Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 03 December 2020
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Beyond the Words: Comparing Interpersonal Engagement Between Maternal and Paternal Infant-Directed Speech Acts

  • 1Laboratory of Applied Psychology, Department of Psychology, University of Crete, Rethymnon, Greece
  • 2Department of Statistics, Athens University of Economics and Business, Athens, Greece

The present study investigates the way infants express their emotions in relation to parental feelings between maternal and paternal questions and direct requests. We therefore compared interpersonal engagement accompanying parental questions and direct requests between infant–mother and infant–father interactions. We video-recorded spontaneous communication between 11 infant–mother and 11 infant–father dyads—from the 2nd to the 6th month—in their home. The main results of this study are summarized as follows: (a) there are similarities in the way preverbal infants use their affections in spontaneous interactions with their mothers and fathers to express signs of sensitivity in sharing knowledge through questions and direct requests; and (b) the developmental trajectories of face-to-face emotional coordination in the course of parental questions descend in a similar way for both parents across the age range of this study. Regarding the developmental trajectories of emotional non-coordination, there is evidence of a linear trend in terms of age difference between the parents’ gender with fathers showing the steeper slope. The results are discussed in relation to the theory of intersubjectivity.

Introduction

Young infants are born with motives that lead them to communicate their feelings with caregivers in order to have their needs met (Trevarthen, 1993a,b, 2011, 2012, 2017). Before language, an infant is considered a person who can participate with an affectionate partner through movements that express feelings for the purpose of negotiation and sharing of meaning (Bullowa, 1979; Reissland, 2012). Young infants (before the 6th month) ‘read’ the intentions inherent in their parents’ actions (Trevarthen et al., 2006). Parents, both mothers and fathers, are sensitive to the infant’s capacities for emotional regulation, coordination, and communication of feelings. In this way, parents build a unique relationship of intimacy with their infant (Trevarthen et al., 2006).

Evidence on young infants’ sensitivity to sentence-type discrimination comes mainly from the field of speech analysis. Within this field, research on infant’s sensitivity to sentence-type distinction through observations of spontaneous infant–parent interactions is rare and even more seldom when comparing mothers and fathers.

Infant Speech Perception Development: Why Is Infant Sensitivity in Discrimination of Speech Acts Still an Open Matter?

The foundation for speech perception and language acquisition is laid before birth. In the last months of pregnancy, fetuses are capable of detecting, recognizing, responding to, and remembering some characteristics of the maternal voice (Kisilevsky et al., 2003; Voegtline et al., 2013). Prenatal exposure to maternal voice and heartbeat sounds may constitute the neural pathways for auditory entrainment and synchrony skills necessary for interpersonal communication (Kisilevsky et al., 2003). In addition, fetuses encode some suprasegmental characteristics of speech (intonation and rhythm) into memory (DeCasper and Spence, 1986). When fetal responses to the recorded maternal and paternal voices were compared, contrasting evidence showed the following: (a) that an increase in the heart was observed during exposure to the mother’s but not father’s voice (following the offset of the father’s voice, fetuses responded with a brief heart rate increase) (Kisilevsky et al., 2009; Lee and Kisilevsky, 2013); and (b) fetuses responded in a similar manner to both maternal and paternal voices before birth. After birth, neonates showed a preference for the mother’s voice and no preference for the father’s voice (Lee and Kisilevsky, 2013).

Regarding infant speech discrimination, there is contradictory evidence on young infants’ ability to discriminate consonant contrasts (Eimas et al., 1971; Eimas, 1974; Lasky et al., 1975; Eimas and Miller, 1980) and vowels (Trehub, 1973; Swoboda et al., 1976; Kuhl, 1983). Recent evidence may favor the proposition that infants come pre-wired with auditory processing skills that are modified selectively by experience in the language-learning environment (Houston, 2016). Findings on word segmentation suggest that well before infants are able to produce words, by 8 months of age, they are able to segment words from fluent speech (Jusczyk and Aslin, 1995). Young infants’ sensitivity to prosodic patterns of their native language (as early as 2 months) constitutes one of the potential sources of information that may facilitate infant word segmentation ability (for a full discussion see Houston, 2005).

In connection with the evidence on fetal suprasegmental information encoding, there is evidence that, after birth, 4-month-old infants’ greater attention to infant-directed speech compared to adult-directed speech has been attributed to positive affect conveyed. Positive affect of infant-directed speech is carried through certain aspects of pitch, particularly through intonation (Fernald and Kuhl, 1987; Fernald, 1989, 1993).

Linguistically focused analyses for the examination of infant discrimination of sentence types start as early as 5 months, and it has been mainly based on visual habituation paradigm. There is evidence that infants are specifically able to discriminate the intonational characteristics of questions from those of statements and exclamations and prefer to listen to questions over statements (Best et al., 1991; Geffen and Mintz, 2011; Soderstrom et al., 2011; Frota et al., 2014). In the meantime, certain methodological issues of these studies imply that young infants’ sentence-type distinctions, according to a straightforward relationship with prosody, remains an open matter (Van de Weijer, 2002; Geffen and Mintz, 2017; Reimchen and Soderstrom, 2017). What is more, discrimination of, and preference for, the intonational forms of questions, does not show whether infants treat questions as having a different communicative function from declaratives. On this ground, taking into account the infants’ active participation in ‘dialogues’ with their mothers may constitute a new perspective in the research on infant sentence-type discrimination (Reimchen and Soderstrom, 2017).

Previous Studies Comparing Emotional Engagement and Speech Acts Between Early Mother–Infant and Father–Infant Playful Interactions

Several studies have compared emotional engagement in playful interactions of infants with their mother and father in the first semester of life. Some researchers did not find significant differences in parental attempts to direct infant’s attention, sensitivity and synchrony (Landerholm and Scriven, 1981; Braungart-Rieker et al., 1998; Feldman, 2003; Branger et al., 2019). O thers described variations between mothers and fathers in playful behavior, responsiveness, emotional matching, and attunement (Yogman, 1981; Belsky et al., 1984; Forbes et al., 2004; Kokkinaki and Vasdekis, 2015).

A limited number of studies have shown variations in emotional expressions in interactions of infants with their mothers and fathers. Infants respond more positively to mothers than to fathers. Mothers express more positive but also more negative affect than fathers (Forbes et al., 2004; Jia, 2014). In the meanwhile, compared to mothers, fathers express more interest and less neutral affect toward their infant (Jia, 2014; Cerezo et al., 2017).

Speech acts addressed by parents to young infants are components of parent–infant communication. Studies focus mainly on the verbal design of this communication, such as frequency and types of questions. Parental infant-directed questions did not differ significantly between mothers and fathers while mothers made more requests than fathers (Kruper and Uzgiris, 1987; Laflamme et al., 2002; Kokkinaki, 2018).

However, the conclusion is based on different family studies varying in their methodology. They lack investigating the early developmental path through which preverbal young infants show sensitivity to the intentions inherent in parents’ different sentence-types. Further, they fail to take into account the inter-sensory nature of communication, i.e., both speech acts and facial expressions of emotion.

Hypotheses and Importance of This Study

Based on the theory of intersubjectivity (Trevarthen et al., 2006), both parents, the mother and the father, are sensitive communicative partners to the infants and they build a unique relationship of intimacy with them. Young infants ‘read’ the intentions inherent in their parents’ actions by coordinating their expressions and forming the basis of embodied intersubjectivity (Trevarthen et al., 2006). In the first semester of life, there are age-related transformations in the way infants regulate their feelings and express their interests to their companions (Trevarthen and Aitken, 2003). In the same period, relevant studies comparing emotional engagement in playful interactions of infants with their mother and father did not find significant differences in parental sensitivity and synchrony, but they showed variations in emotional matching and attunement (Braungart-Rieker et al., 1998; Feldman, 2003; Kokkinaki and Vasdekis, 2015; Branger et al., 2019).

