Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-04T00:46:04.646Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

“What” matters more than “Why” – Neonatal behaviors initiate social responses

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 December 2017

Klaus Libertus
Affiliation:
Learning Research and Development Center (LRDC) & Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260. KLL60@pitt.eduLibertus@pitt.eduhttp://www.psychology.pitt.edu/people/klaus-libertus-phdhttp://www.lrdc.pitt.edu/people/researcher-detail.cshtml?id=530
Melissa E. Libertus
Affiliation:
Learning Research and Development Center (LRDC) & Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260. KLL60@pitt.eduLibertus@pitt.eduhttp://www.psychology.pitt.edu/people/klaus-libertus-phdhttp://www.lrdc.pitt.edu/people/researcher-detail.cshtml?id=530
Christa Einspieler
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience (iDN), Department of Phoniatrics, Medical University of Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria. christa.einspieler@medunigraz.atpeter.marschik@medunigraz.athttp://www.medunigraz.at/idn/team/christa-einspieler/http://www.medunigraz.at/idn/team/peter-b-marschik/
Peter B. Marschik
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Developmental Neuroscience (iDN), Department of Phoniatrics, Medical University of Graz, 8036 Graz, Austria. christa.einspieler@medunigraz.atpeter.marschik@medunigraz.athttp://www.medunigraz.at/idn/team/christa-einspieler/http://www.medunigraz.at/idn/team/peter-b-marschik/ Center of Neurodevelopmental Disorders (KIND), Department of Women's and Children's Health, Karolinska Institutet, 11330 Stockholm, Sweden.

Abstract

Newborns are born into a social environment that dynamically responds to them. Newborn behaviors may not have explicit social intentions but will nonetheless affect the environment. Parents contingently respond to their child, enabling newborns to learn about the consequences of their behaviors and encouraging the behavior itself. Consequently, newborn behaviors may serve both biological and social-cognitive purposes during development.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development 21(3):205–26.Google Scholar
Carpendale, J. I. M. & Carpendale, A. B. (2010) The development of pointing: From personal directedness to interpersonal direction. Human Development 53(3):110–26.Google Scholar
DeCasper, A. J. & Carstens, A. A. (1981) Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development 4:1935.Google Scholar
DeCasper, A. J. & Fifer, W. P. (1980) Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science 208(4448):1174–76.Google Scholar
Einspieler, C., Marschik, P. B. & Prechtl, H. F. R. (2008) Human motor behavior: Prenatal origin and early postnatal development. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology 216(3):147–53. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1027/0044-3409.216.3.147.Google Scholar
Einspieler, C., Prayer, D. & Prechtl, H. F. R. (2012) Fetal behaviour – A neurodevelopmental approach. Wiley.Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2007) Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 48(3–4):329–54. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floccia, C., Nazzi, T. & Bertoncini, J. (2000) Unfamiliar voice discrimination for short stimuli in newborns. Developmental Science 3(3):333–43. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, S., Wang, C., Salgado, M. W., Greene, P. E., Kim, M. & Rapin, I. (2009) Motor stereotypies in children with autism and other developmental disorders. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 51(1):3038. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2008.03178.x.Google Scholar
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Adolph, K. E. (2011) Transition from crawling to walking and infants' actions with objects and people. Child Development 82(4):11991209. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01595.x.Google Scholar
Karasik, L. B., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. & Adolph, K. E. (2014) Crawling and walking infants elicit different verbal responses from mothers. Developmental Science 17(3):388–95. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12129.Google Scholar
Keller, H. (2003) Socialization for competence: Cultural models of infancy. Human Development 46(5):288311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leckman, J. F., Feldman, R., Swain, J. E., Eicher, V., Thompson, N. & Mayes, L. C. (2004) Primary parental preoccupation: Circuits, genes, and the crucial role of the environment. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna) 111(7):753–71. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-003-0067-x.Google Scholar
Liszkowski, U., Brown, P., Callaghan, T., Takada, A. & de Vos, C. (2012) A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science 36(4):698713. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x.Google Scholar
Marschik, P. B., Prechtl, H. F. R., Prayer, D., Peyton, C. & Einspieler, C. (2013) An antecedent of later developing communicative functions: The fetal index finger. British Medical Journal 347:232.Google Scholar
Masten, A. S. & Cicchetti, D. (2010) Developmental cascades. Development and Psychopathology 22(3):491–95. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000222.Google Scholar
Matson, J. L., Dempsey, T. & Fodstad, J. C. (2009) Stereotypies and repetitive/restrictive behaviours in infants with autism and pervasive developmental disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation 12(3):122–27. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1080/17518420902936730.Google Scholar
Needham, A., Barrett, T. & Peterman, K. (2002) A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using ‘sticky mittens’ enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behavior and Development 25(3):279–95. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00097-8.Google Scholar
Prechtl, H. F. R., ed. (1984) Continuity of neural functions from prenatal to postnatal life. Spastics International Medical Publications.Google Scholar
Rovee, C. K. & Rovee, D. T. (1969) Conjugate reinforcement of infant exploratory behavior. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 8(1):33–9. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(69)90025-3.Google Scholar
Stevenson, M. B., Ver Hoeve, J. N., Roach, M. A. & Leavitt, L. A. (1986) The beginning of conversation: Early patterns of mother-infant vocal responsiveness. Infant Behavior and Development 9(4):423–40. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(86)90016-0.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vouloumanos, A. & Werker, J. F. (2007) Listening to language at birth: Evidence for a bias for speech in neonates. Developmental Science 10(2):159–64. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00549.x.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978) Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar