Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 12 May 2020
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Physical Activity ‘Enrichment’: A Joint Focus on Motor Competence, Hot and Cool Executive Functions View all 14 articles

Deliberate Soccer Practice Modulates Attentional Functioning in Children

  • 1Department of Physical Education and Sport Sciences, Catholic Universiy of Valencia San Vicente Mártir, Valencia, Spain
  • 2Department of Experimental Psychology, Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, Granada, Spain

The main purpose of this study was to explore the association between the regular practice of open-skill sports (i.e., soccer) and executive control, along with other attentional functions (i.e., alerting and orienting) during preadolescence. The study was conducted on 131 participants (70 non-athletes and 61 soccer players). To measure cognitive performance, participants performed the Attentional Network Test—Interactions (ANT-I) task. Compared to non-athletes, soccer players showed overall faster responses and better executive control (e.g., reduced interference from distractors). Overall, our results provide new empirical evidence supporting the positive association between regular sports practice and cognitive performance, and more specifically executive functions. However, is important to note that the relationship between regular sport practice and cognition is complex and multifactorial. Our findings can be partly explained by the “cardiovascular fitness hypothesis” and the “cognitive component skills approach,” suggesting that an externally paced sport environment with high physical fitness and perceptual–cognitive demands may be an appropriate setting to optimize the development of cognitive functioning during early adolescence.

Introduction

The physical, psychological, and socio-affective benefits of physical activity have been widely described in numerous scientific studies (see reviews by Eime et al., 2013; Warburton and Bredin, 2017; Milanović et al., 2019). Regarding benefits at the cognitive level, many studies have explored this association in older adults (see the review by Northey et al., 2018). In addition, a great deal of research has recently been conducted on children and young adults, revealing the positive influence of regular physical exercise on cognitive performance (see reviews by Donnelly et al., 2016; Bidzan-Bluma and Lipowska, 2018; de Greeff et al., 2018). However, not all physical activities (i.e., working, free play, and deliberate practice) are the same. Ericsson et al. (1993) defined the term “deliberate practice” in terms of the quality of the practice time, only considering as deliberate practice highly structured activities that have been specially designed to develop specific goals and, consequently, to improve the current level of performance. In our investigation, we will focus our interest on the study of the association between deliberate soccer practice and attentional functioning in children.

Among the various cognitive functions that have been studied, researchers have particularly focused on understanding the relationship between regular exercise and executive control, especially in older adults (see the review by Sanders et al., 2019) and other age groups (see reviews by Best, 2010, with children, and Guiney and Machado, 2013, with other age groups).

Executive control is defined as a set of high-level cognitive functions involved in the planning, initiation, sequencing, and analysis of complex, goal-driven behaviors (Diamond, 2013). These cognitive functions are grouped into three types of sub-processes: (a) inhibitory control, which involves the ability to filter out irrelevant information to avoid unnecessary responses and selectively maintain attention and control over one’s actions; (b) working memory, which reflects the ability to keep in mind and manage information while performing complex cognitive tasks; and (c) cognitive flexibility, that is, the ability to restructure information and knowledge to efficiently adapt to various situational demands. In the sports context, executive control allows athletes to make quick decisions when faced with high cognitive demands. It also enables them to optimize their performance and is essential for their success, especially in externally paced sports with rich and complex stimuli (Vestberg et al., 2017).

The relationship between regular physical activity and executive control has been studied from different methodological approaches. The dominant approach has been to analyze cognitive performance using tasks that measure executive functions in laboratory contexts (e.g., Belsky et al., 2015; Chekroud et al., 2018). However, few studies have explored the association between regular practice of a specific sport and executive control, while simultaneously taking into account other attentional functions such as those described by Posner (1980): alerting, orienting, and executive control. Sports in general, and more specifically externally paced ones, are a rich environment to test this global function of attention given the complexity of the present stimuli and the decision-making processes involved. Therefore, our research focused precisely on analyzing the association between regular practice of a predominantly externally paced or open-skill sport such as soccer and executive control in the context of other attentional networks.

Regarding the positive link between regular physical exercise and executive control in children (see reviews by Guiney and Machado, 2013; de Greeff et al., 2018), most studies have focused on exploring the mediating effect of cardiovascular fitness (Buck et al., 2008; Hillman et al., 2009; Chaddock et al., 2010, 2012; Castelli et al., 2011; Davis et al., 2011; Pontifex et al., 2011; Voss et al., 2011). Overall, a positive correlation has been observed between the level of cardiovascular capacity and the performance of executive functions. These findings are based on the “cardiovascular fitness hypothesis” (North et al., 1990; Voss et al., 2011), according to which improved aerobic abilities, which are inherent to regular physical exercise, are the physiological mediator that determines cognitive benefits. More specifically, Hillman et al. (2009) and Pontifex et al. (2011) used various types of flanker tasks. They observed that preadolescent children with lower aerobic fitness showed less accuracy than those with higher fitness levels, although their speed of response was not affected. By contrast, studies such as that of Kimball (2009) have found no difference in antisaccade or flanker tasks between participants with regular aerobic practice and control participants with no regular practice. On the other hand, many studies have analyzed the relationship between sport expertise and executive control in young people, showing benefits in participants with greater skills in sports such as fencing (Chan et al., 2011), basketball (Alarcón et al., 2017), or badminton (Yu et al., 2017). These findings have been explained on the basis of the “cognitive component skills approach,” according to which sports training acts as a means to improve brain plasticity and develop certain cognitive functions more efficiently (Voss et al., 2010; Scharfen and Memmert, 2019a). This would explain the “specialization” in attentional performance achieved by practicing predominantly open-skill sports. Focusing on studies carried out on children, Verburgh et al. (2014) observed that highly skilled soccer players showed greater motor inhibition than amateur soccer players but no differences in executive attention or visuospatial working memory. In a later study, these authors compared the sport practice level of three groups of children (i.e., highly skilled soccer players with very frequent practice, amateur players with regular practice, and non-athletes with no practice) on motor inhibition, short-term memory, working memory, and the three attentional networks: alerting, orienting, and executive attention (Verburgh et al., 2016). Their findings showed that the higher the levels of sports practice, the better the measured motor inhibition and short-term memory. Moreover, amateur soccer players outperformed non-athletes in both short-term memory and working memory. More recently, Scharfen and Memmert (2019b) reported a positive relationship between performance in basic cognitive skills (i.e., working memory, perceptual load, multiple object tracking, and attention window) and specific motor abilities (i.e., sprint, change of direction, dribbling, ball control, shooting, and juggling) in a small sample of elite youth soccer players (n = 15, Mage = 12.72).