Based on this, we addressed the following hypotheses.

Hypothesis 1: When mothers and fathers ask questions (Hypothesis 1a) and make directive signals (Hypothesis 1b) to their infants, we expect that infants will be similar in the way they will synchronize the shifts of their emotional states between mothers and fathers (synchrony). Further, we expect that mothers and fathers will be similar in the way they will respond with sensitivity to infant negative and neutral emotional expressions. In the meanwhile, we anticipate variations in the way infants will match pleasure and interest facial expressions to the same states of mothers and fathers (matching), and they will complete their mothers’ and fathers’ emotional expression of positive valence (pleasure or interest) (completion). In addition, we expect that infants’ emotional intensity will be differentially affected by the corresponding expression of emotional intensity by the mother and the father (attunement).

Hypothesis 2: Infant age will significantly affect, and in a way that is similar between the two dyads, the interpersonal engagement (coordination/non-coordination) taking place during maternal and paternal questions.

This study is important because we know very little about how young infants (before the 6th month) ‘read’ the intentions inherent in their parents’ actions (Trevarthen et al., 2006). Furthermore, the vocal and verbal experiences to which infants are exposed cannot be not separately considered from facial expressions accompanying them (Jaffe et al., 1973). Within this context, this study may extend our understanding of the intersubjective function of parental infant-directed questions and direct requests (Sylvester-Bradley and Trevarthen, 1978; Trevarthen, 1997; Reddy, 2015; Kokkinaki, 2017). In the meanwhile, male and female speakers may differ in the way they convey the same affective messages to their infants (Slaney and McRoberts, 2003). The ‘emotion lessons,’ combining facial/vocal expressions, available during mother–child and father–child interactions may have significant effects on infants’ developing understanding of others’ emotion expressions (Montague and Walker-Andrews, 2002).

Materials and Methods

Participants

Participants were part of a cross-cultural and longitudinal study which investigated emotional expressions during the course of imitations in dyadic interactions of infants with their mothers and fathers coming from Greek and Scottish families (N = 90 subjects) (see Note). Ethical approval of the project was given by the Lothian Research Ethics Committee and the Psychiatry/Clinical Psychology Research Ethics Sub-Committee of the Royal Infirmary of Edinburgh (NHS Trust).

For the current investigation, 11 mother–infant pairs and 11 father–infant pairs from 11 families were taken randomly from the Greek sample (N = 33). All parents were native Greek speakers. Infants with complications at birth, or with known abnormality of development, as well as single parent families and those with children from previous relationships and with health or drug problems were excluded from the sample of this study. Families did not receive compensation for participating in the study. Table 1 presents demographic information for the sample of this study.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic information for the sample.

Procedure

After parents had chosen to participate in the study (see Kokkinaki, 1998 for details on subject recruitment), an introductory discussion with the researcher took place at their home. Parents were informed about the procedure of the study and asked to sign the consent form prior to video recording. Visits at infants’ home and video recordings were scheduled at a time when the infants were reported by their mothers to be alert and playful and when both the mother and the father were at home. According to video recording schedule, the researcher visited infants’ home and video recorded dyadic interactions of infants with their mother and father at 15-day intervals. Video recordings started at the end of the Primary Intersubjectivity Period (2 months) and continued until the end of Period of Games I (6 months) (Trevarthen, 1984).

During each visit, the order of video recordings of infants interacting with their parents was counterbalanced. In the first visit, video recordings always began with the parent of the same sex as the infant and the order switched in each successive visit. The instruction given to the parents was “Play as you normally do with your baby.”

Given that infant’s state and age constitute factors that appear to influence arousal, attention and affect (Field, 1981), we varied the duration of video-recordings across the age range of this study. Each video-recording lasted 8 min for the younger infants, aged 2–4 months, and 10 min for the older infants, aged 4.5–6 months.

A total of 198 recordings were made for the whole sample. The total duration of micro-analyzed interactions was 1760 min, equally distributed between interactions with mother and father and without any data loss (see Kokkinaki et al., 2016, for further information on the recording conditions).

Coding

Plan of Micro-Analysis and Coding

Micro-analysis of spontaneous infant and parent facial expressions of emotion during parental questions and direct requests was carried out according to the following procedure:

(1) maternal and paternal infant-directed speech and infant vocalizations/non-speech sounds were transcribed from the infant–parent video recordings by the researcher, and transcripts were then checked for accuracy by an assistant;

(2) transcripts of parental speech addressed to the infants were classified into ‘content categories,’ ‘thematic sequences,’ into ‘focus categories’;

(3) each focus category and the thematic sequences were grouped into units and subunits of analysis, and this was done on the basis of the pause duration preceding and following each thematic sequence;

(4) within each subunit of analysis, each focus category and within thematic sequences we coded for (a) speech acts and (b) infant and maternal/paternal facial expressions of emotion, which were then grouped into categories of interpersonal engagement (see Kokkinaki, 2013, 2018; Kokkinaki and Vasdekis, 2015; Kokkinaki et al., 2016 for details of the plan for microanalysis and coding).

Content Categories

The content of parental infant-directed speech was assessed according to the following categories: thematic sequences, non-speech sounds, vocal expressions, vocal and verbal games, and songs. A thematic sequence was defined as a block of utterances, that is, units of spoken language marked off on either side by a pause, with a high degree of semantic coherence (Kerbrat-Orecchioni, 1990; Siegel, 1963; Rabain-Jamin and Sabeau-Jouannet, 1997; Rondal, 1980; for definitions of the rest content categories see Kokkinaki, 2013). In this study, speech acts were coded according to the occurrence of questions, or directive categories, in utterances comprising the thematic sequence(s).

Focus Categories and Thematic Sequences

Table 2 presents the categorization of parental infant-directed speech according to focus categories and thematic sequences.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Description of focus categories with thematic sequences of parental infant-directed speech.

Segmentation Procedure

Each of the above focuses and thematic sequence categories of parental infant-directed speech were measured to an accuracy of 1/25th of a second and segmented into units and sub-units of analysis. In this study, a sub-unit was defined as the temporal period that began at the start of one thematic sequence, or an infant vocalization to which the parent responded to, and ended at the termination of parental response to an infant cue, or at the completion of the thematic sequence. The unit of analysis consisted of one or more subunits depending on their between-pause duration. If the duration of pause between two successive subunits was shorter than or equal to 2 s, these were grouped within the same unit. This means that units of analysis are separated by intervals longer than 2 s. According to Herrera et al. (2004), the 2 s pause is adequate for the change of content of parental utterances.

Coding of Speech Acts

Questions

A question was defined as an utterance ending in a question mark seeking information, or confirmation (Snow, 1977; Elias et al., 1988; Pancsofar and Vernon-Feagans, 2006). Questions were grouped as follows: (1) open-ended questions, (2) close-ended questions, (3) two alternative questions, and (4) questions, including answers (for detailed definitions with examples see Kokkinaki, 2013).

Directives

Direct requests were imperatives which require the addressee to bring about a state of affairs (Levinson, 1984; Stephany and Voelkova, 2015). Coding of direct requests included: (1) direct commands, that is, utterances that controlled the physical behavior of the child by commanding him/her to do or desist from doing something; (2) attentional directives that sought to attract, direct, or redirect the child’s attention (Reissland, 1998); and (3) prompting a certain infant emotional expression.

Coding of Infant and Parental Facial Expressions of Emotion

Detailed analysis of timing of the types and the intensity of facial expressions of emotion was continuous in the course of each identified sub-unit of analysis and it carried out to an accuracy of 1/25th of a second. For the aim of this study, micro-analysis of infant and parental emotional expressions and intensity corresponded only to those subunits of analysis which included thematic sequence(s) of parental speech in which utterances were expressed in questions and directives. Micro-analysis of emotional expressions and intensity was carried out by the first researcher (first observer).

Types of Facial Expressions of Emotion

Within each identified sub-unit, the coding scheme predicted the following types of facial expressions: (1) happy, that is (a) pleasure expressed to the partner or (b) pleasure expressed to an external object/environment; (2) interest, which is (a) expressed to the partner or (b) expressed to an object/environment; (3) neutral expressions; and (4) sad or withdrawn expressions [see Kokkinaki (1998, 2015) for the theoretical framework].

Intensity of Facial Expressions of Emotion

In each identified sub-unit of analysis, the onset time in the emotional expression of one partner coincided with the offset time of the previous emotion of the same partner. When the parent addressed thematic sequences to the infant in which questions and directives were identified, it was most likely that a parent and infant would each express more than one type of facial expression of emotion. In order to obtain a full description of the change of intensity of emotional engagement over time, the valence of facial expression within each category of emotion was recorded by a symbol (Table 3). The sequential order of these symbols for each partner in the course of each sub-unit of analysis determined three categories for the direction of emotional intensity change: ascending, descending, and fluctuating.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Definitions of the qualities of emotional valence and the categories of direction of emotional intensity change.

Interpersonal Engagement Categories

In this study, emotional coordination was assessed with three categories: (a) in cases of matching, one partner (infant/parent) expressed the type of facial expression of emotion of the other partner; (b) in instances of completion, one partner expressed a positive valence of facial expression of emotion, that is, ‘pleasure’ or ‘interest,’ in immediate response to the other partner; (c) in synchrony, the two partners matched the timing of change of emotional expressions with each other; and (d) in attunement, one partner expressed the shifts in the direction of emotional intensity of the other partner. In cases of emotional non-coordination, the two partners did not match the type of facial expression of emotion, did not synchronize their shifts of emotions, or one partner did not attune to the direction of emotional intensity of the other partner (Kokkinaki, 2015) (see Diagram 1 for an example of micro-analysis of spontaneous infant/mother facial expressions of emotion in the course of maternal questions).

DIAGRAM 1
www.frontiersin.org

Diagram 1. An example of micro analysis of facical expressions of emotions in the course of maternal questions in a spontaneous interaction of a 2-month-old infant with his mother.

Inter-observer reliability

With the aim of assessing inter-observer reliability, a second observer who had been trained in the application of the coding system but was not aware of the hypotheses under investigation scored a random sample of 33% of the video files. Inter-observer reliability assessments were calculated between the coding of the first observer (researcher) and the coding of the second observer. Inter-observer reliability was carried out separately for the occurrence of questions, direct requests, the type of facial expressions, the frequency of shifts of emotion, and the categories of emotional intensity. Inter-observer reliability ranged totally from 0.75 to 0.87, with the mean value of k (Cohen’s kappa) being 0.80.

Statistical Analysis

Simple comparisons of proportions between parents’ genders were accomplished using the Fisher’s exact test. Loglinear Modeling (Christensen, 1998) was used to determine relationships between variables and test hypotheses presented in the previous section. Each loglinear model was fit as a Poisson model with log-link exploring associations between triplets of variables. This was accomplished by initially fitting, for each variable triplet, the saturated model including all main effects, all two-way interactions and the three-way interaction. Then, backward selection using the likelihood ratio test (LRT) determined the optimal model. Goodness-of-fit of the finally chosen model was confirmed with a non-significant LRT test against the saturated one. The optimal model contained only statistically significant interactions. These are shown in Table 4. The significance level for goodness-of-fit tests was set at 5%. Depending on the interactions included into the final model, we quantified the association between each triplet of variable presenting appropriate proportions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Summary table of Model Selection Analysis; LRT tests statistical equation with Saturated Model.

As regards the effects of age, we restricted analysis for the relationship between infant age and infant–parent emotional matching and non-coordination only during questions because questions were the predominant speech act in both infant–mother [questions: 1530 (62.5%), direct requests: 916 (37.5%)] and infant–father interactions [questions: 1608 (84.5%), direct requests: 295 (15.5%)]. To take into account the different number of subunits and emotional matchings of pleasure/interest for each dyad and at each age point, we calculated a composite index. This index was defined as the division of the total number of emotional matchings of pleasure/interest in the course of maternal and paternal questions for each age specific point to the total number of emotional matchings for all the subunits (with the respective parent) for the same age point. The range of this index was expressed as a number between 0 and 1. For example, an index of 0.70 (7/10) at 2 months, corresponding to mother–infant interaction of subject n10, means that from the total number of 10 cases of emotional matchings that occurred in the course of maternal infant-directed speech of this dyad at 2 months, and seven of them were expressed during maternal infant-directed questions at the same age level (2 months). The same index was calculated for the case of emotional non-coordination in which the parent expressed positive emotion (pleasure, interest) and the infant expressed either interest to the external world or he/she was in a negative emotional state. Instances in which the parent was in a negative emotional state were not included in this analysis due to low frequencies. Then, repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVAs) with Huynh–Feldt correction was conducted to see if there was a significant interaction effect of parent and infant age or an effect of age or parents’ gender. In case of a significant effect, multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction provided the statistically significant differences. Polynomial contrasts on age or on the interaction of age and parents’ gender were taken into account to suggest trends of the composite index on age for possible further analysis. The significance level was set at 5%. All analyses were performed using SPSS statistical package (Version 25, 2017).

Results

Amount and Types of Parental Infant-Directed Questions in Spontaneous Infant–Mother and Infant–Father Interactions

We showed no difference in the amount mothers (35.9%) and fathers (36.7%) addressed questions to their infants (p = 0.460). According to the types of question, we found no variations in the amount mothers (29.4%) and fathers (28.8%) addressed open-ended questions (p = 0.523), close-ended questions (mothers: 13.5%, fathers: 14.7%) (p = 0.101), or alternative questions (mothers: 0.3%, fathers: 0.5%) (p = 0.191) to their infants. In the meanwhile, mothers addressed questions with answers (3.3%) to their infants more often compared to fathers (1.4%) (p < 0.001). Further, mothers and fathers differed in the number of direct requests they addressed to their infants. In connection to this, maternal infant-directed speech included direct requests (21.5%) more often than paternal speech to infants (6.7%) (p < 0.001).

Emotional Engagement During Parental Questions (Hypothesis 1a)

Relationship Between Infant and Parental Shifts of Emotional Expressions

We showed that knowing how many times parents shifted their emotions during questions, infants changed their emotional expressions in the same way in interactions with mothers and fathers (Table 4, row 1). In particular, when parents changed their emotions once, infants synchronized the shift of their emotional expressions by changing once (44.7%) more frequently than twice (36.6%), or three times (18.7%). In cases in which parents changed their emotions twice, infants shifted similarly their emotional expressions twice (42.6%) more frequently than once (26.5%), or three times (30.9%). When parents shifted their emotions three times, it was more likely for infants to change their expressions twice (45.0%) than once (20.0%) or three times (35.0%). Further, paternal infant-directed questions are more often accompanied by one shift in emotions (52.6%) compared to maternal questions (44.9%).

