The Broad Reach of Multivariable Thinking

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.22329/il.v43i1.7639

Keywords:

causal reasoning, explanation, sociopolitical views, multivariable thinking

Abstract

Simple explanations are very often inadequate and can encourage faulty inferences. We examined college students’ explanations regarding illegal immigration to determine the prevalence of single-factor explanations. The form of students’ explanations was predicted by their responses on a simple three-item forced-choice multivariable causal reasoning task in which they selected the strongest evidence against a causal claim. In a further qualitative investigation of explanations by a sample of community adults, we identified positive features among those who scored high on this multivariable causal reasoning task. We consider limitations of single-factor reasoning and means of encouraging more comprehensive explanations to support claims.

References

Ahn, W. and J. Bailenson. 1996. Causal attribution as a search for underlying mechanisms: An explanation of the conjunction fallacy and the discounting principle. Cognitive Psychology 31(1): 82-123.

Ahn, W., C. Kalish, D. Medin and S. Gelman. 1995. The role of co-variation versus mechanism information in causal attribution. Cognition 54(3): 299-352.

Arvidsson, T. S. and D. Kuhn. 2021. Realizing the full potential of individualizing learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology 65: 1-24. doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2021.101960

Barbera, P., J. Jost, J. Nagler, J. Tucker and R. Bonneau. 2015. Tweet-ing from left to right: Is online political communication more than an echo chamber? Psychological Science 26(10): 1531-1542.

Fernbach, P., A. Darlow and S. Sloman. 2010. Neglect of alternative causes in predictive but not diagnostic reasoning. Psychological Science 21(3): 329-336.

Fisher, M. and F. Keil. 2014. The illusion of argument justification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 143(1): 425-433.

Grant, A. 2021. Think again. New York: Viking Press.

Harman, G. 1965. The inference to the best explanation. The Philo-sophical Review 74(1): 88-95.

Iordanou, K. and D. Kuhn. 2020. Contemplating the opposition: Does a personal touch matter? Discourse Processes 57(4): 343-359.

Jewett, E. and D. Kuhn. 2016. Social science as a tool in developing scientific thinking skills in underserved, low-achieving urban stu-dents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 143: 154-161.

Johnson, S., J. Valenti and F. Keil. 2019. Simplicity and complexity preferences in causal explanation: An opponent heuristic account. Cognitive Psychology 113: 101222.

Johnson, S., T. Merchant and F. Keil. 2020. Belief digitization: Do we treat uncertainty as probabilities or as bits? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 149(8): 1417-1434.

Kahan, D. 2013. Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflec-tion. Judgment and Decision Making 8(4): 407-424.

Kelley, H. 1973. The processes of causal attribution. American Psy-chologist 28(2): 107-128.

Koslowski, B. 1996. Theory and evidence: The development of scientific reasoning. Cambridge: MIT Press.

Kuhn, D. 1991. The skills of argument. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Kuhn, D. 2012. The development of causal reasoning. WIREs Cognitive Science. DOI:10.1002/wcs.1160.

Kuhn, D. 2020. Why is reconciling divergent views a challenge? Cur-rent Directions in Psychological Science 29(1): 27-32.

Kuhn, D. 2022. Metacognition matters in many ways. Educational Psychologist 57(2): 73-86.

Kuhn, D., T. S. Arvidsson, R. Lesperance and R. Corprew. 2017. Can engaging in science practices promote deep understanding of them? Science Education 101: 232-250.

Kuhn, D., A. Cummings and M. Youmans. 2020. Is reasoning a fruit-ful path to changing minds? Discourse Processes 57: 36-47.

Kuhn, D., D. Floyd, P. Yaksick, M. Halpern and W. Ricks. 2018. How does discourse among like-minded individuals affect their thinking about a complex issue? Thinking and Reasoning 25(3): 365-382.

Kuhn, D. and K. Iordanou. 2022. Why do people argue past one anoth-er rather than with one another? In Reason, bias, and inquiry: New perspectives from the crossroads of epistemology and psychology, eds. N. Ballantyne, D. Dunning, 324-338. New York: Oxford Uni-versity Press.

Kuhn, D. and A. Modrek. 2018. Do reasoning limitations undermine discourse? Thinking and Reasoning 24(1): 97-116.

Kuhn, D. and A. Modrek. 2021. Mere exposure to dialogic framing enriches argumentive thinking. Applied Cognitive Psychology 35(5): 1349-1355.

Kuhn, D., M. Pease and C. Wirkala. 2009. Coordinating effects of multiple variables: A skill fundamental to causal and scientific rea-soning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 103(3): 268-284.

Kuhn, D., S. Ramsey and T. S. Arvidsson. 2015. Developing multivari-able thinkers. Cognitive Development 35: 92-110.

Lagnado, D. 2021. Explaining the evidence: How the mind investigates the world. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Lesperance, R. and D. Kuhn. 2023. Breadth and relevance of multivari-able inquiry supports deep understanding of science practice. Sci-ence Education 107: 71-88.

Lim, J. and D. Oppenheimer. 2020. Explanatory preferences for com-plexity matching. Plos One 15(4): doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230929

Lombrozo, T. 2007. Simplicity and probability in causal explanation. Cognitive Psychology 55(3): 232-257.

Mercier, H. and D. Sperber. 2011. Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34(2): 57-111.

Moshman, D. 2015. Epistemic cognition and development: The psychol-ogy of justification and truth. New York: Psychology Press.

Pilditch, T., N. Fenton and D. Lagnado. 2019. The zero-sum fallacy in evidence evaluation. Psychological Science 30(2): 250-260.

Rapanta, C., M. Garcia-Mila and S. Gilabert. 2013. What is meant by argumentative competence? An integrative review of methods of analysis and assessment in education. Review of Educational Re-search 83(4): 483-520.

Ricco, R. 2015. The development of reasoning. In Handbook of child psychology and developmental science. Vol. 2 (7th ed.), vol. eds. L. Liben, U. Mueller, series ed. R. Lerner, 519-570. Hoboken NJ: Wiley.

Rozenblit, L. and F. Keil. 2002. The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science 26(5): 521-562.

Sloman, S. and P. Fernbach. 2017. The knowledge illusion: Why we never think alone. New York, NY: Riverhead Books (a division of Penguin Publishing Group).

Sloman, S. and N. Rabb. 2019. Thought as a determinant of political opinion. Cognition 188(4): 1-7.

Thagard, P. 1978. The best explanation: Criteria for theory choice. The Journal of Philosophy 75(2): 76-92.

Thagard, P. 1989. Explanatory coherence. Behavioral and Brain Scienc-es 12(3): 435-502.

Vasilyeva, N. and T. Lombrozo. 2015. Explanations and causal judg-ments are differentially sensitive to covariation and mechanism in-formation. In Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cogni-tive Science Society, eds D. C. Noelle, R. Dale, A. S. Warlaumont, J. Yoshimi, T. Matlock, C. D. Jennings, P. P. Maglio, 2475-2480. Aus-tin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

Walker, C., T. Lombrozo, J. Williams, A. Rafferty, and A. Gopnik. 2017. Explaining constrains causal learning in childhood. Child De-velopment 88(1): 229-246.

Zavala, J. and D. Kuhn. 2017. Solitary discourse is a productive activi-ty. Psychological Science 28(5): 578-586.

Zemla, J., S. Sloman, C. Bechlivanidis and D. Lagnado. 2017. Evaluat-ing everyday explanations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24(5): 1488-1500.

Zemla, J., S. Sloman, C. Bechlivanidis and D. Lagnado. 2022. Not so simple! Mechanisms increase preference for complex explanations. SSRN. http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.4130789

Downloads

Published

2023-03-28

Issue

Section

Articles