Skip to main content
Log in

Time Transformation in the Sign System of the Conditioned Reflex

  • Original Research
  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

How is time transformed when signs appear? In the sign system of the conditioned reflex, the sign (conditioned stimulus) reverses, changes the direction of time, and overcomes its unidirectionality and irreversibility. In a sense, there is a “return” to the past in the form of the future when the sign is introduced. The sign serves as a “Time machine” of sorts. The mechanism of time transformation is possible because a mirror is embedded inside the sign, the surface of which represents the psychological present time. When past experience meets that surface in the present, it may be reflected in the form of a future event. In the course of reflection, time changes its direction: if earlier time flowed from the event (the cause), it subsequently flows to other events (the purpose). Previously inaccessible past experience is now projected into the present, “comes to life” in it and is transformed into the future. In humans, unlike in animals, signs of the second order appear. Inside the signs of the second order, a mirror is built in, and the experience of the past as a subject is reflected in the future form as an object. An individual has the opportunity to consider oneself externally as an object. It goes beyond the subjective form. Thus, secondary signs once again change time direction. Due to this phenomenon, time becomes doubly reversible: first it turns around in a conditioned stimulus, and then in a secondary sign.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anokhin, P. K. (1975). Essays on the physiology of functional systems. Medicine.

  • Aristotle. (1991). Complete works. Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asratyan, E. A. (1953). Physiology of the central nervous system (Scientific works). Publishing house of the Academy of Medical Sciences of the USSR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asratyan, E. A. (1965). Compensatory adaptations, reflex activity and the brain. Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asratyan, E. A. (1974). Conditional reflex theory and motivational behavior. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 34(1), 15–31.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Asratyan, E. A. (1977). Essays on the physiology of higher nervous activity (in Russian). Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Augustine. (2006). Confessions. Hackett Pub. Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergson, H. (1929). Matter and Memory. George Allen & Unwin Ltd, The Macmillan Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beritashvili, I. S. (1974). Vertebrate Animal Memory. Nauka.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassirer, E. (1953). An Essay on Man Garden City. Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Tienne, A. (2015). The flow of time and the flow of signs: A basis for Peirce’s cosmosemiotics. The American Journal of Semiotics, 1–2(31), 29–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deely, J. (2015). From Semiosis to Semioethics. In P. Trifonas (Ed.), International handbook of semiotics. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9404-6_36

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dostalek, C. (1964). Rücklaüfige bedingte Verbindungen. Verlag der Tschechoslovakischen Akademie der Wissenschaften.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebbinghaus, H. (1885). Über das gedächtnis: Untersuchungen zur experimentellen psychologie. Duncker & Humblot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goltz, F. (1881). Ueber die verrichtungen des grosshirns: Gesammelte abhandlungen. Universitaets-Buchdruckerei von Carl Georgi in Bohn.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Gorlée, D. L. (2016). On habit: Peirce’s story and history. In D. West & M. Anderson (Eds.), Consensus on Peirce’s concept of habit: Before and beyond consciousness (pp. 315–340). Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guillaume, G. (1992). Principles of theoretical linguistics (in Russian). Progress.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobbes, T. (1840). The English works of Thomas Hobbes Vol IV John Bohn. Osmania University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1964). On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time. Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kull, K. (2015). Evolution, choice, and scaffolding: Semiosis is changing its own building. Biosemiotics, 8, 223–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kull, K. (2018). On the logic of animal umwelten: The animal subjective present and zoosemiotics of choice and learning. In Semiotics of animals in culture (pp. 135–148). Springer.

  • Kupalov, P. S. (1949). On the mechanism of the conditioned excitation process (in Russian). Fiziologicheskii Zhurnal SSSR Imeni IM Sechenov, 35, 582–586.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Labra-Spröhnle, F. (2016). Human, all too human: Euclidean and multifractal analysis in an experimental diagrammatic model of thinking. In Mihai Nadin (Ed.), Anticipation across disciplines (pp. 105–136). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Magnus, R. (2011). Time-plans of the organisms: Jakob von Uexküll’s explorations into the temporal constitution of living beings. Sign Systems Studies, 39(2/4), 37–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nomura, N., Matsuno, K., & Muranaka, T. (2019). How does time flow in living systems? Retrocausal scaffolding and e-series time. Biosemiotics, 12, 267–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1949). Lectures on the work of the cerebral hemisphere. USSR Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1951). Lectures on the work of the main digestive glands. USSR Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I. P. (1973). Twenty-five years of objective study of the higher nervous activity (behaviour) of animals. Nauka.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1868). Some Consequences Of Four Incapacities. The Journal of Speculative Philosophy, 2(3), 140–157.

  • Rosen, R. (2012). Anticipatory systems: Philosophical, mathematical & methodological foundations. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sechenov, I. M. (1953). Selected works (in Russian). Uchpedgiz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shingarov, G. H. (1978). The Conditioned reflex and the problem of sign and meaning (in Russian). Nauka.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shingarov, G. K. (2008). Pavlovsky conditioned reflex-natural science model for the study of sign systems. Epistemology & Philosophy of Science, 18(4), 145–163. https://doi.org/10.5840/eps200818413

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skipin G. V. (1947). On the mechanism of formation of the alimentary conditioned reflexes (in Russian). Soviet Sci.

  • Stjernfelt, F. (2007). Diagrammatology: An investigation on the borderlines of phenomenology, ontology and semiotics. Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1928). A text-book of psychology. The Macmillan Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and man. Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E. C. (1933). Sign-Gestalt or conditioned reflex. Psychological Review, 40(3), 246–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0071472

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vekker, L. M. (2000). Psyche and reality: A uniform theory of psychologicalal processes. Smysl.

    Google Scholar 

  • Voronin, L. G. (1965). A course of lectures on the physiology of higher nervous activity. High School.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1984). Collected Works: In 6 volumes. Vol. 6. Tool and symbol in child development. Pedagogy

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (2005). The history of the development of higher mental functions (in Russian), In Psychology of human development (in Russian), Meaning, Eksmo.

  • Whitrow, G. J. (1980). The natural philosophy of time. Thomas Nelson and Sons Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, K. A. (1929). The conditioned reflex and the sign function in learning. Psychological Review, 36(6), 481–497. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0074200

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. B. (1936). Effectiveness of token rewards for chimpanzees. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 12, 1–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes, P. M. (1943). Chimpanzees: A laboratory colony. Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank the anonymous reviewers for the comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Konstantin S. Mochalov.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author has declared no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mochalov, K.S. Time Transformation in the Sign System of the Conditioned Reflex. Biosemiotics 16, 85–104 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-022-09514-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-022-09514-7

Keywords

Navigation