Scarce evidence exist on the link between regular physical exercise and exogenous spatial orienting, yielding variable results (Nougier et al., 1996; McAuliffe, 2004; Verburgh et al., 2014). Nougier et al. (1996) found that young adults who practiced predominantly open-skill sports showed greater flexibility and attentional control than their counterparts who practiced predominantly closed-skills sports. In the same age group, using a spatial cueing task, McAuliffe (2004) found that cueing effects were greater in university volleyball players than in non-athletes. At younger ages, only the study by Verburgh et al. (2014), comparing a sample of highly skilled vs amateur soccer players (8–12 years old), reported no differences between groups in attentional orienting. The study by Buck (2008) also failed to find a relationship between aerobic physical fitness and orienting in a sample of preadolescent athletes.

Finally, the few studies conducted to date on the relationship between regular physical exercise and phasic alerting in children have yielded scarce and inconclusive results. Buck (2008) found no effect of increased aerobic fitness on the alerting network in preadolescent athletes. However, a study by Verburgh et al. (2014) showed that highly skilled soccer players responded faster to a warning signal than amateur soccer players. Other studies have shown a positive relationship between deliberate practice of sports with high perceptual and decision-making demands and tonic alertness or vigilance (Ballester et al., 2015, 2018, 2019).

As described in the above review, the association between regular exercise and attentional functioning in general, and executive control in particular, has generally been approached by analyzing each cognitive function in isolation. These studies have been mainly focused on samples of older adults (e.g., Colcombe and Kramer, 2003; Bixby et al., 2007; Prakash et al., 2011) and, to a lesser extent, on younger adults (e.g., Stroth et al., 2009; Kamijo and Takeda, 2010) and children (Chaddock et al., 2011a, b; Davis et al., 2011). In addition, most studies mentioned above have highlighted the mediating role of cardiovascular or aerobic fitness when explaining the beneficial effect of regular exercise on executive attention (e.g., Guiney and Machado, 2013).

The main novelty in our study resides is the exploration of the link between deliberate practice of a predominantly open-skill sport (soccer) and executive control in interaction with other attentional networks in children. Our research is based on previous findings, which generally showed some benefits of regular physical exercise on various functions linked to executive control. Accordingly, we expect to find better executive control in a group of children with deliberate soccer practice compared to a group of children with no practice.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We conducted a cross-sectional study on a sample of 131 male children aged 10–12 years [Mage = 10.87 years old, standard deviation (SD) = 0.85]. Seventy of them were non-athlete primary school students (Mage = 10.84 years old, SD = 0.84) who fulfilled our previously defined inclusion criteria: no systematic sport practice or less than 5 h per week of sport participation outside school. These criteria were established according to data obtained from a survey on physical activity and sport practice habits. In order to guarantee systematization of practice and homogeneity of training, sleeping, and study habits, the athlete group, 61 children (Mage = 10.90 years old, SD = 0.86), were recruited from several U10 and U12 teams enrolled in a youth elite soccer academy of La Liga club in the Valencia region of Spain.

All participants self-reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. Statistical analyses were conducted on data from only 113 participants, after excluding those who did not perform the task appropriately (a higher than 25% error rate—4 participants) and 4 non-athletes who practiced sport for more than 5 h per week.

A sensitivity analysis conducted with G Power (Faul et al., 2007) showed that with our sample size divided into two groups (i.e., soccer players, 59, vs non-athletes, 54), the minimum effect size that could be detected for the between-group differences regarding each attentional function for α = 0.05 and 1 – β = 0.80 was f = 0.188 (i.e., minimum detectable effect).

The Research Institute of Sport Sciences of the Catholic University of Valencia granted ethical approval for this study (code UCV/2015/2016/22), which also complied fully with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments. Participation in the study was voluntary; all participants and their parents or legal guardians were properly informed about the risks and benefits of the study prior to any data collection and signed an institutionally approved informed consent form. The participants were also informed of their right to leave the experiment at any time.

Procedure

All the tests were performed in a single experimental session lasting approximately 45 min in the afternoon during normal school hours or during the training hours of the soccer players. First, the group of non-athletes completed the questionnaire on their physical-sports activity practice habits, in which they had to specify whether they practiced any sports outside their school physical education classes, which sport or sports they practiced, if any, and the number of hours they performed this activity per week. This questionnaire was not completed by the group of soccer players because they all had three training sessions a week lasting approximately 90 min, in addition to a weekend match lasting approximately 90 min. Participants were given an explanation of the ANT-I (Attentional Network Test–Interactions) task developed by Callejas et al. (2004) and were then asked to complete it.

Cognitive Task

The ANT-I is a modified version of the Attentional Network Task, known as ANT (Fan et al., 2002). It was developed to assess the independent functioning of the three attentional networks (alerting, orienting, and executive control or inhibitory control), as well as the potential interactions between them; it combines a spatial orienting task (Posner, 1980), a flanker task (Eriksen and Eriksen, 1974), and an audio signal to assess the functioning of the phasic alerting network. The efficiency of each network and its interactions are measured by registering reaction times (RTs) and response accuracy (i.e., percentage of errors). Considering the complex relationships that underlie the perceptual contexts that characterize most team sports (for a review of the benefits of using the ANT-I vs the ANT, see Ishigami and Klein, 2010), the ANT-I seems a suitable task to measure attentional functioning.

Data collection took place in specially equipped data collection rooms, under dimly lit conditions with no distracting noises. Participants were seated approximately 60 cm from a 15-inch monitor in which the attentional task stimuli were presented using E-Prime software (Schneider et al., 2002). Participants wore headphones through which they could hear the acoustic warning signal. The experiment began by displaying instructions to perform the task, while the researcher in charge of the experiment provided any necessary additional information.

Participants were encouraged to respond as quickly and accurately as possible by pressing the “C” (left) or “M” (right) key on the keyboard depending on the direction of the target stimulus (a central target arrow 0.55° long pointing either left or right), which was flanked by two other irrelevant arrows identical to the target on each side (0.06° away from each other). In each trial, the target arrow was preceded by an acoustic alerting tone (2,000 Hz and 50 ms) and/or a spatial orienting visual cue (an asterisk 0.6° in size and 50 ms). Participants were strongly encouraged to fixate the fixation point (variable duration of 400–1,600 ms) throughout the entire trial. The sequence of events for each trial is shown in Figure 1. The interference variable was defined according to the congruency of the direction of the flankers and target arrows: congruent trials (50% of trials), in which the target was flanked by arrows pointing in the same direction, and incongruent trials (the remaining 50% of trials), in which the flanking arrows and the target pointed in opposite directions. The orienting signal was presented above or below the fixation point in two-thirds of the trials. Three orienting conditions were thus established according to the presence of the cue: cued location trials, when the cue was presented at the same location as the target; uncued location trials, when the cue was presented at the opposite location to the target; and absence of cue, or no cue trials, when no cue was presented. The alerting signal was presented before the onset of the target in only half of the trials. The alerting variable was established according to the presence (tone) or absence (no tone) of the acoustic warning signal. The target was presented until the participants responded or for 1,700 ms. After the response, or after the allocated time had elapsed, the fixation point was presented for a variable length of time (depending on RTs and the duration of the initial display for that trial) so that all trials were equally long (4,450 ms). Initially, participants completed a practice block of two trials (including feedback about the response), followed by six experimental blocks of 48 trials each (without feedback), with resting intervals of about 1 min between them. Participants performed the ANT-I for approximately 25 min.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental procedure and stimuli sequence in the Attentional Network Test—Interactions task.

Statistical Analyses

Descriptive statistics (mean ± SD) were used to collect data on various aspects of the sample and groups. Normal distribution of data and the assumptions of sphericity were examined using Shapiro–Wilk and Mauchly tests, respectively. We conducted a mixed repeated-measures MANOVA on the data of each dependent variable (RTs and accuracy), with Deliberate Sport Practice (soccer players vs non-athletes) as a between-group factor and the other independent variables (Alerting: tone, no tone; Orienting: uncued, no cue, cued; and Congruency: congruent, incongruent) as within-participant factors. A first analysis was conducted by means of repeated-measures ANOVAs on both mean RTs and error percentages, including Alerting signal (no tone/tone), Orienting visual cue (invalid/no cue/valid), and Congruency (congruent/incongruent), in order to establish whether the usual pattern of results was observed. Post hoc analyses (paired t-tests) were conducted to further explore significant interactions, with the Tukey correction for multiple comparisons. The alpha level was set at p < 0.05 for univariate ANOVAs and repeated-measures ANOVAs. The partial eta squared (ηp2) effect size was also reported, indicating small (ηp2 > 0.01), moderate (ηp2 > 0.06), or strong (ηp2 > 0.14) effects (Field, 2009).

Results

Descriptive Analysis of the Attentional Variables

Data from incorrect response trials (2.68%), or those whose RTs were 2.5 SD above (2.63% lapses) or below (0.05% anticipation) the average RTs for each participant, were not included in statistical analyses of RTs. Descriptive results are displayed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean (M) ± SD reaction times and error percentage (in parenthesis) for each experimental condition and group.

Analysis of Attentional Functioning

Attentional effects were evaluated by means of 2 (Alerting) × 3 (Orienting) × 2 (Congruency) repeated-measures ANOVAs of mean RTs and error percentages. The analysis of RTs showed the typical functioning patterns of each of the attentional networks, Congruency, F(1,112) = 486.81, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.81; Alerting, F(1,112) = 145.83, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.57; and Orienting, F(2,224) = 169.72, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.60. Similarly, executive control was modulated by the other networks in RT, Alerting × Congruency, F(1,112) = 11.68, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.09, and Orienting × Congruency, F(2,224) = 23.36, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.17. A significant interaction was also found between Alerting and Orienting, F(2,224) = 11.77, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.09, in RT. Regarding the analysis on response accuracy, the main effects of Congruency, F(1,112) = 101.11, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.47, and Orienting, F(2,224) = 9.90, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.08, were replicated, but not those of Alerting, p = 0.102. We also observed the typical interactions between Alerting × Congruency, F(1,112) = 9.03, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.08, and Orienting × Congruency, F(2,224) = 4.33, p = 0.014, ηp2 = 0.04; the Alerting × Orienting interaction, F(2,224) = 2.93, p = 0.055, ηp2 = 0.03, was marginally significant. Therefore, the task showed the usual pattern of results.

Analysis of the Attentional Differences Between Non-Athletes and Soccer Players

More in line with the main aims of this study, the results of the 2 (Deliberate Sport Practice) × 2 (Alerting) × 3 (Orienting) × (2 Congruency) MANOVA revealed a main effect of Deliberate Sport Practice on RTs, F(1,111) = 38.01, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.25, but not on response accuracy (p = 0.145). Soccer players were 164 ms faster than non-athletes and also tended to show higher accuracy (4.28 and 5.67% error rates, respectively).

More importantly, our results revealed a positive association between deliberate sport practice and executive control. Soccer players showed reduced conflict compared to non-athletes with both response speed [F(1,111) = 28.01, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20] and response accuracy, although in this case, the difference was not significant (p = 0.181).

We also observed between-group differences in the functioning of attentional orienting, both in response speed, F(2,222) = 4.31, p = 0.015, ηp2 = 0.04, and in response accuracy, F(2,222) = 3.85, p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.03. However, as shown in Figure 2, post hoc analyses showed no differences between soccer players and non-athletes in the overall orienting effect (uncued–cued), p = 0.320. Between-group differences (with faster RTs in the group of soccer players) were particularly evident in the no-cue condition, in which no temporal or spatial signal was presented that could be used as a reference for preparation. A similar effect was found in response accuracy, with a lower percentage of errors especially in the cued and no-cue conditions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean reaction time (RT) for each experimental condition used to measure each attentional function, as a function of Deliberate Sport Practice. For alerting, only no-cue trials were used to compute the means. Note that soccer players were faster in general than non-athletes and showed reduced interference (i.e., reduced difference between congruent and incongruent conditions). Error bars represent the standard error of the mean. *Indicates significant differences between groups and attentional conditions (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.0001).

Finally, alerting was not significantly affected by Deliberate Sport Practice in either response speed (p = 0.070) or response accuracy (p = 0.072).

Finally, no relationship was observed between Deliberate Sport Practice and the interaction between executive control and the other attentional networks (all ps > 0.05).

Discussion

The purpose of this study was to test the association between deliberate sports practice and attentional functioning, especially at early ages (10–12 years), given that in recent decades, there has been a growing interest in clarifying the relationship between physical exercise and cognition (McMorris, 2016). Specifically, our approach is novel in the analysis of the influence of predominantly open-skill sports (i.e., soccer) on executive control, in a context of complex interaction with other attentional networks (i.e., alerting and orienting). The study of these relationships from childhood to preadolescence is especially interesting, as it is a crucial period for sports initiation (e.g., Raya et al., 1993, in the case of soccer) and coincides with a key evolutionary stage in cognitive development in general and concerning attentional networks in particular (Casey et al., 2000; Rueda et al., 2004).

Regarding the functioning of attentional networks, our results replicated those of previous studies conducted using the ANT task (Mezzacappa, 2004) or the ANT-I (Callejas et al., 2004; Rueda et al., 2004) revealing the typical main effects of executive control, alerting, and orienting, as well as the interactions between them (Alerting × Conflict, Orienting × Conflict, and Alerting × Orienting). Our findings confirm that the ANT-I is a useful and valid tool for assessing attentional functioning in children aged between 10 and 12 years. The ANT-I is a task which demands a complex space–time interaction of visual and sound stimuli. In our opinion, it is more similar to the demands of many situations in the real environment and, specifically, in many sports setting. The complexity of the stimuli presented by the ANT-I requires the simultaneous functioning of all three attentional networks. We therefore consider that the methodological paradigm used to study the relationship between sports practice and executive control is more ecological than those previously proposed in other studies using tasks that evaluate the functioning of this network in isolation (e.g., Castelli et al., 2011; Davis et al., 2011; Chaddock et al., 2012).

Regarding the specific objectives of our study, we found that deliberate practice of an open-skill sport such as soccer was associated to faster and more accurate responses, although the accuracy result did not reach statistical significance. These findings are consistent with those reported by previous studies showing the benefits of aerobic fitness, inherent to regular sports practice, on response speed (see the meta-analysis by Voss et al., 2010). Our results are also consistent with the findings of Ballester et al. (2015, 2018) showing that deliberate soccer practice during preadolescence was positively associated with response speed, albeit those studies used a different cognitive task (i.e., the Psychomotor Vigilance Task).

More importantly, and focusing on the association between deliberate sport practice and the functioning of executive control, our results showed a lower level of conflict in the group of soccer players compared to their non-athletes counterparts. As stated in the section “Introduction,” numerous studies have explored this relationship using different paradigms. In general, a pattern of results similar to that obtained in the present study has been observed. However, most studies used tasks suitable to measure executive control in isolation, independently from other cognitive functions (see reviews by Best, 2010; Guiney and Machado, 2013; de Greeff et al., 2018; Sanders et al., 2019). We consider that between-groups differences in executive control could be explained by the two dominant hypotheses in the study of the relationship between sports practice and cognitive functioning.

It is logical to argue that, though this variable was not measured in our study, the physiological demands of regular sport practice may have led to a better aero-anaerobic physical fitness in soccer players vs non-athletes (Ballester et al., 2018). In this regard, the cardiovascular fitness hypothesis (North et al., 1990; Voss et al., 2011) postulates that higher cardiovascular capacity, which is inherent to the regular practice of physical exercise, accounts for the cognitive improvement of individuals who exercise regularly due to physiological adaptations. Some examples of this are increased VO2, increased brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), and increased cerebral blood flow (North et al., 1990; Voss, 2016). In fact, the reviewed literature offers ample evidence of the positive impact of an improvement in aerobic fitness on the executive function in children (Buck et al., 2008; Hillman et al., 2009; Chaddock et al., 2010, 2012; Castelli et al., 2011; Davis et al., 2011; Pontifex et al., 2011; Voss et al., 2011). However, there is also some evidence that cardiovascular fitness is not a physiological mediator for improved cognitive performance in children (e.g., see Etnier et al., 2006; Ballester et al., 2018; Sanabria et al., 2019). In our opinion, these discrepancies may be partially explained by the methodological variations between the different studies, which make their results difficult to compare.

In view of this, the observed better executive control in soccer players than in non-athletes could alternatively be explained by the cognitive component skills approach (Mann et al., 2007; Voss et al., 2010). In our case, it is important to note that optimal performance in externally paced sports such as soccer requires not only a good level of physical fitness but also the ability to quickly adapt and respond to the demands of complex and constantly changing situations. Thus, the systematic and structured practice of externally paced sports, such as soccer, involves the learning and practice of basic cognitive abilities to manage these situations. The cognitive component skills approach would imply that this learning would be transferred to other general or specific domains, as proven by previous studies (Best, 2010; Williams et al., 2011; Ballester et al., 2019). In this vein, Wang et al. (2013) observed that university students who practiced an externally paced sport (i.e., tennis) exhibited greater inhibitory control than those who practiced a self-paced sport (i.e., swimming). The specific demands of the soccer environment may require this “cognitive specialization” in attentional performance, given that players are exposed to situations where they have to select relevant stimuli in a complex environment and decide between several possible options under high time pressure (Abernethy et al., 1993; Williams et al., 1999; Voss et al., 2010).

Our results seem to show that the association between deliberate soccer practice and executive control occurs at early ages and in a complex cognitive evaluation context, where the response is conditioned by stimuli that require the simultaneous participation of other attentional networks (i.e., orienting and alerting). Likewise, we observed a connection between sport practice and the functioning of orienting. However, in-depth analyses of this interaction showed that the greatest benefit observed in the group of non-athletes was due to the fact that they were significantly slower than soccer players in the no-cue conditions, not that they processed the orienting signal differently. This is consistent with the existing literature, given that there is very little evidence linking systematic sport practice to spatial orienting. Previous studies have, rather, focused on the analysis of the acute effect of exercise on exogenous (e.g., Huertas et al., 2011, 2019; Sanabria et al., 2011; Llorens et al., 2015) or endogenous spatial orienting (e.g., [citeskum]BR53,BR54,BR55,BR56,BR57,BR58[citeekum]Pesce et al., 2002, 2003, 2004, 2007a, 2007b, 2011).

Finally, our study did not find any link between deliberate sport practice and the functioning of the alerting network. Although the existing literature is scarce and controversial, our results are in line with those described by Buck (2008) showing no between-groups differences in the alerting network (evaluated with the ANT) in preadolescent children classified according to their level of aerobic fitness. By contrast, Verburgh et al. (2014) found that, in a sample of young soccer players aged 8–16 years from teams of different levels, presenting the warning signal benefited the players in higher-level teams. Regarding vigilance, the studies by Sanabria et al. (2019) and Ballester et al. (2018) did not find that the differences in vigilance functioning were explained by the cardiovascular fitness hypothesis but, rather, that these differences were due to the type of sport practiced. This leads us to confirm again that, when analyzing the relationships between systematic sport practice and any cognitive function, it is essential to control the influence of the variables that can mediate in this relationship: both those related to the development of the physical fitness of the participants and those related to the level of expertise, skill, or experience in the practice of the sport. It would be interesting to conduct further studies analyzing phasic alerting while controlling for aerobic capacity, in order to verify whether this factor is a positive mediator of this attentional function.

Some of the controversial findings described in several studies regarding the relationship between regular sports practice and cognitive functioning could be explained by the different methodological approaches used. First, it is important to analyze the effect of the age of the participants given that, as shown by many studies, it modulates the functioning of the different attentional functions and may explain these differences. It should be noted that the orienting network (e.g., Wainwright and Bryson, 2002) and the executive control network (e.g., Rueda et al., 2004; Simonds et al., 2007) seem to become consolidated before the age of 10, whereas the alerting network seems to continue its development after this age (e.g., Rueda et al., 2004; Pozuelos et al., 2014). In view of this, it would be interesting to conduct further longitudinal studies or studies including different age groups to determine how systematic sports practice modulates the development of each network at different stages. Moreover, when explaining the association between deliberate sport practice and executive control and the rest of the attentional functions, it is important to describe the type of task used for its assessment, the level of complexity and cognitive demand, the interaction with other networks, and other aspects. In this regard, difficulties could arise when comparing our results with those obtained by Verburgh et al. (2014), given that they used the ANT, which measures alerting through visual signals, as opposed to acoustic signals used in the ANT-I and thus in our research. These differences in the specificity of the stimuli used to assess the various cognitive functions, in accordance with the cognitive component skills approach, may explain the controversy in the results of various studies (e.g., in soccer, visual warning signals are likely to be much more relevant and specific than acoustic warning signals).

Finally, we suggest that further research about the relationship between regular sports practice and cognition should consider both the role of the type of sport practiced (i.e., self-paced vs externally paced), linked to the cognitive component skills approach (Mann et al., 2007; Voss et al., 2010), and the practitioner’s physical fitness, as proposed by the cardiovascular fitness hypothesis (North et al., 1990; Voss et al., 2011). This will allow assessment of the influence of each of these potential mediating factors in the development of every cognitive function studied. In this sense, one limitation of the present study is that its methodological design does not allow us to adopt a defined position in favor of either of the two hypotheses described above. The reason is that we consider that, as shown by previous studies, the soccer player group probably had a greater cardiovascular fitness level (e.g., Vänttinen et al., 2011; Plaza-Carmona et al., 2013; Ballester et al., 2015) and a better level of development of the attentional functions (Daus et al., 1989; Abernethy et al., 2001; Farrow and Abernethy, 2003) than the non-athlete group. In short, we believe that the contextual demands to which these players are regularly subjected in training and competition may lead to physical, physiological, perceptual, and cognitive adaptations and induce changes in the functioning of executive control and other cognitive functions.

Furthermore, our results could be partly explained by the existence of a self-selection bias in the sport context, the so-called “neuroselection effect,” according to which individuals with better cognitive functioning choose more active and healthier lifestyles (e.g., Kanazawa, 2013; Belsky et al., 2015). According to this hypothesis, children who play soccer in high-level clubs could have higher development of certain cognitive abilities, such as executive control, which would facilitate their involvement in these types of open-skills sports. An important direction for further research might be to investigate the functioning of the attentional networks in different sport disciplines (self-paced vs externally paced) controlling both the athlete’s level of expertise and the athlete’s levels of cardiovascular fitness, in order to clarify their relative contribution to attentional performance. Given the cross-sectional nature of the present study, future research should also use experimental designs with random assignment of participants to the experimental and control groups in order to add more solid evidence about the causal effect of deliberate sport practice on executive functions.

Conclusion

Our results shed new light on the advantageous connection between the deliberate practice of externally paced sports during preadolescence and both response speed and executive control, as measured in a context of interaction with other attentional networks.

However, given the cross-sectional design of our study, our findings do not allow us to confirm the underlying reasons for the advantage observed in the cognitive performance of soccer players. Different hypotheses could explain our results. First, the potential higher physical fitness level of soccer players may have an influence on neurophysiological aspects that benefit executive control, in line with the cardiovascular fitness hypothesis. Additionally, athletes’ executive control may also have been improved by repeated exposure to situations where these functions are in high demand, as proposed by the cognitive component skills approach. Finally, and according to the “neuroselection effect,” sport practitioners probably had, before starting the training program, more active lifestyles and better cognitive function than their non-athlete counterparts, which would also contribute to their better cognitive functioning.

