Commentary on Pothos

Word Counts:
Abstract: 66 words
Main Text: 893 words
References: 162 words
Total Text: 1121 words

Processing is shaped by multiple tasks: There is more to rules and similarity than Rules-to-Similarity

Gary Lupyan
Department of Psychology and the Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition
Carnegie Mellon University
5000 Forbes Ave. Baker Hall, 342C
Pittsburgh, PA 15213
USA
412-258-8112
glupyan@cnbc.cmu.edu
http://www.cnbc.cmu.edu/~glupyan

Gautam Vallabha
Center for the Neural Basis of Cognition
Carnegie Mellon University
4400 Fifth Ave. 115 Mellon Institute
Pittsburgh, PA, 15213
USA
412-268-4115
vallabha@condor.cnbc.cmu.edu




Abstract: We argue that the Rules-Similarity continuum is only a useful formalism for particular, isolated tasks, and must rest on the assumption that representations formed during a particular task are independent of other tasks. We show this to be an unrealistic conjecture. We additionally point out that describing categorization as selective weighing and abstracting of features misses the important step of discovering what the possible features are.




We applaud Pothos' push for a unitary understanding of rules and similarity and agree with the general idea that rules operations may be reducible to similarity ones. We find the main appeal of the Rules-Similarity view to be its theoretical parsimony—it attempts to unify disparate views of cognitive processing using a single descriptive formalism. However, we have two concerns with this particular approach. The first concern is that the R-S classification cannot be applied to an entire domain of related tasks. For example, it makes no sense to ask where lexical processing is on the Rules-Similarity (henceforth R-S) continuum—some lexical tasks (such as word inflection) may imply a Rule-like process, while others (such as contextual priming) may imply a Similarity-based process.

One alternative is to assume that while R-S classifications of different lexical tasks influence each other, the lexical system as a whole employs a common blend of Rule and Similarity operations. However, such an appeal to a domain holism mars the theoretical attractiveness of the R-S formalism. Even if subjects' behavior on a given task is Rule-like, we cannot assume this is caused by Rules operations because the underlying processes are assumed to be shaped by the ensemble of tasks. Thinking in terms of the R-S continuum is most useful when applied to domains where the tasks are relatively independent (or compartmentalized) from each other. The assumption of task independence further requires that the representations employed by the different tasks be independent of each other, otherwise, the R-S classifications of the different tasks can influence each other via the shared representation.

So, is the assumption of narrow tasks a viable one? We argue that it is not. For instance, while the syntax-level representation needs only to represent aspects of the speech signal relevant to syntactic tasks, the existence of a representation in its pure form (a restatement of autonomy of syntax), is doubtful. The following example illustrates the problem. Autonomy of syntax predicts that that the syntactic representation of sentences (1) and (2), would be the same given their identical structure. This is clearly not the case considering the alternative clause attachment suggested by the semantics of "saw" versus "shot" (McClelland et al., 1989).

Semantic knowledge also influences morphology. In performing a past-tense judgment, people are sensitive to context, inflecting a nonce word frink as frinked if its meaning is closer to blink and as frank if its meaning is closer to drink (Ramscar, 2002). In addition, the preferred past tense inflection of a word is related to the frequency of its phonological use, that is, the morphology and phonology mutually constrain each other (Burzio, 2002). In speech perception, the phonemic and talker characteristics of an utterance (putatively, two separate tasks) are not in fact separate—listeners can identify words more reliably when they are familiar with the speaker's voice (Nygaard & Pisoni, 1998). Similarly, the phonemic classification of a vowel is influenced by global characteristics of the utterance (Ladefoged & Broadbent, 1957). Finally, the perceptual learning of new categories often affects the discrimination behavior (Goldstone, 1998; Guenther et al., 1999).

In summary, then, we feel that at least the language domain is composed of many tasks that are not independent of each other. Since representations are generally shaped by their use in multiple tasks, it is meaningless either to assign a single R-S classification to the entire task domain, or to assign a separate R-S classification to each task.

Our second concern is that the R-S classification may prove inadequate even in those domains where tasks may be independent of each other. Consider a perceptual skill such as wine tasting, or a cognitive skill such as playing chess, both of which require the learner to transform the perceptual domain into one with dimensions useful for categorization. The R-S approach assumes an array of object properties is readily available, and that the task faced by the cognitive system is to map from this high-dimensional space of object properties to a low-dimensional space of object categories. This might be valid for objects such as "red circle" and "blue square," but how about a musician learning to "pick out" an instrument in a symphony? She cannot accomplish the task by weighting different aspects of the raw acoustic input, since the acoustic signatures of the instruments overlap both in time and in the frequency spectrum. Rather, she needs to discover how to transform the acoustic information into a more manageable space—to discover the array of object properties (Schyns et al., 1998). With expertise, the transformation may become more reliable and robust, and the musician may only depend on a few of the properties. However, characterizing this operation as Rule-like elides the vital role of the initial transformation.

The above two concerns suggest to us that a single Rules-Similarity continuum is not sufficient to capture the complex interrelation of tasks, at least in the domains of language and perception. This insufficiency, we argue, is partly due to the emphasis on the categorization task. Objects within a domain (e.g. words and grammatical constructs in language) are not simply classified but are means towards larger ecological goals. Both Rule-like and Similarity-like operations may be concurrently recruited in order to achieve a particular goal.

References

Burzio, L. (2002). Missing Players: Phonology and the Past-tense Debate. Lingua 112, 157-199.

Goldstone, R. (1998). Perceptual learning. Annual Review of Psychology 49, 585-612.

Guenther, F. H., Husain, F. T., Cohen, M. A., & Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. (1999). Effects of categorization and discrimination training on auditory perceptual space. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 106, 2900-2912.

Ladefoged, P., & Broadbent, D. E. (1957). Information conveyed by vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 29, 98-104.

McClelland, J. L., St. John, M., & Taraban, R. (1989). Sentence comprehension: A parallel distributed processing approach. Language and Cognitive Processes, 4, 287-335

Nygaard, L. C., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Talker-specific learning in speech perception. Perception & Psychophysics 60, 355-376.

Ramscar, M.(2002) The role of meaning in inflection: Why the past tense does not require a rule. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 45-94.

Schyns, P. G., Goldstone, R. L., & Thibaut, J-P. (1998). The development of features in object concepts. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, 1-54.




Acknowledgements

We thank Jay McClelland, David Plaut, and members of the Carnegie Mellon PDP group for useful discussion related to this commentary.