Skip to main content
Log in

Why implicit attitudes are (probably) not beliefs

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Should we understand implicit attitudes on the model of belief? I argue that implicit attitudes are (probably) members of a different psychological kind altogether, because they seem to be insensitive to the logical form of an agent’s thoughts and perceptions. A state is sensitive to logical form only if it is sensitive to the logical constituents of the content of other states (e.g., operators like negation and conditional). I explain sensitivity to logical form and argue that it is a necessary condition for belief. I appeal to two areas of research that seem to show that implicit attitudes fail spectacularly to satisfy this condition—although persistent gaps in the empirical literature leave matters inconclusive. I sketch an alternative account, according to which implicit attitudes are sensitive merely to spatiotemporal relations in thought and perception, i.e., the spatial and temporal orders in which people think, see, or hear things.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. This case is based on Rydell et al. (2006), which measured the influence of subliminal conditioning on a timed association task (the Implicit Association Test) but not on more ecologically valid behaviors (e.g., Kawakami et al. 2007a, b).

  2. Sommers (2009) writes, “To believe is to take something to be so and so... animal and human belief is mainly... propositionless” (pp. 269, 270).

  3. This “descriptive” approach differs from the “normative” understanding of logical form, as the idealized structure of sentences or thoughts. Here logical form refers to properties of concrete entities, not idealized abstractions.

  4. Thanks to two referees for noting these possibilities. See Brownstein (2015), Huebner (forthcoming), Gendler (2008a, b), Leslie (forthcoming) for various ways implicit attitudes might be structured.

  5. What role does the agent play in such psychological transitions? Which sensitivities and abilities must an agent, or a cognitive system, have for these transitions to take place? I remain neutral about these questions, which require a separate treatment. My focus is on the properties of certain states within the cognitive system.

  6. Critics of “wide-scope” interpretations of rationality point to asymmetries between ways of resolving inconsistency (e.g., Kolodny 2005). If Madeleine intends to drink a beer and believes beer is in the fridge, it seems better, rationally speaking, to resolve the situation by going to the fridge to get the beer than by abandoning her belief that there is beer in the fridge. But both responses reflect form-sensitivity.

  7. In (4) and (4*), Madeleine is a little better off than a broken clock, perhaps more like the frog who endlessly laps its tongue at things that look like flies and never learns any better (Fodor 1990; Gendler 2008b).

  8. Satisfaction of this condition might require that the agent be equally familiar with the two distinct formulations, but it is not clear how much prior familiarity is necessary. A lot of bad, all-too-easily intelligible poetry rearranges words in this sort of way. Garbled as his syntax may be, Master Yoda’s sage advice is easy to understand (“Strong is Vader. Mind what you have learned. Save you it can!”).

  9. See my (Madva, forthcoming) for further discussion of cognitive accessibility and the mechanisms underlying implementation intentions, from which this summary borrows.

  10. Thanks to an anonymous referee for emphasizing this point.

  11. Thanks to Katie Gasdaglis for this suggestion.

  12. See also Horcajo et al.’s (2010) findings that persuasive arguments influenced implicit attitudes toward vegetables and brands. See Levy (2014a, b) and de Houwer (2011, 2014) for surveys of other bbc-relevant studies.

  13. Gawronski and Bodenhausen (2011, Sect. 2.1.2) summarize several potential pathways of indirect influence, but do not discuss the strategy I highlight below.

References

  • Agerström, J., & Rooth, D. O. (2011). The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96(4), 790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1946). Forming impressions of personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 259–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2007). Working memory, thought, and action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bermúdez, J. L. (2006). Animal reasoning and proto-logic. In S. Hurley & M. Nudds (Eds.), Rational animals? (pp. 127–137). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & McCaslin, M. (2009). Changing attitudes on implicit versus explicit measures: What is the difference (pp. 285–326). Attitudes: Insights from the new implicit measures.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownstein, M. (2015). Implicit bias. In: Edward N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/spr2015/entries/implicit-bias/.

  • Brownstein, M., & Madva, A. (2012a). Ethical automaticity. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 42(1), 67–97.

  • Brownstein, M., & Madva, A. (2012b). The normativity of automaticity. Mind and Language, 27(4), 410–434.

  • Calanchini, J., Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., & Klauer, K. C. (2013). Counter-prejudicial training reduces activation of biased associations and enhances response monitoring. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(5), 321–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, J., & Armitage, C. J. (2010). Evidence that boosters augment the long-term impact of implementation intentions on fruit and vegetable intake. Psychology and Health, 25(3), 365–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Dreu, C. K., Greer, L. L., Van Kleef, G. A., Shalvi, S., & Handgraaf, M. J. (2011). Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(4), 1262–1266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2011). Evaluative conditioning: A review of procedure knowledge and mental process theories. In T. R. Schachtman & S. S. Reilly (Eds.), Associative learning and conditioning theory: Human and non-human applications (pp. 399–416). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2014). A propositional model of implicit evaluations. Social and Personality Compass, 8(7), 342–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, R., & Strack, F. (2010). Building blocks of social behavior: Reflective and impulsive processes. In B. Gawronski & B. K. Payne (Eds.), Handbook of implicit social cognition: Measurement, theory, and applications (pp. 62–79). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. J., & Cox, W. T. (2012). Long-term reduction in implicit race bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 1267–1278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eberl, C., Wiers, R. W., Pawelczack, S., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2013). Approach bias modification in alcohol dependence: Do clinical effects replicate and for whom does it work best? Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 38–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1990). A theory of content and other essays. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forbes, C. E., & Schmader, T. (2010). Retraining attitudes and stereotypes to affect motivation and cognitive capacity under stereotype threat. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frankish, K. (Forthcoming). Implicit bias, dual process, and metacognitive motivation. In Brownstein & Saul (Eds.), Implicit Bias & Philosophy: Volume I, Metaphysics and Epistemology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2011). The associative-propositional evaluation model: Theory, evidence, and open questions. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gawronski, B., Deutsch, R., Mbirkou, S., Seibt, B., & Strack, F. (2008). When “Just Say No” is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 370–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendler, T. S. (2008a). Alief and belief. The Journal of Philosophy, 105(10), 634–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gendler, T. S. (2008b). Alief in action (and reaction). Mind and Language, 23(5), 552–585.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gertler, B. (2011). Self-knowledge and the transparency of belief. In A. Hatzimoysis (Ed.), Self-knowledge. Oxford: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. (2008). Implicit motivation to control prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 164–172.

  • Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation intentions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and processes. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 69–119). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Govorun, O., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Ego-depletion and prejudice: Separating automatic and controlled components. Social Cognition, 24(2), 111–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Han, H. A., Czellar, S., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2010). Malleability of attitudes or malleability of the IAT? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 286–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harman, G. (1970). Deep structure as logical form. Synthese, 21(3–4), 275–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henley, N. M., Miller, M., & Beazley, J. A. (1995). Syntax, semantics, and sexual violence agency and the passive voice. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 14(1–2), 60–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horcajo, J., Briñol, P., & Petty, R. E. (2010). Consumer persuasion: Indirect change and implicit balance. Psychology & Marketing, 27(10), 938–963.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, B. (2009). Trouble with stereotypes for Spinozan minds. Philosophy of the Social Sciences, 39, 63–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huebner, B. (Forthcoming). Implicit bias, reinforcement learning, and scaffolded moral cognition. In Brownstein & Saul (Eds.), Implicit bias & philosophy: Volume I, metaphysics and epistemology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Hunter, D. (2011). Alienated belief. Dialectica, 65(2), 221–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karg, B., & Young, J. K. (2009). 101 secrets of the Freemason: The truth behind the world’s most secret society. Avon, MA: Adams Media.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., Moll, J., Hermsen, S., & Russin, A. (2000). Just say no (to stereotyping): Effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 871–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Dovidio, J. F., & van Kamp, S. (2007a). The impact of counterstereotypic training and related correction processes on the application of stereotypes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10(2), 139–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kawakami, K., Phills, C. E., Steele, J. R., & Dovidio, J. F. (2007b). (Close) distance makes the heart grow fonder: Improving implicit racial attitudes and interracial interactions through approach behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(6), 957–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolodny, N. (2005). Why be rational? Mind, 114, 509–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwong, J. M. C. (2012). Resisting aliefs: Gendler on alief-discordant behaviors. Philosophical Psychology, 25(1), 77–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepore, E., & Ludwig, K. (2002). What is logical form? Logical Form and Language, 54, 90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, S. (Forthcoming). The original sin of cognition: Fear, prejudice, and generalization. The Journal of Philosophy.

  • Levy, N. (2014a). Consciousness, implicit attitudes, and moral responsibility. Noûs, 48(1), 21–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, N. (2014b). Neither fish nor fowl: Implicit attitudes as patchy endorsements. Noûs. doi:10.1111/nous.12074.

  • Lowery, B. S., Hardin, C. D., & Sinclair, S. (2001). Social influence effects on automatic racial prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madva, A. (2012). The hidden mechanisms of prejudice: Implicit bias and interpersonal fluency. PhD dissertation, Columbia University.

  • Madva, A. (Forthcoming). Virtue, social Knowledge, and implicit bias. In Brownstein & Saul (Eds.), Implicit Bias & Philosophy: Volume I, Metaphysics and Epistemology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  • Madva, A., & Brownstein, M. (under review). The blurry boundary between stereotyping and evaluation in implicit cognition.

  • Mandelbaum, E. (2013). Against alief. Philosophical Studies, 165(1), 197–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandelbaum, E. (2014). Attitude. Inference, association: On the propositional structure of implicit bias. Noûs. doi:10.1111/nous.12089.

  • Mendoza, S. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Amodio, D. M. (2010). Reducing the expression of implicit stereotypes: Reflexive control through implementation intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(4), 512–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32(02), 183–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moran, T., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2013). The effect of object–valence relations on automatic evaluation. Cognition & Emotion, 27(4), 743–752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pearson, A. R., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2009). The nature of contemporary prejudice: Insights from aversive racism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rooth, D. O. (2010). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: Real world evidence. Labour Economics, 17(3), 523–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rowbottom, D. P. (2007). ‘In-between believing’ and degrees of belief. Teorema, 26, 131–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Mackie, D. M., & Strain, L. M. (2006). Of two minds: Forming and changing valence-inconsistent implicit and explicit attitudes. Psychological Science, 17(11), 954–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Strain, L. M., Claypool, H. M., & Hugenberg, K. (2007). Implicit and explicit attitudes respond differently to increasing amounts of counterattitudinal information. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(5), 867–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwitzgebel, E. (2010). Acting contrary to our professed beliefs, or the gulf between occurrent judgment and dispositional belief. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 91, 531–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sherman, S. J., Rose, J. S., Koch, K., Presson, C. C., & Chassin, L. (2003). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward cigarette smoking: The effects of context and motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 22(1), 13–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. T., & De Houwer, J. (2014). The impact of persuasive messages on IAT performance is moderated by source attractiveness and likeability. Social Psychology, 45(6), 437–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sommers, F. (2009). Dissonant beliefs. Analysis, 69(2), 267–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanley, J. (2000). Context and logical form. Linguistics and Philosophy, 23(4), 391–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terbeck, S., Kahane, G., McTavish, S., Savulescu, J., Cowen, P. J., & Hewstone, M. (2012). Propranolol reduces implicit negative racial bias. Psychopharmacology, 222(3), 419–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walther, E., Weil, R., & Düsing, J. (2011). The role of evaluative conditioning in attitude formation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 192–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implementation intention effects: The role of goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 373–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webb, T. L., Sheeran, P., & Pepper, J. (2012). Gaining control over responses to implicit attitude tests: Implementation intentions engender fast responses on attitude-incongruent trials. British Journal of Social Psychology, 51(1), 13–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webber, J. (Forthcoming). Habituation and first-person authority. In R. Altshuler & M. Sigrist (Eds.), Time and the philosophy of action. Routledge.

  • Wiers, R. W., Eberl, C., Rinck, M., Becker, E. S., & Lindenmeyer, J. (2011). Retraining automatic action tendencies changes alcoholic patients’ approach bias for alcohol and improves treatment outcome. Psychological Science, 22(4), 490–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zanon, R., De Houwer, J., Gast, A., & Smith, C. T. (2014). When does relational information influence evaluative conditioning? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(11), 2105–2122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, A. (2007). The nature of belief. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14(11), 61–82.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This essay was revised during my Mellon Postdoctoral Fellowship at the University of California, Berkeley, and subsequently with institutional support from Vassar College. For insightful comments on earlier drafts or lively debates about the ideas in this paper, I am indebted to Alejandro Arango, Charles Michael Brent, Michael Brownstein, Taylor Carman, Guillermo del Pinal, Andrew Franklin-Hall, Katie Gasdaglis, Bertram Gawronski, Tamar Szabó Gendler, Lydia Goehr, Brian Kim, Patricia Kitcher, Felix Koch, Chloe Layman, Eric Mandelbaum, Christia Mercer, Nate Meyvis, John Morrison, Matthew Moss, Marco Nathan, Andreja Novakovic, Christiana Olfert, Katherine Rickus, David Rosenthal, Michael Seifried, Beau Shaw, Susanna Siegel, Virginia Valian, Anubav Vasudevan, Sebastian Watzl, and several anonymous referees. Thanks also to audiences at Columbia University and the Eastern Division meeting of the American Philosophical Association in Atlanta, December 2012.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alex Madva.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Madva, A. Why implicit attitudes are (probably) not beliefs. Synthese 193, 2659–2684 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0874-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-015-0874-2

Keywords

Navigation