Independently of the moderators and mediators underlying this association, these findings are particularly relevant, as they highlight the need to promote physical-sports activity at an institutional level from an early age, given its positive impact on both physical and cognitive development. They also reinforce the recommendations issued by the World Health Organization (World Health Organization [WHO], 2010) on the regular practice of physical exercise. In view of our findings, we propose that, within the recommended parameters of physical activity intensity (i.e., moderate and intense for at least 60 min per day), it would be interesting to specify the following: the activity should involve perceptual–cognitive and decision-making processes (e.g., externally paced sports such as team, combat, or racket sports). In fact, as shown by the current literature (e.g., Tomporowski and Pesce, 2019), physical-sport activities with higher perceptual–cognitive demands seem to be the most beneficial at the cognitive level.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made readily available to all qualified researchers. Requests for access to the data should be addressed to CM at consuelo.moratal@ucv.es.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Research Ethical Committee from Catholic University of Valencia (UCV/2015/2016/22). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

CM: conception and design of the study, the data acquisition, analysis, and interpretation, drafting of the article and critical revision for important intellectual content with specific contribution regarding the physical activity–attention relationship, final approval of the version to be published, and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the research. JL: contribution to the conception of the study, creation of the attentional test, the data analysis and interpretation, drafting and critical revision of the article for important intellectual content with specific contribution regarding attentional functioning issues, final approval of the version to be published, and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the research. RB: data interpretation, drafting and critical revision of the study for important intellectual content with specific contribution regarding systematic sport practice issues, contribution to drafting the article, final approval of the version to be published, and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the research. FH: main role in the conception and design of the study with a relevant role in project coordination and data acquisition, interpretation and critical revision of the article with specific contribution regarding the physical activity–attention relationship, final approval of the version to be published, and agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the research.

Funding

This research was supported by grants from San Vicente Mártir Catholic University of Valencia (2019-158-003 to FH and 2018-158-004 to CM) and the Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness (PSI2017-84926-P) to JL and FH. The funding bodies had no role in the study design, the data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank all the players, parents, coaches, and staff members of Valencia C.F. and the students, parents, and teachers of the Villar Palasí Primary Education School (Sagunto, Spain) for their participation in this research project.

References

Abernethy, B., Gill, D. P., Parks, S. L., and Packer, S. T. (2001). Expertise and the perception of kinematic and situational probability information. Perception 30, 233–252. doi: 10.1068/p2872

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Abernethy, B., Thomas, K. T., and Thomas, J. T. (1993). “Strategies for improving understanding in motor expertise (or mistakes we have made and things we have learned!!),” in Cognitive Issues in Motor Expertise, eds J. L. Starkes and F. Allard (Amsterdam: Elsevier), 317–356.

Google Scholar

Alarcón, F., Ureña, N., Castillo, A., Martín, D., and Cárdenas, D. (2017). Executive functions predict expertise in basketball players. Rev. Psicol. Deport. 26, 71–74.

Google Scholar

Ballester, R., Huertas, F., Molina, E., and Sanabria, D. (2018). Sport participation and vigilance in children: influence of different sport expertise. J. Sport Health Sci. 7, 497–504. doi: 10.1016/j.jshs.2017.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballester, R., Huertas, F., Pablos-Abella, C., Llorens, F., and Pesce, C. (2019). Chronic participation in externally paced, but not self-paced sports is associated with the modulation of domain-general cognition. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 19, 1110–1119. doi: 10.1080/17461391.2019.1580318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ballester, R., Huertas, F., Yuste, F. J., Llorens, F., and Sanabria, D. (2015). The relationship between regular sports participation and vigilance in male and female adolescents. PLoS One 10:e0123898. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0123898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, D. W., Caspi, A., Israel, S., Blumenthal, J. A., Poulton, R., and Moffitt, T. E. (2015). Cardiorespiratory fitness and cognitive function in midlife: neuroprotection or neuroselection? Ann. Neurol. 77, 607–617. doi: 10.1002/ana.24356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Best, J. R. (2010). Effects of physical activity on children’s executive function: contributions of experimental research on aerobic exercise. Dev. Rev. 30, 331–351. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2010.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidzan-Bluma, I., and Lipowska, M. (2018). Physical activity and cognitive functioning of children: a systematic review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15:800. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15040800

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bixby, W. R., Spalding, T. W., Hautler, A. J., Deeny, S. P., Mahlow, P. T., Zimmerman, J. B., et al. (2007). The unique relation of physical activity to executive function in older men and women. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 39, 2093–2093. doi: 10.1249/01.mss.0000296709.64629.6e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buck, S. M. (2008). Aerobic fitness an attentional control in preadolescent children. (Doctoral dissertation, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 2008). Diss. Abstr. Int. 68:7138.

Google Scholar

Buck, S. M., Hillman, C. H., and Castelli, D. M. (2008). The relation of aerobic fitness to stroop task performance in preadolescent children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 40, 166–172. doi: 10.1249/mss.0b013e318159b035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., and Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: on their independence and interactions. Brain Cogn. 54, 225–227. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., and Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biol. Psychol. 54, 241–257. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00058-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelli, D. M., Hillman, C. H., Hirsch, J., Hirsch, A., and Drollette, E. (2011). FIT Kids: time in target heart zone and cognitive performance. Prev. Med. 52, S55–S59. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2011.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Kim, J. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M., et al. (2010). A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Res. 1358, 172–183. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Erickson, K. I., Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., VanPatter, M., Pontifex, M. B., et al. (2012). A functional MRI investigation of the association between childhood aerobic fitness and neurocognitive control. Biol. Psychol. 89, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., and Cohen, N. J. (2011a). Aerobic fitness and executive control of relational memory in preadolescent children. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 43, 344–349. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181e9af48

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaddock, L., Pontifex, M. B., Hillman, C. H., and Kramer, A. F. (2011b). A review of the relation of aerobic fitness and physical activity to brain structure and function in children. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 17, 975–985. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000567

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chan, J. S. Y., Wong, A. C. N., Liu, Y., Yu, J., and Yan, J. H. (2011). Fencing expertise and physical fitness enhance action inhibition. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 12, 509–514. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.04.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chekroud, S. R., Gueorguieva, R., Zheutlin, A. B., Paulus, M., Krumholz, H. M., Krystal, J. H., et al. (2018). Association between physical exercise and mental health in 1.2 million individuals in the USA between 2011 and 2015: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Psychiatry 5, 739–746. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30227-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colcombe, S., and Kramer, A. F. (2003). Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 14, 125–130. doi: 10.1177/1745691617707316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daus, A. T., Wilson, J., and Freeman, W. M. (1989). Predicting success in football. J. Sports Med. Phys. Fitness 29, 209–212.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Davis, C. L., Tomporowski, P. D., McDowell, J. E., Austin, B. P., Miller, P. H., Yanasak, N. E., et al. (2011). Exercise improves executive function and achievement and alters brain activation in overweight children: a randomized, controlled trial. Health Psychol. 30, 91–98. doi: 10.1037/a0021766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Greeff, J. W., Bosker, R. J., Oosterlaan, J., Visscher, C., and Hartman, E. (2018). Effects of physical activity on executive functions, attention and academic performance in preadolescent children: a meta-analysis. J. Sci. Med. Sport 21, 501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2017.09.595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Diamond, A. (2013). “Executive functions,” in Annual Review of Psychology, Vol. 64, ed. S. T. Fiske (Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews), 135–168.

