Skip to main content
Log in

Consciousness and choking in visually-guided actions

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Choking under pressure describes the phenomenon of people performing well below their expected standard under circumstances where optimal performance is crucial. One of the prevailing explanations for choking is that pressure increases the conscious attention to the underlying processes of the performer's task execution, thereby disrupting what would normally be a relatively automatic process. However, research on choking has focused mainly on the influence of pressure on motor performance, typically overlooking how it might alter the way that vision is controlled when performing these motor actions. In this article we ask whether the visual component of expert motor-skill execution is susceptible to choking much like the motor component is thought to be. To do so, we draw heavily on empirical findings from studies of sporting expertise, in particular focussing on the role of gaze in three types of visually-guided actions: interceptive actions, aiming tasks, and anticipatory skill. For each of these skills we (i) evaluate the nature of the expert advantage, (ii) discuss the role of consciousness in their control, (iii) examine the potential impact of pressure on task performance, and (iv) consider interventions designed to reduce the likelihood of choking when performing these tasks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Abernethy, B., & Russell, D. G. (1987). The relationship between expertise and visual search strategy in a racquet sport. Human Movement Science, 6, 283–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abernethy, B., Wood, J. M., & Parks, S. (1999). Can the anticipatory skills of experts be learned by novices? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70, 313–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abernethy, B., Schorer, J., Jackson, R. C., & Hagemann, N. (2012). Perceptual training methods compared: The relative efficacy of different approaches to enhancing sport-specific anticipation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18, 143–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alder, D.B., Ford, P.R., Causer, J., Williams, A.M. (2014). Perceptual training with anxiety: effects on the anticipatory judgements of elite athletes. Meeting of the Expertise and Skill Acquisition Network, Sheffield UK.

  • Anderson, J. R. (1993). Rules of mind. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 610–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behan, M., & Wilson, M. (2008). State anxiety and visual attention: The role of the QE period in aiming to a far target. Journal of Sports Sciences, 26, 207–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 701–725.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Carr, T. H., MacMahon, C., & Starkes, J. L. (2002a). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 6–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S., Wierenga, S. A., & Carr, T. H. (2002b). Expertise, attention, and memory in sensorimotor skill execution: Impact of novel task constraints on dual-task performance and episodic memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1211–1240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., Bertenthal, B. I., McCoy, A. M., & Carr, T. H. (2004). Haste does not always make waste: Expertise, direction of attention, and speed versus accuracy in performing sensorimotor skills. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11, 373–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bootsma, R. J., & van Wieringen, P. C. W. (1990). Timing an attacking forehand drive in table tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 21–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cappuccio, M. & Ilundain-Agurruza, J. (this issue). Introduction to the special issue on unreflective action and the choking effect. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.

  • Carr, T. (this issue). Strengths and weaknesses of reflection as a guide to action: Pressure assails performance in multiple ways. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.

  • Causer, J., Holmes, P. S., Smith, N. C., & Williams, A. M. (2011a). Anxiety, movement kinematics, and visual attention in elite-level performers. Emotion, 11(3), 595–602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Causer, J., Holmes, P. S., & Williams, A. M. (2011b). Quiet eye training in a visuomotor control task. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 43, 1042–1049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deikman, A. J. (1966). De-automatization and the mystic experience. Psychiatry, 29, 324–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deschamps, T., Nouritt, D., Caillou, N., & Delignières, D. (2004). Influence of a stressing constraint on stiffness and damping functions of a ski simulator’s platform motion. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 867–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dicks, M., Davids, K., & Button, C. (2010). Individual differences in the visual control of intercepting a penalty kick in association football. Human Movement Science, 29, 401–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (2002). Intelligence without representation – Merleau-Ponty’s critique of mental representation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 1, 367–383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2002). Can anticipatory skills be learned through implicit video based perceptual training? Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 471–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farrow, D., Chivers, P., Hardingham, C., & Sachse, S. (1998). The effect of video-based perceptual training on the tennis return of serve. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 23, 231–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P., & Posner, M. (1967). Human performance. Belmont: Brooks-Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, A. H. (1962). The progression–regression hypothesis in perceptual-motor skill learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 177–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottlieb, G. (this issue). Know-how, procedural knowledge, and choking under pressure. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences.

  • Gray, R. (2004). Attending to the execution of a complex sensorimotor skill: Expertise differences, choking, and slumps. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 10, 42–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hagemann, N., Strauss, B., & Cañal-Bruland, R. (2006). Training perceptual skill by orienting visual attention. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 28, 142–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, L., Mullen, R., & Jones, G. (1996). Knowledge and conscious control of motor actions under stress. British Journal of Psychology, 87, 621–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harle, S. K., & Vickers, J. N. (2001). Training quiet eye improves accuracy in the basketball free throw. Sport Psychologist, 15, 289–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, C. M., & Miles, T. R. (1978). Use of advance cues in predicting the flight of a lawn tennis ball. Journal of Human Movement Studies, 4, 231–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koedijker, J. M., Poolton, J. M., Maxwell, J. P., Oudejans, R. R., Beek, P. J., & Masters, R. S. W. (2011). Attention and time constraints in perceptual-motor learning and performance: Instruction, analogy, and skill level. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koedijker, J. M., Kredel, R., Schindler, S., & Hossner, E.-J. (2013). Effects of vibrations on gaze behavior in high-speed navigation. Proceedings of the EuroMov Inaugural Conference, Montpellier, France.

