Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-p2v8j Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2024-05-28T10:02:51.980Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evolutionary Psychology, Ethology, and Essentialism (Because What They Don't Know Can Hurt Us)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 March 2020

Abstract

In 2002, Evolution and Human Behavior published a study purporting to show that the differences in toy preferences commonly attributed to girls and boys can also be found in male and female vervet monkeys, tracing the origin of these differing preferences back to a common ancestor. Despite some flaws in its design and the prima facie implausibility of some of its central claims, this research received considerable attention in both scientific circles and the popular media. In what follows, I survey some of the problems with this study that seem to be characteristic of research into sex differences in a particular research program in evolutionary psychology. I suggest that an epistemology of ignorance is at work that suppresses the methods and insights of an earlier research program, which emphasized the complexity and contingency that ultimately grounds the variety of human behaviors, in favor of one that has been widely criticized as empirically flawed and politically pernicious. I conclude with some speculative remarks on the persistence of this problematic research program in evolutionary psychology.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2012 by Hypatia, Inc.

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alcoff, Linda. 1988. Cultural feminism versus post‐structuralism: The identity crisis in feminist theory. Signs 13 (3): 405–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alexander, Gerianne, and Hines, Melissa. 2002. Sex differences in response to children's toys in nonhuman primates (Cercopithecus aethiops sabaeus). Evolution and Human Behavior 23 (6): 467–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron‐Cohen, Simon, Knickmeyer, Rebecca, and Belmonte, Matthew. 2005. Sex differences in the brain: Implications for explaining autism. Science 310 (5749): 819–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Battro, Antonio. 2000. Half a brain is enough: The story of Nico. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Birke, Lynda. 2000. Feminism and the biological body. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.Google Scholar
Blackless, Melanie, Charuvastra, Anthony, Derryck, Amanda, Fausto‐Sterling, Anne, Lauzanne, Karl, and Lee, Ellen. 2000. How sexually dimorphic are we? Review and synthesis. American Journal of Human Biology 12 (2): 151–66.3.0.CO;2-F>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boyd, Robert S. 2005a. Even monkey boys choose cars as toys; Research on play finds gender preferences similar to humans. Charlotte Observer, December 8.Google Scholar
Boyd, Robert S. 2005b. “NATURE, NOT NURTURE: New techniques and experiments make a case that sex‐linked behaviors spring from differences in the structure of the brain. St. Paul Pioneer Press, December 18.Google Scholar
Boyd, Robert S. 2005c. With monkeys, boys will be boys: Study: Males dig cars, females like dolls; As in humans, gender differences show up in toy preferences. Montreal Gazette, December 8.Google Scholar
Buller, David. 2005. Adapting minds: Evolutionary psychology and the persistent quest for human nature. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Buller, David J., and Gray Hardcastle, Valerie. 2000. Evolutionary psychology, meet developmental neurobiology: Against promiscuous modularity. Brain and Mind 1 (3): 307–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burghardt, George. 1997. Amending Tinbergen: A fifth aim for ethology. In Anthropomorphism, anecdotes, and animals, ed. Mitchell, Robert W., Thompson, Nicholas S. and Lyn Milies, H.Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Burkhardt, Richard. 2005. Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Butler, Judith. 1990. Gender trouble: Feminism and the subversion of identity. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Cahill, Larry. 2005. His brain, her brain. Scientific American 20 (5): 4047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cullen, Esther. 1957. Adaptations of the kittiwake to cliff‐nesting. The Ibis 99 (2): 275302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, Marian Stamp. 1989. The future of ethology: How many legs are we standing on? In Whither ethology? Perspectives on ethology, ed. Bateson, P. P. G. and Klopfer, Peter H.New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Dillon, Sam. 2005. Harvard chief defends his talk on women. The New York Times, January 18. http://www.nytimes.com/2005/01/18/national/18harvard.html?ref=education (accessed March 6, 2010).Google Scholar
Ereshefsky, Marc. 2004. Bridging the gap between human kinds and biological kinds. Philosophy of Science 71 (5): 912–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fausto‐Sterling, Anne. 2000. Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Fausto‐Sterling, Anne. 2001. Beyond difference: Feminism and evolutionary psychology. In Alas poor Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary psychology, ed. Rose, Hilary and Rose, Steven. London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Fleming, Nic. 2008. Boys and their toys? It's biological, not social. The Telegraph, April 7.Google Scholar
Fracchia, Joseph, and Lewontin, Richard. 1999. Does culture evolve? History and Theory 38 (4): 5278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frye, Marilyn. 1983. The politics of reality: Essays in feminist theory. Berkeley, Calif.: The Crossing Press.Google Scholar
Gould, Stephen Jay, and Lewontin, Richard. 1979. The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, Series B 205 (1161): 581–98.Google Scholar
