is associated with reliable differences in resting respiration rate. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–6. doi: 10.1038/srep27533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T. P. (2016). “Meaning-seeking, self-transcendence, and well-being,” in Logotherapy and Existential Analysis: Proceedings of the Viktor Frankl Institute, ed A. Batthyany (Cham: Springer), 311–321. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-29424-7_27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, W., Camfield, D. A., Woods, W., Sarris, J., and Pipingas, A. (2015). Spectral power and functional connectivity changes during mindfulness meditation with eyes open: a magnetoencephalography (MEG) study in long-term meditators. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 98, 95–111. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Vago, D. R., and Newberg, A. (2017a). The varieties of self-transcendent experience. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 21, 143–160. doi: 10.1037/gpr0000102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yaden, D. B., Iwry, J., and Newberg, A. (2017b). Neuroscience and Religion: Surveying the Field. 17, 227–299.

Google Scholar

Yaden, D. B., Kaufman, S. B., Hyde, E., Chirico, A., Gaggioli, A., Zhang, J. W., et al. (2018). The development of the Awe Experience Scale (AWE-S): a multifactorial measure for a complex emotion. J. Positive Psychol. 14, 474–488. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2018.1484940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, S., Kitamura, Y., Yamada, N., Nakashima, Y., and Kuroda, S. (2006). Medial profrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex in the generation of alpha activity induced by transcendental meditation: a magnetoencephalographic study. Acta Medica Okayama 60, 51–58. doi: 10.18926/AMO/30752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, Y., Hu, J., Jing, F., Nguyen, B., Yang, Y., Hu, J., et al. (2018). From awe to ecological behavior: the mediating role of connectedness to nature. Sustainability 10:2477. doi: 10.3390/su10072477

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhao, H., Zhang, H., Xu, Y., Lu, J., and He, W. (2018). Relation between awe and environmentalism: the role of social dominance orientation. Front. Psychol. 9:2367. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02367

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zheng, Y.-L., Wang, D.-X., Zhang, Y.-R., and Tang, Y.-Y. (2019). Enhancing attention by synchronizing respiration and fingertip pressure: a pilot study using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Front. Neurosci. 13:1209. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.01209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhu, B., Hedman, A., Feng, S., Li, H., and Osika, W. (2017). Designing, prototyping and evaluating digital mindfulness applications: a case study of mindful breathing for stress reduction. J. Med. Internet Res. 19:e197. doi: 10.2196/jmir.6955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-transcendence, review, research methods, mindfulness, flow, peak experiences, neurophenomenolgy, emotional experiences

Citation: Kitson A, Chirico A, Gaggioli A and Riecke BE (2020) A Review on Research and Evaluation Methods for Investigating Self-Transcendence. Front. Psychol. 11:547687. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.547687

Received: 04 May 2020; Accepted: 30 September 2020;
Published: 16 November 2020.

Edited by:

Antonino Raffone, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Reviewed by:

Andrea Zaccaro, University of Pisa, Italy
Alessio Matiz, University of Udine, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Kitson, Chirico, Gaggioli and Riecke. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alexandra Kitson, akitson@sfu.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.