Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 October 2022
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Longitudinal measurement invariance of the meaning in life questionnaire in Chinese college students

Jie Luo&#x;Jie Luo1Fu-Chuan Tang&#x;Fu-Chuan Tang1Ren Yang&#x;Ren Yang2Jie GongJie Gong3Cheng-Kui YaoCheng-Kui Yao1Xinquan Huang
Xinquan Huang4*Wei Chen
Wei Chen1*Shuo-Ying Zhao
Shuo-Ying Zhao5*
  • 1School of Psychology, Guizhou Normal University, Guiyang, China
  • 2Office of Academic Research, University of Finance and Economics, Hangzhou, China
  • 3The School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 4School of Marxism, Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China
  • 5Department of Psychology, Kaili University, Kaili City, China

The Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ) is a popular tool to measure the presence of and one’s search for meaning in life. Although the validity of the MLQ has been verified in previous studies, the evidence from longitudinal measurement invariance (LMI) of the MLQ is still lacking. The current study aimed to examine the LMI of the MLQ in a sample of Chinese college students (N = 328) at a 1-year interval. Multigroup confirmatory factor analysis (MCFA) was used to examine the LMI of the MLQ over four time points (over the course of 1 year). Results indicate that the MLQ has strict longitudinal invariance across 1-year in Chinese college students, and the latent means difference of MLQ-P is not significant differences across time, while the latent means difference of MLQ-S show significant differences between Time 1 and the other time points. Moreover, the internal consistency reliabilities (e.g., alpha and omega) of the MLQ scores were acceptable at all four time points, and the stability coefficients across time were moderate. These findings provide preliminary evidence that the MLQ has satisfactory longitudinal properties in Chinese college students.

Introduction

Meaning in life refers to the sense made of and the significance one feels regarding the nature of one’s being and existence (Steger et al., 2006). As one of the most popular concepts in positive psychology, meaning in life plays an important role in the development of mental health and well-being (King et al., 2006; Ryff and Singer, 2010; Glaw et al., 2016). Previous studies have shown that a lack of meaning in life can lead to numerous mental health disorders, such as anxiety (Shiah et al., 2015), depression (Salami et al., 2022), and loneliness (King and Hicks, 2021; Macià et al., 2021), even increasing the risk of aggressive behavior or suicide (Wilchek-Aviad, 2015). Therefore, in the prevention of or intervention against these mental health disorders, it is crucial to be able to measure meaning in life accurately (Chen et al., 2020).

Several tools have been developed to assess meaning in life over the past 40 years (Brandstätter et al., 2012). Popular questionnaires include the Purpose in Life Test (PIL; Crumbaugh, 1969), the Life Regard Index (LRI, Battista and Almond, 1973), and the Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ; Steger et al., 2006). Compared to the first two tools, the MLQ has unique advantages. For one, the factor structure of the PIL has rarely been verified in empirical studies, despite its development initially stemming from meaning therapy (Reker and Cousins, 1979; McGregor and Little, 1998). Furthermore, the items used in PIL and LRI can be seen as problematic in the measurement of meaning in life. For instance, the PIL and the LRI contain items such as, “With regard to suicide, I have thought of it seriously as a way out” and “I feel really good about my life.” Such statements can also be measured based on concepts other than meaning, such as emotion (cf. Steger et al., 2006). Finally, according to Frankl, 1963 theory, the core content of meaning in life is the seeking of meaning, but this has rarely been supported using existing questionnaires which measure meaning in life (Steger et al., 2006).

To address these gaps in knowledge, Steger et al. (2006) developed and validated the 10-item MLQ to measure meaning in life. The MLQ includes Presence and Search subscales, with the Presence subscale representing the presence of meaning or purpose in one’s life, and the Search subscale measuring one’s search for meaning in their life. According to Steger et al. (2006), presence of meaning can be assessed separately from search for meaning (Steger et al., 2006).

The MLQ has been used widely since its development, and many studies in different cultural contexts (i.e., Western and non-Western countries) have shown that the MLQ has satisfactory psychometric properties (e.g., internal consistency, retest reliability, structure validity; see Table 1 for details). For example, an Italian study in adults showed that the Italian version of the MLQ had good reliability (both αs >0.80) and the original two-factor model was supported (CFI = 0.99, GFI = 0.94, AGFI = 0.91, RMSEA = 0.059; Negri et al., 2020). Some studies have also replicated the original structure model in Chinese populations (e.g., Wang and Dai, 2008; Liu and Gan, 2010; Chen et al., 2015). Chen et al. (2015) revealed that the Chinese version of the MLQ had high structural validity when maintaining the two-factor structure, and showed that the same conclusion was supported through Rasch analysis. Studies have also been done in specific groups such as patients with life-threatening illnesses (Naghiyaee et al., 2020), displaced people (Chika et al., 2019), and those caring for chronically ill people (Chan, 2014). The factor structures obtained were all the same as those compiled by Steger et al. (2006), and the internal consistency coefficients were all above 0.8. Overall, prior studies support the proposed psychometric properties of the MLQ, but none of these studies have tested the longitudinal properties of the MLQ, specifically longitudinal measurement invariance (LMI) of MLQ scores over time.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of findings of psychometric properties of the MLQ in prior studies.