Relationship Between Infant and Parental Emotional Expressions

Analysis indicates a different pattern in the way infants coordinated the type of their positive emotional expressions with mothers and fathers in the course of parental questions (row 2). When mothers expressed interest to their infant, infants expressed interest (95.5%) slightly more often than when mothers expressed pleasure to their infants (91.8%). This indicates that in the course of maternal infant-directed questions, matching of interest expressions occurs slightly more often—or to the same extent—than emotional completion (with a 3.7% variation). In cases in which fathers expressed interest in their infant, infants expressed interest (96.3%) more often than when fathers expressed pleasure to their infants (86.3%). This indicates that in the course of paternal infant-directed questions, matching of interest expressions occurs slightly more often than emotional completion.

Further analysis indicates that parents coordinated their emotional expressions of pleasure and interest in the same way in cases in which infants were in a negative or a neutral emotional state (row 3). In particular, when infants were in a negative emotional state, parents more often expressed interest in the infant (76%) than pleasure (24%) (with a 52% prevalence). In cases in which infants were interested in the external world, parents were more often pleased to their infants than interested in them (53.1% vs. 46.9% with a 6.2% prevalence). Taken these together, parental expressions of interest and pleasure differed according to infant negative and neutral emotions and variations between parental pleasure and interest were larger in cases of infant negative expressions than in cases of infant neutral state.

Further, in the course of parental questions, infant negative emotional expressions were more often in interactions with mothers (24.8%) than with fathers (15.4%) while the reverse was observed for infant neutral expressions which were more often in interactions with fathers (40.7%) than with mothers (31.2%).

Relationship Between Infant and Parental Direction of Emotional Intensity

We confirmed that the way infants coordinated the direction of emotional intensity is the same in interactions with mothers and fathers in the course of parental questions (row 4). In particular, when parental emotional expressions were ascending in intensity, infant emotional expressions were equally ascending (40.7%) and fluctuating (40.7%) than descending (18.5%). When parental expressions changed in a descending way, infants’ emotions changed more frequently in a fluctuating (39.6%) than an ascending way (21.7%), but they changed almost in the same probability in a descending way (38.7%).

Emotional Engagement in the Course of Parental Direct Requests (Hypothesis 1b)

Relationship Between Infant and Parental Shifts in the Frequency of Emotional Expressions

We showed that when parents addressed direct signals to their infants, there is no relationship between parental and infant shifts of emotional expressions in interactions with mothers and fathers. Further, paternal directive requests are more often accompanied by one shift in emotions (59.6%) compared to maternal directive signals (42.3%) (row 5).

Relationship Between Infant and Parental Emotional Expressions

In the course of parental direct requests, we showed that knowing which type of positive emotion infants expressed, parental facial expressions of emotion changed in the same way in interactions with their mothers and fathers (row 6). In particular, when mothers and fathers expressed pleasure to their infant, infants were more likely to express interest (91.4%) than pleasure (8.6%). This indicates that emotional completion with the mother and the father is more frequent than matching of pleasure expressions. In cases in which mothers and fathers expressed interest in their infant, infants were more likely to share the same emotion (interest) (96%) than complete it with an emotion of similar valence (pleasure) (4.0%). This results in more matching of interest expressions than emotional completion with both fathers and mothers. Furthermore, in the course of parental direct requests, infants expressed interest to the father (97.2%) slightly more often than to the mother (93.3%).

Further analysis indicates that parents coordinated their emotional expressions of pleasure and interest in the same way in cases in which infants were in a negative or a neutral emotional state (row 7). In particular, when infants were in a negative emotional state, parents expressed more often interest in the infant (80.9%) than pleasure (19.1%, a 61.8% prevalence). In cases in which infants were interested in the external world, parents were more often pleased to their infants than interested in them (56.6% vs. 43.4%, a 13.2% prevalence). Taken these together, parental expressions of interest and pleasure differed according to infant negative and neutral emotions and variations between parental pleasure and interest were larger in cases of infant negative expressions than in cases of infant neutral state.

Further, in the course of parental direct requests, infant negative emotional expressions were more often in interactions with mothers (84.0%) than with fathers (16%).

Relationship Between Infant and Parental Direction of Emotional Intensity

We showed that the relationship between infant and parental shifts in the direction of their emotional intensity does not differ between interactions of infants with their mothers and fathers (row 8). In particular, when parents shifted their emotional expressions in an ascending way, infants’ expressions were more likely to be fluctuating (40.4%) than descending (21.1%), but they changed almost in the same probability in a ascending way (38.6%). In cases in which parental emotional expressions changed in a descending way, infants’ emotional expressions changed more often in a fluctuating (49.5%) than an ascending intensity (19.6%).

Relationship Between Infant Age, Parental Gender, and Infant–Parent Emotional Matching/Non-coordination in the Course of Questions (Hypothesis 2)

Repeated measures analysis of variance showed a significant infant age effect on emotional matchings in the course of questions, F(6.61,132.17) = 7.044, p < 0.001, the same for both parents, F(6.61,132.17) = 0.816, p = 0.583. In particular, we noticed (Figure 1) that the developmental trajectory of face-to-face emotional matchings between both mother– and father–infant interactions is descending almost linearly across the age range of this study forming a plateau at the largest age values. This was confirmed by inspection of the polynomial age contrasts which indicated a significant linear effect, F(1.20) = 27.667, p < 0.001, and quadratic effect, F(1.20) = 5.145, p = 0.047. As regards comparisons between ages, emotional matchings varied significantly between 2 and 5 months (mean difference: 0.224, standard error: 0.051) (p = 0.049), 2 and 5.5 months (mean difference: 0.268, standard error: 0.056) (p = 0.028), 2 and 6 months (mean difference: 0.272, standard error: 0.050) (p = 0.010), 2 and 4 months (mean difference: 0.214, standard error: 0.051) (p = 0.017), and 2.5 and 6 months (mean difference: 0.207, standard error: 0.054) (p = 0.036) (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Developmental curves of pleasure and interest emotional matching in the course of parental infant-directed questions across the age range of this study (2nd to 6th month of infant’s life).

Further, analysis showed non-significant effects of (infant age)(parent gender), F(5.45,108.98) = 0.924, p = 0.477, parent gender, F(1.00,20.00) = 3.547, p = 0.089, and infant age, F(5.45,108.98) = 0.764, p = 0.588, on the developmental trajectory of emotional non-coordination in the course of parental infant-directed questions (Figure 2). Despite the non-significant effects of infant age, parent gender and (infant age)(parent gender) interaction on the developmental trajectory of emotional non-coordination, inspection of the polynomial age contrasts indicated of a negative linear trend on age different between parents’ gender, F(1.20) = 5.785, p = 0.037, with fathers showing the steepest slope in terms of age.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Developmental curves of emotional non-coordination in the course of parental infant-directed questions across the age range of this study (2nd to 6th month of infant’s life).

Discussion

We aimed to compare the way infants unfolded their facial expressions of emotion in relation to parental expressions of feelings between maternal and paternal questions and direct requests.

Summary of Main Results

We confirmed (Hypothesis 1a) to a certain extent. As is consistent with our expectations, when mothers and fathers addressed questions to their infants, we found similarities in the way infants synchronized the shifts of their emotional expressions. In addition, we showed slight variations between the two dyads only for the type of positive facial expressions. In cases in which infants were in a negative emotional state, both parents responded with sensitivity by expressing interest in the infant more often than pleasure. In contrast to our expectations, we showed similarities in the way infants attuned the intensity of their emotional expressions to their parents, and we found similar instances of emotional non-coordination when the infant did not attune to the direction of emotional intensity of the parent.