Google Scholar

Donnelly, J. E., Hillman, C. H., Castelli, D., Etnier, J. L., Lee, S., Tomporowski, P., et al. (2016). Physical activity, fitness, cognitive function, and academic achievement in children: a systematic review. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 48, 1197–1222. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eime, R. M., Young, J. A., Harvey, J. T., Charity, M. J., and Payne, W. R. (2013). A systematic review of the psychological and social benefits of participation in sport for children and adolescents: informing development of a conceptual model of health through sport. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 10:98. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-10-98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., and Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol. Rev. 100, 363–406. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eriksen, B. A., and Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Percept. Psychophys. 16, 143–149. doi: 10.1016/j.aap.2008.09.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etnier, J. L., Nowell, P. M., Landers, D. M., and Sibley, B. A. (2006). A meta-regression to examine the relationship between aerobic fitness and cognitive performance. Brain Res. Rev. 52, 119–130. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., and Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farrow, D., and Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception - action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception 32, 1127–1139. doi: 10.1068/p3323

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., and Buchner, A. (2007). A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, A. (2009). Discovering Statistics Using SPSS. London: SAGE Publications.

Google Scholar

Guiney, H., and Machado, L. (2013). Benefits of regular aerobic exercise for executive functioning in healthy populations. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 20, 73–86. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0345-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hillman, C. H., Buck, S. M., Themanson, J. R., Pontifex, M. B., and Castelli, D. M. (2009). Aerobic fitness and cognitive development: event-related brain potential and task performance indices of executive control in preadolescent children. Dev. Psychol. 45, 114–129. doi: 10.1037/a0014437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huertas, F., Blasco, E., Moratal, C., and Lupiáñez, J. (2019). Caffeine intake modulates the functioning of the attentional networks depending on consumption habits and acute exercise demands. Sci. Rep. 9:10043. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-46524-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huertas, F., Zahonero, J., Sanabria, D., and Lupiáñez, J. (2011). Functioning of the attentional networks at rest vs. during acute bouts of aerobic exercise. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 33, 649–665. doi: 10.1123/jsep.33.5.649

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ishigami, Y., and Klein, R. M. (2010). Repeated measurement of the components of attention using two versions of the Attention Network Test (ANT): stability, isolability, robustness, and reliability. J. Neurosci. Methods 190, 117–128. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kamijo, K., and Takeda, Y. (2010). Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 75, 304–311. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanazawa, S. (2013). Childhood intelligence and adult obesity. Obesity 21, 434–440. doi: 10.1002/oby.20018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kimball, A. (2009). A Study of the Effects of Aerobic Exercise on the Executive Cognitive Functioning of Overweight Children. Available online at: http://juro.uga.edu/2009/papers/anne_kimball.pdf (accessed February 27, 2009).

Google Scholar

Llorens, F., Sanabria, D., and Huertas, F. (2015). The influence of acute intense exercise on exogenous spatial attention depends on physical fitness level. Exp. Psychol. 62, 20–29. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000270

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mann, D. T. Y., Williams, A. M., Ward, P., and Janelle, C. M. (2007). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport: a meta-analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 29, 457–478.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

McAuliffe, J. (2004). Differences in attentional set between athletes and nonathletes. J. Gen. Psychol. 131, 426–437. doi: 10.3200/GENP.131.4.426-437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McMorris, T. (2016). Exercise-Cognition Interaction: Neuroscience Perspectives. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

Google Scholar

Mezzacappa, E. (2004). Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and socio-demographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young urban children. Child Dev. 75, 1373–1386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milanović, Z., Pantelić, S., Čović, N., Sporiš, G., Mohr, M., and Krustrup, P. (2019). Broad-spectrum physical fitness benefits of recreational football: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 53, 926–939. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2017-097885

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

North, T. C., McCullagh, P., and Tran, Z. V. (1990). Effect of exercise on depression. Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 18, 379–415.

Google Scholar

Northey, J. M., Cherbuin, N., Pumpa, K. L., Smee, D. J., and Rattray, B. (2018). Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adults older than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br. J. Sports Med. 52, 154–160. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2016-096587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nougier, V., Rossi, B., Alain, C., and Taddei, F. (1996). Evidence of strategic effects in the modulation of orienting of attention. Ergonomics 39, 1119–1133. doi: 10.1080/00140139608964533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Capranica, L., Tesittore, A., and Figura, F. (2002). Effects of a submaximal physical load on the orienting and focusing of visual attention. J. Hum. Mov. Stud. 42, 401–420.

Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Capranica, L., Tesittore, A., and Figura, F. (2003). Focusing of visual attention under submaximal physical load. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 1, 275–292.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Casella, R., and Capranica, L. (2004). Modulation of visuospatial attention at rest and during physical exercise: gender differences. Int. J. Sport Psychol. 35, 328–341.

Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Cereatti, L., Casella, R., Baldari, C., and Capranica, L. (2007a). Preservation of visual attention in older expert orienteers at rest and under physical effort. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 29, 78–99. doi: 10.1123/jsep.29.1.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Tessitore, A., Casella, R., Pirritano, M., and Capranica, L. (2007b). Focusing of visual attention at rest and during physical exercise in soccer players. J. Sports Sci. 25, 1259–1270. doi: 10.1080/02640410601040085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesce, C., Cereatti, L., Forte, R., Crova, C., and Casella, R. (2011). Acute and chronic exercise effects on attentional control in older road cyclists. Gerontology 57, 121–128. doi: 10.1159/000314685

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaza-Carmona, M., Ubago-Guisado, E., Sánchez-Sánchez, J., Felipe, J. L., and Fernández-Luna, A. (2013). Composición corporal y condición física en niñas pre-púberes nadadoras y futbolistas. J. Sport Health Res. 5, 251–258.