Measurement invariance of the MLQ

Measurement invariance (MI) is vital for cross-group comparison because the interpretation of mean differences across groups may be misguided and questionable unless latent constructs in different subgroups are equivalent (Byrne and Watkins, 2003; Chen, 2008). In other words, the establishment of MI is the precondition for cross-group comparison (e.g., male vs. female; Chen, 2008). Prior studies have tested the MI of the MLQ in various countries including Brazil, China, Greek, Japan, and Romania (e.g., Damásio and Koller, 2015; Stalikas et al., 2018; Satoshi and Kohki, 2019; Balgiu, 2020; Datu and Yuen, 2022). More specifically, the Brazilian version of MLQ showed strict invariance for gender and age groups (i.e., youth, adults, and the elderly) (Damásio and Koller, 2015). Likewise, gender invariance was verified in a Greek (Stalikas et al., 2018) and Romanian samples (Balgiu, 2020). The MI among Chinese students aged 10–25 has been verified and shows strict MI across age groups (i.e., early adolescence, middle adolescence, late adolescence, and early adulthood; Wang et al., 2016).

Measurement invariance involves cross-sectional data as well as longitudinal data, with the latter representing MI over different points in time. Although previous studies have focused on the MI of the MLQ across different groups (e.g., gender and age) through cross-sectional investigation, the LMI of MLQ scores has not been explored. Similar to MI, LMI also tests the equality of a construct for a tool, but its focus is on equality across time, not across groups (Millsap and Cham, 2012). LMI is a desirable equality, as its results can show that the same construct can be tested across occasions (i.e., configural, metric, scalar, and strict invariance), providing a necessary and stable foundation for the comparison of mean values in longitudinal research. According to Steger and Kashdan (2007), moderate stability was found for the two subscales of the MLQ across a longer term (i.e., over a 13-month period), with a stability coefficient of 0.41 for the Presence subscale (MLQ-P) and 0.50 for the Search subscale (MLQ-S). Moreover, prior longitudinal studies have tested the correlation between meaning in life and other covariates in health and positive psychology (e.g., Hsiao et al., 2013; Negru-Subtirica et al., 2016; Park et al., 2020; Shek et al., 2021), yet these studies did not examine whether meaning in life and its two components have MI over time. The present study is thus the first to measure whether the MLQ has LMI across time.

The current study

The main purpose of the present study was to examine the LMI of MLQ in a sample of Chinese college students. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was conducted to test whether the MLQ scores had LMI across time, testing the configural, metric, scalar, and strict invariance over a 12-month interval. Based on Steger et al. (2006)’s original work, we assumed that the LMI of MLQ could be verified. Finally, the internal consistency indices (i.e., Cronbach’s α, McDonald’s ω, and mean inter-item correlation), stability coefficients, and latent factor means comparison were also tested.

Materials and methods

Participants

The sample for the present study was drawn from a normal university in Guizhou province, China. A Monte Carlo study suggested that a sample size of 200 would be needed for a power of 0.80 in a CFA model (Muthén and Muthén, 2002), and sample would be used to test the longitudinal properties of the MLQ across a 1-year interval with four waves. A total of 328 college students comprising 50 males (15.2%) and 278 females (84.8%) took part in the first survey in April 2018 (Time 1; aged 18 to 24; Mage = 20.82, SD = 1.18). The second survey involved the same participants as in August 2018, and took place at an interval of 3 months (Time 2). The third and fourth surveys were conducted in December 2018 (Time 3) and April 2019 (Time 4), respectively, with no sample loss. The sample was made up of 153 sophomores (46.6%) and 175 juniors (53.4%). In terms of cultural background, 197 students (60.1%) were of Han ethnicity, with the remaining 39.9% of ethnic minority backgrounds (N = 131). Finally, 150 students majored in science (45.7%) and 178 students in liberal arts (54.3%).

Procedure

The study questionnaire was completed in a classroom setting while participants attended their classes. More specifically, the investigation was announced to the students in class by the lecturers (who included the researchers), which had been agreed upon earlier in discussion with the researchers of the current study. Participants completed the paper and pencil questionnaire and returned it to the study administrator directly after completion. To facilitate identification and follow-up, each participant was given a random number at the baseline survey point, and participants completed the questionnaire at four times over the 1-year period of the study. Before beginning the formal study, all participants were informed as to the nature, goal, confidentiality, and anonymity of the study, and were told that their responses would not affect their school performance. The full investigation followed the principle of voluntariness and participants were able to withdraw from the study at any time. This study questionnaire took about 5 to 8 min to complete each time. The study was approved by the Subjects Review Committee of Guizhou Normal University (GZNUPSY.No2018M[005]).

Measures

The meaning in life questionnaire

The MLQ is a self-report questionnaire which was developed to assess one’s attitudes regarding the presence of and search for meaning in life (Steger et al., 2006). It includes 10 items and two factors: Presence (MLQ-P), and Search (MLQ-S). Five items assess the presence factor (e.g., “My life has a clear sense of purpose”) and five items assess the search factor (e.g., “I am looking for something that makes my life feel meaningful”). Each item is rated on a seven-point scale ranging from 1 (absolutely untrue) to 7 (absolutely true). The higher the MLQ scores, the higher the happiness experienced (Steger et al., 2009). The Chinese version of the MLQ (C-MLQ) has been validated and showed adequate internal consistency, and good factorial validity and construct validity (Wang and Dai, 2008). In the present study, the alphas for MLQ-P and MLQ-S at the four time points were 0.78/0.81, 0.84/0.85, 0.85/0.86, and 0.85/0.84, respectively.

Data analysis

First, descriptive statistics (e.g., M, SD, SK, and KU) for the MLQ were performed using SPSS 26.0 (IBM Crop, 2019). Then, a series of CFAs were performed using Mplus 7.0 (Muthén and Muthén, 1998) to examine the LMI across the four time points (over a 12-month period). Given that the values of the skewness and kurtosis in certain items were not within a range of −1 to +1 (e.g., items 1, 2, 8, 9, and 10; see Table 2), the maximum likelihood estimation with a mean-adjusted chi-square (MLM) was robust. The model fits of the CFA were also calculated, including the comparative fit index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), and the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). If the values of CFI and TLI are both higher than 0.90 and RMSEA is lower than 0.08, it indicates an acceptable model fit. If CFI and TLI are above 0.95 and RMSEA is below 0.05, it indicates good model fit (Hu and Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2010).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics for the MLQ scores over time.

Second, the LMI of the MLQ was examined by qualifying the model parameters equality between a series of nested models (i.e., configural, metric, scalar, and strict invariance models). The configural invariance model hypothesis states that the factor structure should follow the same pattern over all time points (Millsap and Cham, 2012). In the second step, the metric invariance model required that the corresponding factor loadings were equal across all time points. Then, in the scalar invariance model, the item intercepts would also be set as equal on the basis of the previous model. Finally, the strict invariance model set the corresponding factor loadings, intercepts, and error variances to be equal across time points (Wang et al., 2012). LMI is achieved (configural, metric, scalar, and strict invariance) when the difference between the unconstrained and constrained models are not significant. Because the chi-square difference test is sensitive to sample size, we examined the changes in CFI, RMSEA, and SRMR to compare the nested models (Chen, 2007). As recommended by Chen (2007), to test the metric invariance model, changes in CFI (ΔCFI) of ≥0.01, supplemented by changes in RMSEA (ΔRMSEA) of ≥0.015 and changes in SRMR (ΔSRMR) of ≥0.03 indicates an absence of MI. When testing the scalar and strict invariance model, ΔCFI of ≥0.01, supplemented by ΔRMSEA of ≥0.015 and ΔSRMR of ≥0.01 indicates an absence of MI.

Third, the internal consistency coefficients of the MLQ at each time point were calculated, including Cronbach’s α, McDonald’s ω, and mean inter-item correlation (MIC). According to Barker et al. (1994), a Cronbach’s α of 0.70 to 0.79 = acceptable, 0.80 to 0.89 = good, and above 0.90 = excellent. A McDonald’s ω greater than 0.70 is acceptable (Hair et al., 2014). Finally, if the MIC value ranges from 0.15 to 0.50 the correlation coefficients should be acceptable (Clark and Watson, 1995). In addition, the stability coefficients were also obtained by testing the correlations between the factors while the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was also calculated for all four time points.

Finally, we explored the differences in the MLQ at the different time points by comparing the latent factor means across time based on LMI. Specifically, the MLQ two-factor means were set at Time 1 to zero, and while the latent factor mean was estimated freely at the other time points (i.e., Times 2, 3, and 4).

Results

Descriptive statistics

Descriptive statistics results for the MLQ scores at the four time points are shown in Table 2, including the mean, standard deviation, skewness, kurtosis, and corrected item-total correlations with each item’s respective factor (CITC).

Factor structure and LMI of the MLQ

As shown in Table 3, the model indices of the MLQ at the four time points achieved acceptable levels (i.e., CFIs and TLIs >0.90, RMSEAs <0.08), with the exception of the TLI at Time 1, which was 0.894. However, this was also suitable for subsequent analysis based on the CFI and RMSEA. A series of nest models were then used to compared the LMI of the MLQ across the time points (i.e., the configural invariance model, the metric invariance model, the scalar invariance model, and the strict invariance model; see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Longitudinal measurement invariance model fit statistics for the MLQ.

First, the configural model had adequate model fit (CFI = 0.937, TLI = 0.925, RMSEA = 0.041). The correlations within and between factors for the model can be found in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Diagram for the longitudinal configural invariance model. P1, presence of MLQ at Time 1; S1, search of MLQ at Time 1; P2, presence of MLQ at Time 2; S2, search of MLQ at Time 2; P3, presence of MLQ at Time 3; S3, search of MLQ at Time 3; P4, presence of MLQ at Time 4; S4, search of MLQ at Time 4.

The metric model fit was also satisfactory (CFI = 0.936, TLI = 0.927, RMSEA = 0.040). There were inappreciable differences in CFI, TLI, SRMR, and RMSEA between the configural and metric model (ΔCFI = −0.001, ΔTLI = 0.002, ΔSRMR = 0.003, ΔRMSEA = −0.001). These findings supported the metric invariance of the MLQ scores across time points. The fit indices of the scalar invariance model (CFI = 0.934, TLI = 0.926, RMSEA = 0.041) were also acceptable. Negligible changes (ΔCFI = −0.002, ΔTLI = −0.001, ΔSRMR = 0.000, ΔRMSEA = 0.001) indicated that scalar invariance was obtained.

Finally, strict invariance (CFI = 0.924, TLI = 0.919, RMSEA = 0.043) was also supported in terms of the change values of CFI, TLI, SRMR, and RMSEA (ΔCFI = −0.010, ΔTLI = −0.007, ΔSRMR = 0.002, ΔRMSEA = 0.002). In summary, our results demonstrated that the two-factor structure of the MLQ scores had LMI over the 1-year interval. The standardized factor loadings of the longitudinal factor solution are presented in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Standardized factor loadings for the longitudinal factor model of the MLQ.

Internal consistency, stability coefficients, and latent factor means comparison over time

The coefficient αs for the MLQ two-factor scores were acceptable (αs > 0.70) at all four time points, separately. As to the MLQ-P factor, the Cronbach’s αs were 0.78 at Time 1 (MIC = 0.43), 0.84 at Time 2 (MIC = 0.54), 0.85 at Time 3 (MIC = 0.55), and 0.85 at Time 4 (MIC = 0.55). The Cronbach’s αs at the four time points were 0.81 (MIC = 0.45), 0.85 (MIC = 0.52), 0.86 (MIC = 0.56), and 0.84 (MIC = 0.51) for the MLQ-S subscale, respectively. The McDonald’s ω results showed that the MLQ scores also had satisfactory internal consistency values. More specifically, for the MLQ-P subscale, the ωs were 0.79, 0.86, 0.86, and 0.86 at the four time points, respectively; for the MLQ-S subscale, the ωs were all above 0.80 (i.e., 0.81 at Time 1, 0.85 at Time 2, 0.86 at Time 3, and 0.84 at Time 4). Moreover, the results of the stability coefficients between Time 1, Time 2, Time 3, and Time 4 ranged from 0.64 to 0.76 for MLQ-P, and 0.43 to 0.67 for MLQ-S (ps < 0.001). The results of the intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) were 0.62 for MLQ-P and 0.51 for MLQ-S (ps < 0.001). Finally, using the strict longitudinal invariance model, the mean of each factor at each of the four different time points could be compared meaningfully. More specifically, the latent mean was not significantly different between Time 1 and Time 2 (mean difference = 0.043, p = 0.344), Time 1 and Time 3 (mean difference = 0.038, p = 0.442), and Time 1 and Time 4 (mean difference = 0.089, p = 0.088) for MLQ-P. For the MLQ-S subscale, taking Time 1 as a reference, Time 2, Time 3, and Time 4 had significant differences in comparison to their latent means (i.e., mean difference = −0.194/−0.289/−0.453, ps < 0.001).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to examine the longitudinal properties of the MLQ (Steger et al., 2006), which was developed to assess the presence of and search for meaning in life, in a sample of Chinese college students. The results showed that the MLQ and its two subscales have strict longitudinal invariance over a 12-month interval (i.e., equality of factor patterns, factor loadings, item intercepts, and item uniqueness for all 10 items across a 1-year interval). Meanwhile, the internal consistencies and stability coefficients also supported the MLQ scores across time. Overall, our findings replicate and extend the findings of previous studies on the psychometric properties of the MLQ and, for the first time, validate the longitudinal properties of the MLQ across time.

Longitudinal measurement invariance of the MLQ

Longitudinal measurement invariance was used to evaluate whether the same factor structure could be verified at different points in time (Wu, 2017). This is essential because the structure of meaning of life could develop or change as the time goes by (Steger et al., 2009). In other words, it is important that changes over time can be attributed to actual differences or individual developments (Dimitrov, 2010; Millsap and Cham, 2012). The aim of the present study was therefore to examine the LMI of the MLQ two-factor model. The results of this study showed that the MLQ had strict MI across time, which extends previous findings that measured invariance across gender and age groups (e.g., Damásio and Koller, 2015; Wang et al., 2016). While prior research has focused on MI across groups, the present study focused on MI across different time points. Our results indicated that meaning in life in Chinese college students had strict LMI over time (i.e., configural, metric, scalar, and strict invariance), at least in individuals aged 18 to 24 over an interval of a year. Consistent with Steger et al. (2009), meaning in life was shown to be stable over the course of a year, and was independent of other forms of happiness (Steger, 2009). This suggests that the MLQ was able to measure the same structure at the different time points, and that the two-factor structure in this study was good, consistent with previous studies. Importantly, this implies that the mean difference in meaning in life scores as measured through the MLQ over time can be considered as true changes in an individual’s meaning in life. However, given that the MI across age was inconsistent, future examinations of LMI of the MLQ should test the presented findings against other age group populations (e.g., younger adolescents or older adults).

Internal consistency, stable coefficients, and latent factor means over time

The internal consistency coefficients also offered some meaningful information about the stability of MLQ scoring over time. Similar to previous findings (e.g., Negri et al., 2020), the coefficient αs of the MLQ factor scores were acceptable over time. Furthermore, the McDonald’s ω and MIC values also suggest that the MLQ has satisfactory and acceptable internal consistency across time.

As to the stability coefficient, the MLQ latent factor scores across the four time points were significantly correlated (i.e., rs ranged from 0.43 to 0.76). Moreover, the ICC indices showed that the stability coefficient for the MLQ subscale scores were moderate (Weir, 2005). Consistent with manifest factor correlations (Steger and Kashdan, 2007), the moderate to strong correlation between the latent factors as well as the ICC values suggested that meaning in life is at least moderately stable over a 1-year period (Steger and Kashdan, 2007).

As the LMI results of the MLQ were verified, the differences of the latent factor means could be compared for further exploration. Using Time 1 as a baseline for comparison, the latent means difference of the MLQ-P at Time 2, Time 3, and Time 4 were not significant (ps > 0.05). In contrast, the latent means difference of the MLQ-S at Time 2, Time 3, and Time 4 showed significant differences (ps < 0.001) when compared with Time 1. These results are contrary to the findings of Hsiao et al. (2013), who found significant differences in MLQ-P mean values over a 14-month follow-up period, but no differences in MLQ-S, even though meaning in life was not the primary variable of concern in the study. We speculate that one reason for these opposite results could be due to the differences in sample sampling. In our study, the samples comprised college students, while in the Hsiao study, the subjects were breast cancer survivors. Changes in the MLQ-S could also have been caused by different cognitive styles. Steger et al. (2008) showed that their findings supported with Maddi’s 1970 theory that search for meaning was associated with different cognitive styles, which were characterized by a tendency to question the status quo and continuous negative thinking about the past and present. Therefore, people may perceive reality in different ways over time intervals of more than a year, which may have implications on the search for meaning in life.

Limitations and future directions

The findings from the present study must be considered with consideration of several limitations. First, the participants in the current study came from Southwest China, which may limit the generalizability of our results. Future research should consider including university students from other regions when attempting to replicate the current findings. Second, as the sample size and gender ratio was too broad, the current study did not measure the LMI of the MLQ in different gender or age populations. Future studies should extend to include such an exploration when measuring LMI in the context of balancing gender and population proportions from the various age groups. Third, we only examined the LMI of the MLQ scores at 3-month intervals between data collection; future studies should test the LMI of the MLQ over longer time intervals. Finally, the current study examined the LMI of the MLQ in college students (i.e., emerging adulthood). Future research should test the LMI of the MLQ in other demographics (e.g., community adults or younger adolescents).

Despite these limitations, the present study expands our understanding of the longitudinal properties of MLQ scores. Overall, the current study proves that the MLQ can be an effective measure to assess meaning in life of Chinese college students across time. Specifically, the MLQ was shown to have strict LMI (including configural invariance, metric invariance, scalar invariance, and strict invariance) across a 1-year interval. Future study should further explore this property of the MLQ.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the first author, JL, upon reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Subjects Review Committee at Guizhou Normal University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

JL was mainly responsible for the conception and design of this study, investigated and analyzed the data, drafted the manuscript, and provided the final approval for the manuscript. F-CT contributed to the analysis and interpretation of the data, drafted the manuscript, and provided the final approval for the manuscript. RY helped revise the manuscript and provided edits for the revision. JG and C-KY helped revise the manuscript. XH helped guide the revision and provided financial support. WC and S-YZ revised the manuscript, provided financial support, and provided the final approval for the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was funded by the Guizhou Philosophy and Social Science Planning General Project (18GZYB57), Guizhou Philosophy and Social Science Planning Key Project (21GZZD51), and Guizhou Province Science and Technology Foundation (Qian Ke He Jichu-ZK [2022] General 303).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that this research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as potential conflicts of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