Similarly, we confirmed (Hypothesis 1b) to a certain extent. In contrast to our expectations, when mothers and fathers addressed direct requests to their infants, we showed similarities in the way infants unfolded their facial expressions of emotion to match and complete those of their mothers and fathers. In accordance with our expectations, in cases in which infants were in a negative emotional state, both parents responded with sensitivity by expressing more often interest in the infant than pleasure. In contrast to our expectations, when parents addressed direct signals to their infants, we provided evidence of similar instances of emotional non-coordination when (a) infants did not synchronize the shifts of their emotions to their parents and (b) when the infant did not attune to the direction of emotional intensity of the parent.

We confirmed (Hypothesis 2) to a certain extent. We showed that infant age affected significantly, and in a similar way between the two dyads, the developmental trajectories of emotional matching but not the developmental course of emotional non-coordination during maternal and paternal infant-directed questions. Regarding the development of infant–parent emotional non-coordination, we indicated a linear trend on age difference between parents’ gender with fathers showing a steepest slope on age.

Similarities in the amount of maternal and paternal infant-directed questions and the predominance of maternal over paternal direct requests to infants is in accordance with relevant studies (Kruper and Uzgiris, 1987; Laflamme et al., 2002; Kokkinaki, 2018). In addition, similarities in the way infants synchronize, match, complete and attune their emotional expressions with their mothers and fathers are in partial consistency with findings of relevant studies (Braungart-Rieker et al., 1998; Feldman, 2003; Jia, 2014; Kokkinaki and Vasdekis, 2015; Cerezo et al., 2017). The significant effect of infant age on emotional matchings in the course of questions, which is the same for both parents, is in partial consistency with relevant findings (Kokkinaki and Vasdekis, 2015).

Discussion and Interpretation of Main Results

Emotional Coordination Between Infant–Mother and Infant–Father Interactions in the Course of Parental Questions (Hypothesis 1a)

We showed similarities in the way infants coordinated their emotional expressions in synchrony, matching, and attunement with mothers and fathers in the course of parental questions. In infant–caretaker intersubjective interactions, motives manifested in emotional expressions reveal coordination in three essential dimensions of communication: ‘kinematics’ (changes of expressive behaviors in time), ‘physiognomics’ (transformations in the form of expressive organs), and ‘energetics’ (variations in the intensity of expressions) (Trevarthen, 2012, 2017). On this ground, the results of this study confirm previous evidence that both infants and parents (mothers and fathers) are sensitive to the time, form, and intensity of their partner’s emotional expression (Trevarthen, 1993b; Kokkinaki, 2009; Kokkinaki et al., 2016).

Furthermore, this study showed similarities in the patterns of interruptions of cooperative dialogue in the course of both maternal and paternal questions. These were expressed by cases of emotional non-coordination when the infant did not attune to the direction of emotional intensity of the parent. Interruption of interpersonal communication with an infant signifies shifts in the motivation for communication, possibly toward a self-protecting agitation, distress, and call for attention to relieve some internal physiological need or for physical comfort (Trevarthen, 2005; Porges, 2010; Kokkinaki et al., 2016). In connection with this, this study showed that both mothers and fathers are similarly sensitive to signs of infant agitation, distress and call for attention. We showed that when infants were in a negative emotional state, both parents responded with serious attention to their infant than with pleasure. Furthermore, we showed that infant negative emotional expressions occurred more often in interactions with mothers than with fathers, while the reverse was observed for infant neutral expressions, which occurred more often in interactions with fathers than with mothers. It seems that mothers and fathers are effective affectionate companions in a complementary way in comforting infant self-protecting agitation (expressed either by negative or through neutral emotion of self-absorption).

Emotional Coordination Between Infant–Mother and Infant–Father Interactions in the Course of Parental Direct Requests (Hypothesis 1b)

We showed similar patterns of emotional coordination and non-coordination between periods in which mothers and fathers made direct requests to their infants. Interpersonal engagement profiles that accompany early parental direct requests may be precursors of the way infants’ awareness of the adult’s communicative intention emerges within a ‘response space’ created by the parents’ directives (Reddy, 2015). In connection to this, these profiles add support to the assumption that ‘…embodied directives demonstrate how mutual accountability, and trust are produced and instantiated through family members’ moment-to-moment participation in everyday interactions’ (Cekaite, 2010, p. 20). Directive trajectories influence the ongoing production of the intentional action (Cekaite, 2010). In particular, in the course of directive/response sequence, parental attention to the infant interest expressions (matching of interest expressions) may reflect the speaker’s effort (parent) to monitor the body of her addressee (infant). This will enable the speaker (parent) to assess whether his/her talk is receiving the forms of alignment and co-participation that she proposes to be relevant. The configuration of bodies into facing formations permit direct face-to-face interpersonal communication through close parent–child alignment in an intercorporeal framework for mutual engagement (Cekaite, 2010). This example may constitute an early developmental path for the embodied shape and the interactional architecture of directives in toddlers and children through which their active participation in intimate interpersonal relations is accomplished.

We provided evidence of similar instances of emotional non-coordination in the two dyads when (a) infants did not synchronize the shifts of their emotions to their parents and (b) when the infant did not attune to the direction of emotional intensity of the parent. It seems that interruptions of cooperative dialogue between parent-infant pairs vary more during parental direct requests compared to questions. On the basis of this, we assume that preverbal infants may use their affections in spontaneous interaction with their parents to express early signs of sensitivity to the different intentions inherent in parental infant-directed questions and directives. This suggestion invites further empirical investigation.

Relationship Between Infant Age, Parental Gender, and Infant–Parent Emotional Matching/Non-coordination in the Course of Questions (Hypothesis 2)

This study indicated that the developmental trajectory of face-to-face emotional matching in the course of questions follows a similar decreasing trend in interactions of infants with their mothers and fathers across the age range of this study (2nd to 6th month of life). This is in accordance with developments in the body and in the motivating processes of the infant brain. These developments trigger transformations in the way infants regulate their feelings and express their interests to their companions at certain ages (Trevarthen, 2011). After 6 weeks, in the period of ‘Primary Intersubjectivity,’ during intimate protoconversations, the young infant shows direct sensitivity to the timing and values of facial expressions of feeling in intimate contact with caregivers (Trevarthen, 1993b). From 3 to 6 months of life, in the ‘Period of Games,’ the infant becomes more playful, responding to rhythmic provocations of expressive movements in ‘person–person games’ and body play. As a consequence of this transformation, infants lose their excitement for face-to-face proto-conversation while exploration of the environment increases and the manipulation of objects becomes effective (Trevarthen, 2017).

Further, we showed non-significant effects of infant age, parent gender and (infant age)(parent gender) interaction on the developmental trajectory of emotional non-coordination. Despite these non-significant effects, we showed a negative linear trend on age different between interaction of infants with mothers and fathers, with fathers showing a steepest slope on age (Figure 2). This finding does not verify our expectations of an upward trend of infant-parent emotional non-coordination after the 3rd month (‘Period of Games’) due to transformations in the way infants regulate their feelings while exploring the environment. This unexpected result may be due to the following factors: (a) repeated measures analysis of variance was based on a composite index which incorporated both cases of infant external interest expressions and infant negative emotional states (see in the section of Statistical Analysis), and the inclusion of infant negative emotions in this case may thus restrict us from getting a full picture of the transformations in the way infants regulate their emotional expressions and explore the environment after the 3rd month; (b) we did not include in this analysis cases of mutual infant–parent external interest expressions since these were expressed in low frequencies by parents; and (c) the developmental trajectories that showed cases of infant–parent emotional non-coordination are restricted only to maternal and paternal infant-directed questions. These issues invite further research which will separate cases of infant external interest expressions from instances of infant negative emotional states. In addition, future research may include more speech acts and verbal expressions of parental infant-directed speech. This will enable us to gain a complete picture of the way parents and their infants express their emotional ‘disagreement’ on the basis of embodied intersubjectivity.