  • Land, M. F., & McLeod, P. (2000). From eye movements to actions: How batsmen hit the ball. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1340–1345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liao, C. H., & Masters, R. S. W. (2001). Analogy learning: A means to implicit motor learning. Journal of Sports Sciences, 19, 307–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, D. T. Y., Williams, A. M., Ward, P., & Janelle, C. M. (2007). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 457–478.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, D. L., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2010). The resilience of natural interceptive actions to refractive blur. Human Movement Science, 29, 386–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, D. L., Spratford, W., & Abernethy, B. (2013). The head tracks and gaze predicts: How the world’s best batters hit a ball. PloS One, 8, e58289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W., & Maxwell, J. P. (2004). Implicit motor learning, reinvestment and movement disruption. What you don’t know won’t hurt you? In A. M. Williams & N. J. Hodges (Eds.), Skill acquisition in sport: Research, theory and practice (pp. 207–228). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S., Maxwell, J. P., & Eves, F. F. (2009). Marginally perceptible outcome feedback, motor learning and implicit processes. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 639–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. S. W., Kerr, E., & Weedon, E. (2001). The implicit benefit of learning without errors. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1049–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, L., Vine, S. J., Cooke, A., Ring, C., & Wilson, M. R. (2012). Quiet eye training expedites motor learning and aids performance under pressure: The roles of response programming and external attention. Psychophysiology, 49, 1005–1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., & Oudejans, R. R. (2010). Effects of anxiety on handgun shooting behavior of police officers: A pilot study. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 23, 225–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nieuwenhuys, A., Pijpers, J. R., Oudejans, R. R., & Bakker, F. C. (2008). The influence of anxiety on visual attention in climbing. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 30, 171–185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryu, D., Kim, S., Abernethy, B., & Mann, D. L. (2013). Guiding attention aids the acquisition of anticipatory skill in novice soccer goalkeepers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 84, 252–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sailer, U., Flanagan, J. R., & Johansson, R. S. (2005). Eye–hand coordination during learning of a novel visuomotor task. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 8833–8842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh, G. J. P., Williams, A. M., Van der Kamp, J., & Ward, P. (2002). Visual search, anticipation and expertise in soccer goalkeepers. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 279–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh, G. J. P., van der Kamp, J., Williams, A. M., & Ward, P. (2005). Anticipation and visual search behaviour in expert soccer goalkeepers. Ergonomics, 48, 1686–1697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savelsbergh, G. J. P., van Gastel, P. J., & van Kampen, P. M. (2010). Anticipation of penalty kicking direction can be improved by directing attention through perceptual learning. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 41, 24–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, J. (2014). Conscious control over action. Mind and Language.

  • Smeeton, N. J., Williams, A. M., Hodges, N. J., & Ward, P. (2005). The relative effectiveness of various instructional approaches in developing anticipation skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 11, 98–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vickers, J. N. (1996). Visual control when aiming at a far target. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 342–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vine, S. J., & Wilson, M. R. (2010). Quiet eye training: Effects on learning and performance under pressure. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 22, 361–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vine, S. J., & Wilson, M. R. (2011). The influence of quiet eye training and pressure on attention and visuomotor control. Acta Psychologica, 136, 340–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vine, S. J., Moore, L. J., & Wilson, M. R. (2011). Quiet eye training facilitates competitive putting performance in elite golfers. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vine, S. J., Moore, L. J., Cooke, A., Ring, C., & Wilson, M. R. (2013). Quiet eye training: A means to implicit motor learning. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 44(4), 367–386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A case study and implications. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M., Ward, P., Knowles, J. M., & Smeeton, N. J. (2002). Anticipation skill in a real-world task: Measurement, training, and transfer in tennis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 259–270.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R., Vine, S. J., & Wood, G. (2009a). The influence of anxiety on visual attentional control in basketball free throw shooting. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 152–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R., Wood, G., & Vine, S. J. (2009b). Anxiety, attentional control, and performance impairment in penalty kicks. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 31, 761–775.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. R., Vine, S. J., Bright, E., Masters, R. S., Defriend, D., & McGrath, J. S. (2011). Gaze training enhances laparoscopic technical skill acquisition and multi-tasking performance: A randomized, controlled study. Surgical Endoscopy, 25, 3731–3739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolpert, D. M., Diedrichsen, J., & Flanagan, J. R. (2011). Principles of sensorimotor learning. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12, 739–751.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Figure 1 presents results from a Masters project performed by Rick Dumoulin and Michiel Vervoort at VU University Amsterdam with the support of Vosse de Boode and Ajax Football Club.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to David L. Mann.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Koedijker, J.M., Mann, D.L. Consciousness and choking in visually-guided actions. Phenom Cogn Sci 14, 333–348 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9398-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-014-9398-3

Keywords

Navigation