Akin, A., and Taş, İ. (2015). Meaning in life questionnaire: A study of validity and reliability. Electronic Turkish Studies 10, 27–36. doi: 10.7827/TurkishStusies.7860

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aquino, T. A. A. D., Veloso, V. G., Aguiar, A. A. D., Serafim, T. D. B., Pontes, A. D. M., Pereira, G. D. A., et al. (2015). Meaning in life questionnaire: Evidence of its factor validity. Psicologia: Ciência e Profissão 35, 4–19. doi: 10.1590/1982-3703001332012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Balgiu, B. A. (2020). Meaning in life questionnaire: factor structure and gender invariance in a Romanian undergraduates sample. Revista Românească Pentru Educaţie Multidimensională 12, 132–147. doi: 10.18662/rrem/12.2/270

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barker, C., Pistrang, N., and Elliot, R. (1994). Research Methods in Clinical and Counselling Psychology Chichester: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Battista, J., and Almond, R. (1973). The development of meaning in life. Psychiatry 36, 409–427. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1973.11023774

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandstätter, M., Baumann, U., Borasio, G. D., and Fegg, M. J. (2012). Systematic review of meaning in life assessment instruments. Psychol. Oncol. 21, 1034–1052. doi: 10.1002/pon.2113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Byrne, B. M., and Watkins, D. (2003). The issue of measurement invariance revisited. J. Cross-Cult. Psychol. 34, 155–175. doi: 10.1177/002202210225

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Çelik, N. D., and Gazioğlu, E. İ. (2015). Meaning in life questionnaire high school form: Turkish validity and reliability. Mehmet Akif Ersoy University Journal of Education Faculty 33, 42–60.

Google Scholar

Chan, W. C. H. (2014). Factor structure of the Chinese version of the meaning in life questionnaire among Hong Kong Chinese caregivers. Health Soc. Work 39, 135–143. doi: 10.1093/hsw/hlu025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2007). Sensitivity of goodness of fit indexes to lack of measurement invariance. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 14, 464–504. doi: 10.1080/10705510701301834

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, F. F. (2008). What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 95, 1005–1018. doi: 10.1037/a0013193

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, W., Ge, Y., Hu, Y., and Zhang, J. (2015). Applicability and generalizability of the revised meaning in life questionnaire: based on classical test theory and multidimensional rasch model. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 23, 604–607. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2015.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., Wang, X., He, X., Ji, L., Liu, M., and Ye, B. (2020). The relationship between search for meaning in life and symptoms of depression and anxiety: key roles of the presence of meaning in life and life events among Chinese adolescents. J. Affect. Disord. 282, 545–553. doi: 10.1016/J.JAD.2020.12.156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chika, C. J., Ekpedoho, E. A., Ifeagwazi, C. M., Iorfa, S. K., and Nwonyi, S. K. (2019). Psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire–Hausa version among internally displaced persons in Nigeria. Transcult. Psychiatry 56, 103–122. doi: 10.1177/1363461518794218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L. A., and Watson, D. (1995). Constructing validity: basic issues in objective scale development. Psychol. Assess. 7, 309–319. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.7.3.309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crop, IBM (2019). IBM SPSS for Windows, Version 26.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Crop.

Google Scholar

Crumbaugh, J. C. (1969). Manual of Instruction for the Purpose-in-Life Test. Munster: Psychometric Affiliates.

Google Scholar

Damásio, B. F., and Koller, S. H. (2015). Meaning in life questionnaire: adaptation process and psychometric properties of the Brazilian version. Revista Latinoamericana De Psicología 47, 185–195. doi: 10.1016/j.rlp.2015.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Datu, J. A. D., and Yuen, M. (2022). Factorial validity of meaning in life questionnaire in Hong Kong secondary school students: a construct validation approach. Couns. Psychol. Q. 35, 467–480. doi: 10.1080/09515070.2021.1875989

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demirdağ, S., and Kalafat, S. (2015). Meaning in life questionnaire (MLQ): the study of adaptation to Turkish, validity and reliability. J. Fac. Educ. 16, 83–95. doi: 10.17679/iuefd.16250801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dimitrov, D. M. (2010). Testing for factorial invariance in the context of construct validation. Meas. Eval. Couns. Dev. 43, 121–149. doi: 10.1177/0748175610373459

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frankl, V. E. (1963). Man’s Search for Meaning: An Introduction to Logotherapy. New York: Washington Square Press.

Google Scholar

Glaw, X., Kable, A., Hazelton, M., and Inder, K. (2016). Meaning in life and meaning of life in mental health care: an integrative literature review. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 38, 243–252. doi: 10.1080/01612840.2016.1253804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Góngora, V., and Grinhauz, A. S. (2011). Validación del cuestionario MLQ de sentido de la vida en población adolescente de la ciudad de Buenos Aires. Memorias, 119–120.

Google Scholar

Góngora, V., and Solano, A. C. (2011). Validación del cuestionario de significado de la vida MLQ en población adulta y adolescente argentina. Revista Interamericana de Psicología/Interamerican Journal of Psychology 45, 395–404.

Google Scholar

Hair, J. F., Black, W. C., Babin, B. J., and Anderson, R. E. (2014). Multivariate Data Analysis. 7nd Edn London: Pearson Education Limited.

Google Scholar

Hsiao, F. H., Chang, K. J., Kuo, W. H., Huang, C. S., Liu, Y. F., Lai, Y. M., et al. (2013). A longitudinal study of cortisol responses, sleep problems, and psychological well-being as the predictors of changes in depressive symptoms among breast cancer survivors. Psychoneuroendocrinology 38, 356–366. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2012.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, L. A., and Hicks, J. A. (2021). The science of meaning in life. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 72, 561–584. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-072420-122921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

King, L. A., Hicks, J. A., Krull, J. L., and Del Gaiso, A. K. (2006). Positive affect and the experience of meanign in life. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 90, 179–196. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.90.1.179

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kline, R. B. (2010). Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling (3rd Edn.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Kolesovs, A. (2019). Adaptation of the meaning in life questionnaire in Latvian. Social Sciences Bulletin/Socialo Zinatnu Vestnesis 1, 109–120.

Google Scholar

Kossakowska, M., Kwiatek, P., and Stefaniak, T. (2013). Sens w życiu. Polska wersja kwestionariusza MLQ (Meaning in Life Questionnaire) [Meaning in Life. Polish Version of MLQ]. Psychologia Jakości Życia 12, 111–131. doi: 10.5604/16441796.1090786

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, S. S., and Gan, Y. Q. (2010). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the meaning in life questionnaire. Chin. Ment. Health J. 24, 478–482. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2010.06.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macià, D., Cattaneo, G., Solana, J., Tormos, J. M., Pascual-Leone, A., and Bartrés-Faz, D. (2021). Meaning in life: a major predictive factor for loneliness comparable to health status and social connectedness. Front. Psychol. 12, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.627547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McGregor, I., and Little, B. R. (1998). Personal projects, happiness, and meaning: on doing well and being yourself. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 74, 494–512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.494

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Millsap, R. E., and Cham, H. (2012). “Investigating factorial invariance in longitudinal data,” in Handbook of Development Research Methods. eds. B. Laursen, T. D. Little, and N. A. Card (New York, NY: Guildford), 265–278.

Google Scholar

Muthén, L., and Muthén, B. (1998). Mplus User’s Guide 7th Edn. Los Angeles. CA: Muthén and Muthén.

Google Scholar

Muthén, L. K., and Muthén, B. O. (2002). How to use a Monte Carlo study to decide on sample size and determine power. Struct. Equ. Mod. 9, 599–620. doi: 10.1207/S15328007SEM0904_8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Naghiyaee, M., Bahmani, B., and Asgari, A. (2020). The psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire (MLQ) in patients with life-threatening illnesses. Sci. World J. 2020, 1–8. doi: 10.1155/2020/8361602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Negri, L., Bassi, M., and Fave, D. A. (2020). Italian validation of the meaning in life questionnaire: factor structure, reliability, convergent, and discriminant validity. Psychol. Rep. 123, 578–600. doi: 10.1177/0033294118821302

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Negru-Subtirica, O., Pop, E. I., Luyckx, K., Dezutter, J., and Steger, M. F. (2016). The meaningful identity: a longitudinal look at the interplay between identity and meaning in life in adolescence. Dev. Psychol. 52, 1926–1936. doi: 10.1037/dev0000176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., Knott, C. L., Williams, R. M., Clark, E. M., Williams, B. R., and Schulz, E. (2020). Meaning in life predicts decreased depressive symptoms and increased positive affect over time but does not buffer stress effects in a national sample of African-Americans. J. Happiness Stud. 21, 3037–3049. doi: 10.1007/s10902-019-00212-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reker, G. T., and Cousins, J. B. (1979). Factor structure, construct validity and reliability of the seeking of noetic goals (SONG) and purpose in life (PIL) tests. J. Clin. Psychol. 35, 85–91. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(197901)35:1<85::AID-JCLP2270350110>3.0.CO;2-R

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rose, L. M., Zask, A., and Burton, L. J. (2017). Psychometric properties of the meaning in life questionnaire (MLQ) in a sample of Australian adolescents. International Journal of Adolescence and Youth 22, 68–77. doi: 10.1080/02673843.2015.1124791