Google Scholar

Pontifex, M. B., Raine, L. B., Johnson, C. R., Chaddock, L., Voss, M. W., Cohen, N. J., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and the flexible modulation of cognitive control in preadolescent children. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1332–1345. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2010.21528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 32, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pozuelos, J. P., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., and Rueda, M. R. (2014). Development of attention networks and their interactions in childhood. Dev. Psychol. 50, 2405–2415. doi: 10.1037/a0037469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Ericsson, K. I., Lewis, J. M., Chaddock, L., Malkowski, E., et al. (2011). Cardiorespiratory fitness and attentional control in the aging brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4:229. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00229

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Raya, A., Fradua, L., and Pino, J. (1993). Metodología Diferencial Para la Mejora de Gestos Técnicos Frecuentes en Fútbol. Congreso Mundial de Ciencias de la Actividad Física y el Deporte. Granada: Universidad de Granada.

Google Scholar

Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., et al. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia 42, 1029–1040. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanabria, D., Luque-Casado, A., Perales, J. C., Ballester, R., Ciria, L. F., Huertas, F., et al. (2019). The relationship between vigilance capacity and physical exercise: a mixed-effects multistudy analysis. PeerJ 7:e7118. doi: 10.7717/peerj.7118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanabria, D., Morales, E., Luque, A., Gálvez, G., Huertas, F., and Lupiáñez, J. (2011). Effects of acute aerobic exercise on exogenous spatial attention. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 12, 570–574. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2011.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanders, L. M. J., Hortobagyi, T., la Bastide-van Gemert, S., van der Zee, E. A., and van Heuvelen, M. J. G. (2019). Dose-response relationship between exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairment: a systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS One 14:e0210036. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0210036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharfen, H. E., and Memmert, D. (2019a). Measurement of cognitive functions in experts and elite-athletes: a meta-analytic review. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 33, 843–860. doi: 10.1002/acp.3526

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scharfen, H. E., and Memmert, D. (2019b). The relationship between cognitive functions and sport-specific motor skills in elite youth soccer players. Front. Psychol. 10:817. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, W., Eschman, A., and Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime User’s Guide. Pittsburg, CA: Psychology Software Tools Inc.

Google Scholar

Simonds, J., Kieras, J. E., Rueda, M. R., and Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Effortful control, executive attention, and emotional regulation in 7-10-year-old children. Cogn. Dev. 22, 474–488. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2007.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stroth, S., Hille, K., Spitzer, M., and Reinhardt, R. (2009). Aerobic endurance exercise benefits memory and affect in young adults. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 19, 223–243. doi: 10.1080/09602010802091183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomporowski, P. D., and Pesce, C. (2019). Exercise, sports, and performance arts benefit cognition via a common process. Psychol. Bull. 145, 929–951. doi: 10.1037/bul0000200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vänttinen, T., Blomqvist, M., Nyman, K., and Häkkinen, K. (2011). Changes in body composition, hormonal status, and physical fitness in 11-, 13-, and 15-year-old Finnish regional youth soccer players during a two-year follow-up. J. Strength Cond. Res. 25, 3342–3351. doi: 10.1519/JSC.0b013e318236d0c2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verburgh, L., Scherder, E. A., van Lange, P. M., and Oosterlaan, J. (2014). Executive functioning in highly talented soccer players. PLoS One 9:e91254. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verburgh, L., Scherder, E. J., Van Lange, P. A., and Oosterlaan, J. (2016). Do elite and amateur soccer players outperform non-athletes on neurocognitive functioning? A study among 8-12 year old children. PLoS One 11:e0165741. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vestberg, T., Reinebo, G., Maurex, L., Ingvar, M., and Petrovic, P. (2017). Core executive functions are associated with success in young elite soccer players. PLoS One 12:e0170845. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W. (2016). “The chronic exercise–cognition interaction: fMRI research,” in Exercise-Cognition Interaction, ed. T. McMorris (London: Elsevier), 187–209.

Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Kramer, A. F., Basak, C., Prakash, R. S., and Roberts, B. (2010). Are expert athletes expertin the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic review of cognition and sport expertise. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 24, 812–826. doi: 10.1002/acp.1588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voss, M. W., Nagamatsu, L. S., Liu-Ambrose, T., and Kramer, A. F. (2011). Exercise, brain, and cognition across the lifespan. J. Appl. Physiol. 111, 1505–1513. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wainwright, A., and Bryson, S. E. (2002). The development of exogenous orienting: mechanisms of control. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 82, 141–155. doi: 10.1016/s0022-0965(02)00002-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, C. H., Chang, C. C., Liang, Y. M., Shih, C. M., Chiu, W. S., Tseng, P., et al. (2013). Open vs. closed skill sports and the modulation of inhibitory control. PLoS One 8:e55773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warburton, D. E. R., and Bredin, S. S. D. (2017). Health benefits of physical activity: a systematic review of current systematic reviews. Curr. Opin. Cardiol. 32, 541–556. doi: 10.1097/HCO.0000000000000437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A. M., Davids, K., and Williams, J. G. (1999). Visual Perception and Action in Sport. London: Routledge.

Google Scholar

Williams, A. M., Ford, P. R., Eccles, D. W., and Ward, P. (2011). Perceptual-cognitive expertise in sport and its acquisition: implications for applied cognitive psychology. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 432–442. doi: 10.1002/acp.1710

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO] (2010). Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Yu, Q., Chan, C. C., Chau, B., and Fu, A. S. (2017). Motor skill experience modulates executive control for task switching. Acta Psychol. 180, 88–97. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.08.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: executive control, orienting, alerting, childhood, team sport

Citation: Moratal C, Lupiáñez J, Ballester R and Huertas F (2020) Deliberate Soccer Practice Modulates Attentional Functioning in Children. Front. Psychol. 11:761. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00761

Received: 06 February 2020; Accepted: 27 March 2020;
Published: 12 May 2020.

Edited by:

Kimberley D. Lakes, The University of California School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

António José Figueiredo, University of Coimbra, Portugal
Donatella Di Corrado, Kore University of Enna, Italy

Copyright © 2020 Moratal, Lupiáñez, Ballester and Huertas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Florentino Huertas, florentino.huertas@ucv.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.