Taken these together, the developmental trajectories of infant–mother and infant–father emotional coordination in the course of parental infant-directed questions across the age range of this study imply that mothers and fathers respond with similar sensitivity to clearly marked advances in the infant’s attentiveness and playfulness, self-awareness, and engagements with objects (Trevarthen and Aitken, 2003). Further research is needed to complete the evidence of this study on the developmental course of infant–parent emotional non-coordination in the course of parental infant-directed speech.

Potential Clinical Implications

Similarities in the way infants’ facial “unfolded” in relation to parental expressions of feelings provided evidence of the sensitive and complementary company and care both parents give to developing infant. The potential implications of this study are related to the following issues:

(a) the inclusion of both mothers and fathers in designing interventions for the promotion of early interactions and play between children with their parents. The evidence of our study may extend our understanding of the moderating role that fathers may have on the family process and on infant development (Vakrat et al., 2018); and (b) the connection between early patterns of infant-parent emotional communication with later cognitive and behavioral child development outcomes and attachment. In particular, typical development (vs. psychopathological development), and more positive child outcomes have been correlated with better mother–child synchrony. The quality of social interactions is a function of an active infant–parent ‘dialogue’ and depends on the infant’s desire to be interactive and the parent’s capacity to be attuned. Mother–child interactions are poorer in synchrony when one of the partners is impeded by internal (pathological) or environmental distractor. For instance, on the one hand, studies with depressed mothers (with limited maternal sensitivity and empathy) showed a trend to less synchrony/coherence. On the other hand, interactions of mothers with their premature infants showed lower coherence. Regarding environmental distractors, among high-risk, low-income toddler boys, synchrony was negatively associated with child emotional negativity (for a full discussion see Leclère et al., 2014).

In the meanwhile, during the first year of life, “Synchrony is not an all or nothing concept; rather, it may be more valid to think about dyadic interactions as approaching or moving away from synchrony” (p. 23, Leclère et al., 2014). On this ground, fluctuations in infant–parent emotional regulation, which imply both breaks of communication along with short ‘real’ parent–infant dialogues1, are important for the promotion of adaptation, creativity and stimulation (Leclère et al., 2014; Kokkinaki et al., 2016). The findings of this study may thus add understanding for the developmental importance of interactive errors and repairs in early infancy (Tronick and Cohn, 1989). Further research will inform us on the long-term implications of these engagement patterns on child development outcomes.

Limitations of This Study

The conclusions of this study are restricted in certain ways. The number of subjects was small to allow for efficient measurement of the between-subjects variability. Coherence in interaction patterns of mothers and fathers with their infants as a consequence of their marital life and caregiving may prevent us from identifying the possible differences between them (Cerezo et al., 2017). Our conclusions come from a Cretan (Greek) population, and they need to be verified through replication of the study in other cultures with different beliefs of how parents should give company and care to their developing infant. The families of infants chosen for this study are not representative of the whole population, being families who volunteered. From the current study, no inferences can be made with respect to causality.

Although we video recorded infant–parent pairs in a naturalistic environment familiar to participants, asking parents to play with their baby while being video recorded by the researcher may have been felt to be intrusive. Despite the limited knowledge on this matter, the gender of the parent may influence reactivity (Gardner, 2000). Mothers express a higher frequency of behaviors in situations of video recorded play than during the everyday observations (Abels et al., 2017). The natural behavior of fathers may have been more influenced than mothers by observation (Russell et al., 1992). Furthermore, data coming from language input in the course of naturalistic observations, showing striking fluctuations interspersed with silence, may provide a different picture of infant language experiences than that of structured play, which is consistently dense (Tamis-LeMonda et al., 2017).

Conclusion

In sum, we have provided evidence both maternal and paternal infant-directed questions along with direct requests constitute examples of early embodied intersubjectivity. Here, intersubjectivity is expressed through facial expressions of emotional coordination and non-coordination as well as through age-related changes of interpersonal engagement. These changes reflect transformations in the way infants regulate their feelings and express their interests to their companions in the first semester of life. Further, similarities in the way infants ‘unfolded’ their facial expressions of emotion in relation to parental expressions of feelings between maternal and paternal questions and direct requests, may imply that the human infant is born ready to actively participate in short ‘real’ dialogues with both the mother and the father. We provided preliminary evidence that preverbal infants may use their affections in spontaneous interaction with their parents to express early signs of sensitivity to the different intentions inherent in parental infant-directed questions and directives. This invites further empirical investigation. Additionally, parents, both the mother and the father, are sensitive to the infant’s capacities for emotional regulation, coordination, and communication of feelings (Trevarthen et al., 2006; Trevarthen, 2011, 2012, 2017).

Author’s Note

The video recordings of spontaneous infant–parent interactions used in this study were made for the Ph.D. research of the first author at the Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, under the supervision of Prof. Colwyn Trevarthen.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Lothian Research Ethics Committee and the Psychiatry/Clinical Psychology Research Ethics Sub-Committee of the Royal Infirmary of Edinburgh (NHS Trust). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

TK carried out this research and authored the sections “Introduction,” “Materials and Methods,” and “Discussion.” VV authored the sections “Statistical Analysis” and “Results”. Both authors contributed to manuscript revision and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are deeply indebted to the infants and their families for offering their time, cooperation, and patience to participate in the study.

Footnotes

  1. ^ We thank reviewer 1 for the suggestion of the phrase “short ‘real’ dialogues,” we adopted.

References

Abels, M., Papaligoura, Z., Lamm, B., and Yovsi, R. D. (2017). How usual is “Play as you usually would”? A comparison of naturalistic mother-infant interactions with videorecorded play sessions in three cultural communities. Child Dev. Res. 2017:7842030. doi: 10.1155/2017/7842030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J., Gilstrap, B., and Rovine, M. (1984). The Pennsylvania infant and family development project, I: stability and change in mother-infant and father-infant interaction in a family setting at one, three, and nine months. Child Dev. 55, 692–705. doi: 10.2307/1130122

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, C. T., Levitt, A., and McRoberts, G. W. (1991). “Examination of language-specific influences in infants’ discrimination of prosodic categories,” in Proceedings of the XIIth International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Aix-en Provence: France-Universite de Provence. 4, 162–165.

Google Scholar

Branger, M. C. E., Emmen, R. A. G., Woudstra, M.-I. J., Alink, L. R. A., and Mesman, J. (2019). Context matters: maternal and paternal sensitivity to infants in four settings. J. Fam. Psychol. 33, 851–856. doi: 10.1037/fam0000562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braungart-Rieker, J., Garwood, M. M., Powers, B. P., and Notaro, P. C. (1998). Infant affect and affect regulation during the still-face paradigm with mothers and fathers: the role of infant characteristics and parental sensitivity. Dev. Psychol. 34, 1428–1437. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.6.1428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bullowa, M. (1979). “Introduction. Prelinguistic communication: a field for scientific research,” in Before Speech: the Beginning of Human Communication, ed. M. Bullowa (London: Cambridge University Press), 1–62.

Google Scholar

Cekaite, A. (2010). Shepherding the child: embodied directive sequences in parent-child interactions. Text Talk 30, 1–25. doi: 10.1515/TEXT.2010.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cerezo, M. A., Sierra-Garcia, P., Pons-Salvador, G., and Trenado, R. M. (2017). Parental and infant gender factors in parent-infant interaction: state-space dynamic analysis. Front. Psychol. 8:1724.

Google Scholar

Christensen, R. (1998). Analysis of Variance, Design and Regression. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

DeCasper, A. J., and Spence, M. J. (1986). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behav. Dev. 9, 133–150. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(86)90025-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimas, P. D. (1974). Auditory and linguistic processing of cues for place of articulation by infants. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 513–521. doi: 10.3758/bf03198580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimas, P. D., and Miller, J. L. (1980). Discrimination of the information for manner of articulation. Infant Behav. Dev. 3, 367–375. doi: 10.1016/s0163-6383(80)80044-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eimas, P. D., Siqueland, E. R., Jusczyk, P., and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science 171, 303–306.

Google Scholar

Elias, G., Hayes, A., and Broerse, J. (1988). Aspects of structure and content of maternal talk with infants. J. Child Psychol. Psyc. 29, 523–531. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1988.tb00743.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2003). Infant-mother and infant-father synchrony: the coregulation of positive arousal. Infant Ment. Health J. 24, 1–23. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent in mothers’ speech to infants: is the melody the message. Child Dev. 60, 1497–1510. doi: 10.2307/1130938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernald, A. (1993). Approval and disapproval: infant responsiveness to vocal affect in familiar and unfamiliar languages. Child Dev. 64, 657–674. doi: 10.2307/1131209

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fernald, A., and Kuhl, P. K. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for Motherese speech. Infant Behav. Dev. 10, 279–293. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(87)90017-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (1981). Infant arousal, attention and affect during early interactions. Adv. Infancy Res. 1, 57–100.

Google Scholar

Forbes, E. E., Cohn, J. F., Allen, N. B., and Lewinsohn, P. M. (2004). Infant affect during parent-infant interaction at 3 and 6 months: differences between mothers and fathers and influence of parent history of depression. Infancy 5, 61–84. doi: 10.1207/s15327078in0501_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frota, S., Butler, J., and Vigário, M. (2014). Infants’ perception of intonation: is it a statement or a question? Infancy 19, 194–213. doi: 10.1111/infa.12037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gardner, F. (2000). Methodological issues in the direct observation of parent-infant interaction: do observational findings reflect the natural behavior of participants? Clin. Child Fam. Psych. 3, 185–198. doi: 10.1023/A:1009503409699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geffen, S., and Mintz, T. (2011). “Seven-month-olds discrimination of statements and questions,” in Online Supplement of Proceedings of the 36th Boston University Conference on Language Development, eds, A. K. Biller, E. Y. Chung, and A. E. Kimball (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press).

Google Scholar

Geffen, S., and Mintz, T. H. (2017). Prosodic differences between declaratives and interrogatives in infant-directed speech. J. Child Lang. 44, 968–994. doi: 10.1017/S0305000916000349

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrera, E., Reissland, N., and Shepherd, J. (2004). Maternal touch and maternal child-directed speech: effects of depressed mood in the postnatal period. J. Affect. Disorders 81, 29–39. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houston, D. M. (2005). “Speech perception in infants,” in The Handbook of Speech Perception, eds D. B. Pisoni and R. E. Remez (Oxford: Blackwell Publishing Ltd), 416–448. doi: 10.1002/9780470757024.ch17

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houston, D. M. (2016). “Infant speech perception,” in Comprehensive Handbook of Pediatric Audiology, 2nd Edn, eds A. M. Tharpe and R. Seewald (San Diego, CA: Plural Publishing), 47–62.

Google Scholar

Jaffe, J., Stern, D. N., and Perry, J. C. (1973). Conversational coupling of gaze behavior in prelinguistic human development. J. Psycholinguist Res. 2, 321–330. doi: 10.1007/bf01067054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jia, R. (2014). Dynamic Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Interaction: Contribution of Parents’ and Infants’ Facial Affect and Prediction from Depression, Empathy and Temperament. Ph.D. thesis, Graduate School of the Ohio State University, Ohio.

Google Scholar

Jusczyk, P. W., and Aslin, R. N. (1995). Infants’ detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cogn. Psychol. 29, 1–23. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1995.1010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerbrat-Orecchioni, C. (1990). Les Interactions Verbales, Vol. 1. Paris: Armand Colin.

Google Scholar

Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Lee, K., Xie, X., Huang, H., Hui Ye, H., et al. (2003). Effects of experience on fetal voice recognition. Psychol. Sci. 14, 220–224. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.02435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kisilevsky, B. S., Hains, S. M. J., Brown, C. A., Lee, C. T., Cowperthwaite, B., Stutzman, S. S., et al. (2009). Fetal sensitivity to properties of maternal speech and language. Infant Behav. Dev. 32, 59–71. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T. (1998). Emotion and Imitation in Early Infant-Parent Interaction: A Longitudinal and Cross-cultural Study. Ph.D. thesis, Department of Psychology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh.

Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T. (2009). Emotional expressions during early infant-father conversation. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 6, 705–721. doi: 10.1080/17405620701848871

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T. (2013). Fathers’ speech to infants. Early Child Dev. Care 183, 1005–1025. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2012.712041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T. (2015). “Sex-related differences in cooperation and communication with young infants,” in Intersubjective Paths to Interpersonal Relations and Learning, Special Issue, Eleutherna: The Scientific Journal of the Department of Psychology, eds T. Kokkinaki and C. Trevarthen (Rethymnon: Department of Psychology, University of Crete), 99–150.

Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T. (2017). Paternal questioning as a component of innate intersubjectivity in earlyinfancy. Early Child Dev. Care 189, 583–599. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1332599

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T. (2018). Structural variations, quantitative differences and similarities between maternal and paternal infant-directed speech. Early Child Dev. Care 189, 1–18. doi: 10.1080/03004430.2017.1423482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T., and Vasdekis, V. G. S. (2015). Comparing emotional coordination in early spontaneous mother-infant and father-infant interactions. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 12, 69–84. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2014.950220

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kokkinaki, T., Vasdekis, V. G. S., Koufaki, Z., and Trevarthen, C. (2016). Coordination of emotions in mother-infant dialogues. Infant Child Dev. Version of Record online 26:e1973. doi: 10.1002/icd.1973

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruper, J. C., and Uzgiris, I. C. (1987). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech to young infants. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 16, 597–614. doi: 10.1007/bf01067087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhl, P. K. (1983). Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy. Infant Behav. Dev. 6, 263–285. doi: 10.1016/s0163-6383(83)80036-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Laflamme, D., Pomerleau, A., and Malcuit, G. (2002). A comparison of fathers’ and mothers; involvement in childcare and stimulation behaviors during free-play with their infants at 9 and 15 months. Sex Roles 47, 507–518.

Google Scholar

Landerholm, E. J., and Scriven, G. (1981). A comparison of mother and father interaction with their six-month-old male and female infants. Early Child Dev. Care 7, 317–328. doi: 10.1080/0300443810070405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasky, R. E., Syrdal-Lasky, A., and Klein, R. E. (1975). VOT discrimination by four to six and a half month old infants from Spanish environments. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 20, 215–225. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(75)90099-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leclère, C., Viaux, S., Avril, M., Achard, C., Chetouani, M., Missonnier, S., et al. (2014). Why synchrony matters during mother-child interactions: a systematic review. PLoS One 9:e113571. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0113571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, G. Y., and Kisilevsky, B. S. (2013). Fetuses respond to father’s voice but prefer mother’s voice after birth. Dev. Psychobiol. 56, 1–11. doi: 10.1002/dev.21084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montague, D. R., and Walker-Andrews, A. S. (2002). Mothers, fathers, and infants: the role of person familiarity and parental involvement in infants’ perception of emotion expressions. Child Dev. 73, 1339–1352. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pancsofar, N., and Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mother and father language input to young children: contributions to later language development. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 27, 571–587. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S. W. (2010). “Music therapy and trauma: insights from the polyvagal theory,” in Symposium on Music Therapy & Trauma: Bridging Theory and Clinical Practice, ed. K. Stewart (New York, NY: Satchnote Press), 3–15.

Google Scholar

Rabain-Jamin, J., and Sabeau-Jouannet, E. (1997). Maternalspeechto 4 - month – oldinfants in twocultures: wolof and French. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 20, 425–451. doi: 10.1080/016502597385216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reddy, V. (2015). Joining intentions in infancy. J. Consciousness Stud. 22, 24–44.

Google Scholar

Reimchen, M., and Soderstrom, M. (2017). Do questions get infants talking? Infant vocal responses to questions and declaratives in maternal speech. Infant Child Dev. 26:e1985. doi: 10.1002/icd.1985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reissland, N. (1998). Context dependency in parental speech. Brit. J. Dev. Psychol. 16, 365–373. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835x.1998.tb00758.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reissland, N. (2012). The Development of Emotional Intelligence: A Case Study. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Rondal, J. A. (1980). Fathers’ and mothers’ speech in early language development. J. Child Lang. 7, 353–369. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900002671

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Russell, A., Russell, G., and Midwinter, D. (1992). Observer influences on mothers and fathers: self reported influence during a home observation. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 36, 263–283.

Google Scholar

Siegel, G. (1963). Adult verbal behavior in ‘play therapy’ sessions with retarded children. JSHD Monogr, 10, 34–38.

Google Scholar

Slaney, M., and McRoberts, G. (2003). BabyEars: a recognition system for affective vocalizations. Speech Commun. 39, 367–384. doi: 10.1016/s0167-6393(02)00049-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snow, C. (1977). The development of conversation between mothers and babies. J. Child Lang. 4, 1–22. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900000453

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soderstrom, M., Ko, E.-S., and Nevzorova, U. (2011). It’s a question? Infants attend differently to yes/no questions and declaratives. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 107–110. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stephany, U., and Voelkova, M. D. (2015). Requests, their meanings and aspectual forms in early Greek and Russian Child Language. J. Greek Linguist. 15, 66–90. doi: 10.1163/15699846-01501002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swoboda, P., Morse, P. A., and Leavitt, L. A. (1976). Continuous vowel discrimination in normal and at-risk infants. Child Dev. 47, 459–465. doi: 10.2307/1128802

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sylvester-Bradley, B., and Trevarthen, C. (1978). “Baby talk as an adaptation to the infant’s communication,” in The Development of Communication, eds N. Waterson and C. Snow (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons), 75–92.

Google Scholar

Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., Luo, R., Escobar, K., and Bornstein, M. H. (2017). Power in methods: language to infants in structured and naturalistic contexts. Dev. Sci. 20:10.1111/desc.12456. doi: 10.1111/desc.12456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trehub, S. E. (1973). Infants’ sensitivity to vowel and tonal contrasts. Dev. Psychol. 9, 91–96. doi: 10.1037/h0034999

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1984). “Emotions in infancy: regulators of contact and relationships with persons,” in In Approaches to Emotions, eds K. Scherer and P. Ekman (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 129–157.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1993a). “The self born in intersubjectivity: an infant communicating,” in The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Knowledge of the Self, ed. U. Neisser (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 121–173. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511664007.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1993b). “The function of emotions in early infant communication and development,” in New Perspectives in Early Communicative Development, eds J. Nadel and L. Camaioni (London: Routledge), 48–81. doi: 10.4324/9781315111322-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (1997). The nature of motives for human consciousness. Psychol. J. Hellenic Psychol. Soc. 4, 187–221.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2005). “Stepping away from the mirror: pride and shame in adventures of companionship reflections on the nature and emotional needs of infant intersubjectivity,” in Attachment and Bonding: a New Synthesis, Dahlem Workshop Report 92, eds C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossman, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, S. W. Porges, et al. (Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press), 55–84.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2011). What young children give to their learning, making education work to sustain a community and its culture. Eur. Early Child Educ. 19, 173–193. doi: 10.1080/1350293x.2011.574405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2012). Embodied human intersubjectivity: imaginative agency, to share meaning. Cogn. Semiotics 4, 6–56.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C. (2017). “Chapter 15. Play with infants: the impulse for human story-telling,” in The Routledge International Handbook of Play in Early Childhood, eds T. Bruce, P. Hakkarainen, and M. Bredikyte (Abington: Taylor & Francis/Routledge).

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., and Aitken, K. J. (2003). “Regulation of brain development and age-related changes in infants’ motives: the developmental function of ‘regressive’ periods,” in Regression Periods in Human Infancy, ed. M. Heimann (Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum), 107–184.

Google Scholar

Trevarthen, C., Aitken, K. J., Vandekerckhove, M., Delafield-Butt, J., and Nagy, E. (2006). “Collaborative regulations of vitality in early childhood: stress in intimate relationships and postnatal psychopathology,” in Developmental Psychopathology, Volume 2 Developmental Neuroscience, 2nd Edn, eds D. Cicchetti and D. J. Cohen (New York: Wileys).

Google Scholar

Tronick, E. Z., and Cohn, J. F. (1989). Infant-mother face-to-face interaction: age and gender differences in coordination and the occurrence of miscoordination. Child Dev. 60, 85–92. doi: 10.2307/1131074

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vakrat, A., Apter-Levy, Y., and Feldman, R. (2018). Fathering moderates the effects of maternal depression on the family process. Dev. Psychopathol. 30, 27–38. doi: 10.1017/s095457941700044x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van de Weijer, J. (2002). Terminal rises in infant-directed and adult-directed questions. Proc. Fonetik TMH-QPSR 44, 005–008.

Google Scholar

Voegtline, K. M., Costigan, K. A., Pater, H., and DiPetro, J. A. (2013). Near-term fetal response to maternal spoken voice. Infant Behav. Dev. 36:10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.05.002. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yogman, M. W. (1981). Games fathers and mothers play with their infants. Inf. Ment. Health J. 2, 241–248. doi: 10.1002/1097-0355(198124)2:4<241::aid-imhj2280020406>3.0.co;2-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: questions, direct requests, facial expressions, emotions, interpersonal engagement, innate intersubjectivity, infancy

Citation: Kokkinaki T and Vasdekis VGS (2020) Beyond the Words: Comparing Interpersonal Engagement Between Maternal and Paternal Infant-Directed Speech Acts. Front. Psychol. 11:523551. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.523551

Received: 30 December 2019; Accepted: 10 November 2020;
Published: 03 December 2020.

Edited by:

Caspar Addyman, Goldsmiths, University of London, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Asta Cekaite, Linköping University, Sweden
Christine Heinisch, University of Erlangen–Nuremberg, Germany

Copyright © 2020 Kokkinaki and Vasdekis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Theano Kokkinaki, kokkinaki@uoc.gr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.