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryff, C. D., and Singer, B. (2010). Psychological well-being: meaning, measurement, and implications for psychotherapy research. Psychother. Psychosom. 65, 14–23. doi: 10.1159/000289026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salami, M. O., Khan, R., and Yusuf, M. (2022). The mediating role of meaning in the relationship between depression and hope. Int. J. Psychol. Educ. Stud. 9, 368–375. doi: 10.52380/ijpes.2022.9.2.612

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satoshi, S., and Kohki, M. F. S. (2019). Meaning in life across developmental stages among Japanese adults: scores and correlates. J. Health Psychol. Res. 32, 1–11. doi: 10.11560/jhpr.181206118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shek, S. T., Chai, C. W. Y., and Dou, D. Y. (2021). Parenting factors and meaning of life among Chinese adolescents: a six-wave longitudinal study. J. Adolesc. 87, 117–132. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2021.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shiah, Y. J., Chang, F., Chiang, S. K., Lin, I. M., and Tam, W. C. C. (2015). Religion and health: anxiety, religiosity, meaning of life and mental health. J. Relig. Health 54, 35–45. doi: 10.1007/s10943-013-9781-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, K., Junnarkar, M., Jaswal, S., and Kaur, J. (2016). Validation of meaning in life questionnaire in Hindi (MLQ-H). Mental Health, Religion & Culture 19, 448–458. doi: 10.1080/13674676.2016.1189759

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stalikas, A., Kyriazos, T. A., Yotsidi, V., and Prassa, K. (2018). Using bifactor EFA, bifactor CFA and exploratory structural equation modeling to validate factor structure of the meaning in life questionnaire Greek version. Psychology 9, 348–371. doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.93022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F. (2009). “Meaning in life,” in The Oxford Handbool of Positive Psychology. 2nd Edn. eds. S. J. Lopez and C. R. Snyder (Oxford, United Kingdom: Oxford University Press), 679–687. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195187243.013.0064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Frazier, P., Oishi, S., and Kaler, M. (2006). The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J. Couns. Psychol. 53, 80–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., and Kashdan, T. B. (2007). Stability and specificity of meaning in life and life satisfaction over one year. J. Happiness Stud. 8, 161–179. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9011-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Kashdan, T. B., Sullivan, B. A., and Lorentz, D. (2008). Understanding the search for meaning in life: personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. J. Pers. 76, 199–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steger, M. F., Oishi, S., and Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Meaning in life across the life span: levels and correlates of meaning in life from emerging adulthood to older adulthood. J. Posit. Psychol. 4, 43–52. doi: 10.1080/17439760802303127

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vela, J. C., Karaman, M. A., Garcia, C., and Aguilar, A. (2020). Evaluation of Spanish versions of meaning in life and hope measures with Latinx students. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development 53, 104–116. doi: 10.1080/07481756.2019.1667246

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M., and Dai, X. (2008). Chinese meaning in life questionnaire revised in college students and its reliability and validity test. Chin. J. Clin. Psych. 16, 459–461. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2008.05.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, M.-C., Elhai, J. D., Dai, X., and Yao, S. (2012). Longitudinal invariance of posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms in adolescent earthquake survivors. J. Anxiety Disord. 26, 263–270. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X., He, X., Fan, Y., and Zhang, D. (2016). Structure and levels of meaning in life and its relationship with mental health in Chinese students aged 10 to 25. J. Pac. Rim Psychol. 10:e10. doi: 10.1017/prp.2016.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weir, J. P. (2005). Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. J. Strength Cond. Res. 19, 231–240. doi: 10.1519/15184.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilchek-Aviad, Y. (2015). Meaning in life and suicidal tendency among immigrant (Ethiopian) youth and native-born Israeli youth. J. Immigr. Minor. Health 17, 1041–1048. doi: 10.1007/s10903-014-0028-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, P. C. (2017). Longitudinal measurement invariance of beck depression inventory–II in early adolescents. Assessment 24, 337–345. doi: 10.1177/1073191115608941

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: longitudinal measurement invariance, meaning in life, presence of meaning in life, search for meaning in life, MLQ, Chinese college students

Citation: Luo J, Tang F-C, Yang R, Gong J, Yao C-K, Huang X, Chen W and Zhao S-Y (2022) Longitudinal measurement invariance of the meaning in life questionnaire in Chinese college students. Front. Psychol. 13:1001548. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1001548

Received: 23 July 2022; Accepted: 29 September 2022;
Published: 25 October 2022.

Edited by:

Chee-Seng Tan, Universiti Tunku Abdul Rahman, Malaysia

Reviewed by:

Wanderson Silva, Federal University of Alfenas, Brazil
Snehlata Jaswal, Chaudhary Charan Singh University, India

Copyright © 2022 Luo, Tang, Yang, Gong, Yao, Huang, Chen and Zhao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xinquan Huang, 576749119@qq.com; Wei Chen, chenweihb@yeah.net; Shuo-Ying Zhao, zhanshouying@126.com

These authors share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.