Griffiths, Paul E. 2002. What is innateness? The Monist 85 (1): 7085.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffiths, Paul E. 2008. Ethology, sociobiology and evolutionary psychology. In A companion to philosophy of biology, ed. Sarkar, Sahotra and Plutyinski, Anya. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Hall, Brian K. 1999. Evolutionary developmental biology, 2nd ed. Dordrecht, Holland: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haraway, Donna. 1978. Animal sociology and a natural economy of the body politic, part I: A political physiology of dominance. Signs 4 (1): 2136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassett, Janice, Siebert, Erin, and Wallen, Kim. 2008. Sex differences in rhesus monkey toy preferences parallel those of children. Hormones and Behavior 54 (3): 359–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogan, Jerry A., and Bolhuis, Johan J. 2009. Tinbergen's four questions and contemporary behavioral biology. In Tinbergen's legacy: Function and mechanism in behavioral biology, ed. Bolhuis, Johan J. and Verhulst, Simon. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hopkins, Patrick. 1996. Gender treachery: Homophobia, masculinity, and threatened identities. In Rethinking masculinity: Philosophical explorations in light of feminism, ed. May, Larry, Strikwerda, Robert and Hopkins, Patrick. Lanham, Md.: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Hull, David L. 1987. Human nature. In PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association 1986, volume 2: Symposia and invited papers: 311.Google Scholar
Jamieson, Dale, and Beckoff, Mark. 1992. On aims and methods of cognitive ethology. In PSA: Proceedings of the Biennial Meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association: 110–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaiser, Anelis, Haller, Sven, Schmitz, Sigrid, and Nitsch, Cordula. 2009. On sex/gender related similarities and differences in fMRI language research. Brain Research Reviews 61 (2): 4959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, Evelyn Fox. 2000. The century of the gene. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, Thomas S. 1977. The essential tension: Selected studies in scientific tradition and change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawton, Marcy F., Garstka, William R., and Craig Hanks, J. 1997. The mask of theory and the face of nature. In Feminism and evolutionary biology: Boundaries, intersections, and frontiers, ed. Gowaty, Patricia. New York: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Lloyd, Elisabeth. 2005. The case of the female orgasm: Bias in the science of evolution. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Longino, Helen. 1997. Feminist epistemology as a local epistemology. Proceedings of the Aristotelian Society: Supplementary volume 71: 1935.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGrew, William. 2001. The nature of culture: Prospects and pitfalls of cultural anthropology. In Tree of origin: What primate behavior can tell us about human social evolution, ed. de Waal, Frans. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Meynell, Letitia. 2008. The power and promise of developmental systems theory. Les ateliers de l'ethique: La revue de CREUM 3 (2): 88103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mills, Charles. 1997. The racial contract. Ithaca, N.Y.: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Oyama, Susan. 2000. The ontogeny of information: Developmental systems and evolution, 2nd ed. Durham, N.C.: Duke University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, Jaak, and Panksepp, Jules. 2000. The seven sins of evolutionary psychology. Evolution and Cognition 6 (2): 108–31.Google Scholar
Robert, Jason, Hall, Brian, and Olson, Wendy. 2001. Bridging the gap between developmental systems theory and evolutionary developmental biology. BioEssays 23 (10): 954–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, Hilary, and Rose, Steven, eds. 2001. Alas poor Darwin: Arguments against evolutionary psychology. London: Vintage.Google Scholar
Roughgarden, Joan. 2004. Evolution's rainbow: Diversity, gender, and sexuality in nature and people. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Schiebinger, Londa. 1989. The mind has no sex? Women in the origins of modern science. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Seifritz, Erich, Esposito, Fabrizio, Neuhoff, John, Lüthi, Andreas, Mustovic, Henrietta, Dammann, Gerhard, von Bardeleben, Ulrich, Radue, Ernst W., Cirillo, Sossio, Tedeschi, Gioacchino, and Di Salle, Francesco. 2003. Differential sex‐independent amygdala response to infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological Psychiatry 54 (12): 1367–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sork, Victoria. 1997. Quantitative genetics, feminism, and evolutionary theories of gender differences. In Feminism and evolutionary biology: Boundaries, intersections, and frontiers, ed. Gowaty, Patricia. New York: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Spelman, Elizabeth. 1988. Inessential woman: Problems of exclusion in feminist thought. Boston: The Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Summers, Lawrence H. 2005. Remarks at NBER conference on diversifying the science and engineering work force. http://www.president.harvard.edu/speeches/summers_2005/nber.php (accessed July 4, 2010).Google Scholar
Tang‐Martinez, Zuleyma. 1997. The curious courtship of sociobiology and feminism: A case of irreconcilable differences. In Feminism and evolutionary biology: Boundaries, intersections, and frontiers, ed. Gowaty, Patricia. New York: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Tinbergen, Nikolaas. 1963. On aims and methods of ethology. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie 20: 410–33.Google Scholar
Tooby, John, and Cosmides, Leda. 1992. The psychological foundations of culture. In The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture, ed. Barkow, Jerome, Cosmides, Leda and Tooby, John. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tooby, John, and Cosmides, Leda. 2005. Conceptual foundations of evolutionary psychology. In The handbook of evolutionary psychology, ed. Buss, David. Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Wilson, Margo, Daly, Martin, and Schreib, Joanna. 1997. Femicide: An evolutionary psychological perspective. In Feminism and evolutionary biology: Boundaries, intersections, and frontiers, ed. Gowaty, Patricia. New York: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar