Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 04 November 2022
Sec. Motor Neuroscience
Volume 16 - 2022 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2022.984053

Mapping relational links between motor imagery, action observation, action-related language, and action execution

  • Department of Psychology, University of Limerick, Limerick, Ireland

Actions can be physically executed, observed, imagined, or simply thought about. Unifying mental processes, such as simulation, emulation, or predictive processing, are thought to underlie different action types, whether they are mental states, as in the case of motor imagery and action observation, or involve physical execution. While overlapping brain activity is typically observed across different actions which indicates commonalities, research interest is also concerned with investigating the distinct functional components of these action types. Unfortunately, untangling subtleties associated with the neurocognitive bases of different action types is a complex endeavour due to the high dimensional nature of their neural substrate (e.g., any action process is likely to activate multiple brain regions thereby having multiple dimensions to consider when comparing across them). This has impeded progress in action-related theorising and application. The present study addresses this challenge by using the novel approach of multidimensional modeling to reduce the high-dimensional neural substrate of four action-related behaviours (motor imagery, action observation, action-related language, and action execution), find the least number of dimensions that distinguish or relate these action types, and characterise their neurocognitive relational links. Data for the model comprised brain activations for action types from whole-brain analyses reported in 53 published articles. Eighty-two dimensions (i.e., 82 brain regions) for the action types were reduced to a three-dimensional model, that mapped action types in ordination space where the greater the distance between the action types, the more dissimilar they are. A series of one-way ANOVAs and post-hoc comparisons performed on the mean coordinates for each action type in the model showed that across all action types, action execution and concurrent action observation (AO)-motor imagery (MI) were most neurocognitively similar, while action execution and AO were most dissimilar. Most action types were similar on at least one neurocognitive dimension, the exception to this being action-related language. The import of the findings are discussed in terms of future research and implications for application.

Introduction

Motor cognition relates to any mental process that is associated with neural activity in the motor system (Jeannerod, 2006). It concerns mental functions such as action planning, motor imagery, action observation, and action-related language, which are assumed to be interconnected through their reliance on neural sensorimotor systems (Grèzes and Decety, 2001; Jeannerod, 2001; Prinz, 2014). In particular, motor imagery (MI) is a dynamic process whereby actions are mentally generated and unfold over time without physical movement execution (Decety, 1996; Jeannerod, 2006). Similarly, action observation (AO) involves implicit dynamic mental processes that represent action content and are triggered when observing other’s actions (Jeannerod, 2006; Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). The aim of this study is to explore the interconnectedness between different action processes using non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS). This technique involves mapping dis/similarities between psychological entities (which in this study are different action types) in a multidimensional space, where the distances between entities indicate the extent of relatedness between them (Shepard, 1962; Hout et al., 2013).

Covert (or motor-cognitive) actions, such as MI or AO, are deemed similar to physical action execution because all supposedly have access to overlapping and interacting motor and cognitive systems (Ramsey et al., 2021). The latter cognitive system includes the component processes involved in mental action representation, such as attention, memory, and inhibitory control processes which facilitate, for example, action goal and plan formation (see O’Shea and Moran, 2017). It is generally recognised (with substantial empirical support) that covert actions allow us to activate neural motor systems offline (i.e., cognitively through internal representational processes and without physically moving; e.g., Hardwick et al., 2018; Courson and Tremblay, 2020), thus offering the prospect of improving or rehabilitating the functioning of these systems (Caligiore et al., 2017; Paravlic, 2022), inducing neuroplastic change (e.g., Baeck et al., 2012; Debarnot et al., 2014), learning new skills (e.g., Kraeutner et al., 2016; Ingram et al., 2019), and/or improving behavioural skill (e.g., Schuster et al., 2011; Di Rienzo et al., 2016). It is proposed that action information and effect information are merged in internal motor representations (i.e., mental knowledge structures), and the motor system is primed and the sensory consequences of action anticipated (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Kilintari et al., 2014; Kilteni et al., 2018). Research in this field has largely focused on exploring the similarities between various covert actions and action execution with the aims of: (i) using covert actions, such as MI, as an adjunct to physical action to improve movement skill or develop movement rehabilitation techniques (e.g., Di Rienzo et al., 2016; Bek et al., 2021); (ii) elucidating the neurocognitive architecture of human movement control (Schack and Ritter, 2009; Land et al., 2013; Rosenbaum, 2021); and (iii) developing a unified theoretical framework accounting for the interconnections between perception, cognition, and action (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Hommel et al., 2001; Jeannerod, 2001; Hommel, 2019).

One widely explored theoretical framework accounting for similarities between different action types is motor simulation theory (MST; Jeannerod, 2001, 2006; for review see O’Shea and Moran, 2017), which suggests that covert actions (i.e., MI, AO, perceptually-based decisions, action-related language, etc.) and physical action execution utilise similar motor representational processes (Jeannerod, 2001, 2006). In this regard, all action types (e.g., physical execution, MI, AO, action-related language) assemble mental motor representations of the intention to act, which contain action plans and motor programs that guide subsequent physical action execution or, as in the case of MI for example, the mental unfolding or simulation of the action (Jeannerod, 1994, 2006). Shared mental representational processes signify that different mental and physical action types are alternative ways of generating an action, and so, can be considered functionally equivalent (Jeannerod, 1994, 2006; although see Glover and Baran, 2017). Additionally, mental motor simulation processes are also assumed to provide pragmatic knowledge of action-related words during semantic language processing, and therefore assist in meaning extraction and learning (Jeannerod, 2006; Bonnet et al., 2022). Overlapping motor and cognitive systems, whereby cognitive action states (e.g., MI or action-related language) activate motor centres in the brain, is a key advantage for human behaviour because it offers opportunities for behaviour rehabilitation or improvement, wherein one action type is used to enhance another when dysfunction occurs.

The idea of interconnected motor-cognitive systems is captured not only by MST (Jeannerod, 2001, 2006), but also by several other prominent theoretical views. In this regard, several potential mechanisms have independently been proposed to drive perceptual-cognitive-motor processes, including association (Barsalou, 1999, 2008; Hesslow, 2012; Pulvermüller, 2018), emulation (Grush, 2004), simulation (Barsalou, 1999, 2008; Jeannerod, 2001, 2006), perception-action coding (Hommel et al., 2001; Hommel, 2019), or predictive process (Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000) mechanisms. These theoretical views differ somewhat in their central tenets and mechanisms (of which an in-depth discussion is beyond the remit of the present study, and so the reader is encouraged to explore the cited authors in relation to the details of each) but can be considered broadly compatible because they highlight links between action and the effects of that action, and suggest that lower-level sensorimotor systems are necessarily integrated into larger cognitive systems, that facilitate action planning and programming processes, anticipation of action and sensory outcomes, behaviour mimicking, conceptual processing, and so on. In relation to the nature of the functional interconnections between perceptual, cognitive, and motor systems, whether the aforementioned mechanisms promote automatic association (the result of learning processes; Pulvermüller, 2005, 2012; Hesslow, 2012) or representational and simulation processes (the dynamic assembly and anticipatory processing of sensorimotor information; Jeannerod, 2001; Grush, 2004) may have subtly different manifestations in any overlap between the systems. It might be expected that interconnections between motor, perceptual, and cognitive systems via (Hebbian-like) association would be tighter and/or more complete across motor and cognitive functions than those based on simulation or emulation processes. To provide an example, when the cognitive system is engaged in action-related language processing, association mechanisms predict that sensorimotor systems are cognitively (re)used in an implicit manner to extract word meaning (Pulvermüller, 2018). This mechanism is a consequence of learning where action-related words and their associated neurocognitive sensorimotor functions are automatically and computationally interlinked (i.e., Barsalou, 1999; Hauk et al., 2004; Pulvermüller, 2012, 2018). A consequence of such a close interconnection would be that action performance could facilitate action-related language processing, and vice versa. Alternatively, simulation and emulation mechanisms appear to operate on (at least partially) identified semantic information, in that, the action-related linguistic information is simulated in motor systems after initial meaning-related processing so that a pragmatic knowledge of the word is achieved (Jeannerod, 2006). Different theoretical accounts of motor-cognitive processes highlight the need for continued investigation into the nature of the interconnections between motor and cognitive systems, because developing a single overarching theoretical framework will ultimately benefit application in this area (e.g., how best to exploit interconnections between different systems in the rehabilitation of any one that is dysfunctional).

If motor representational, simulation, or association processes support the covert action processes of MI, AO, and action-related language, it can be expected that activity in their underlying neural systems will overlap due to a shared fundamental neurocognitive mechanism driving their functioning; this is largely what has been reported, albeit across different combinations of action types (Hétu et al., 2013; Hardwick et al., 2018; Courson and Tremblay, 2020). Shared neuroanatomical structures have been demonstrated for AO, MI, and action execution (Caspers et al., 2010; Hétu et al., 2013; Hardwick et al., 2018). In this regard, action execution has been found to share a more similar and/or extended network with MI than with AO (Hardwick et al., 2018; Courson and Tremblay, 2020). Overlapping brain regions between MI and execution typically include the supplementary motor area (SMA), premotor cortex (PMC), posterior parietal areas (PPC; including inferior and superior parietal lobes, IPL, SPL), basal ganglia, and cerebellum (Hardwick et al., 2018). AO also activates the SMA, PMC, parietal, and additionally the occipital areas, but typically fails to activate subcortical areas, such as the cerebellum (Hardwick et al., 2018; although see Calvo-Merino et al., 2006). Some areas are suggested to be distinct to MI, for example, the dlPFC, and given the absolute reliance of MI on stored information and internal manipulation of such information, it is not surprising that this area, which is associated with active or working memory, is triggered (e.g., Frith and Dolan, 1996; Hardwick et al., 2018). Regarding action-related language, there is evidence of activity in neural motor centres during language processing (for review see Jirak et al., 2010; Yang and Shu, 2014; Courson and Tremblay, 2020), and this appears to be somatotopically organised (Hauk et al., 2004; Pulvermüller, 2005). Furthermore, research demonstrates overlap in the frontoparietal neural circuits supporting action-related language and those supporting action execution (e.g., Harpaintner et al., 2020), mental representation of action (Péran et al., 2010), and AO (Meister and Iacoboni, 2007; Courson and Tremblay, 2020).

Challenging the idea that different action-types are similar in nature, are recent theoretical proposals of “dual-simulation” where, for example, AO and MI can hypothetically be simultaneously represented in the brain and recent research findings relating to the effects of various combinations of action types (Vogt et al., 2013; Eaves et al., 2016; Bruton et al., 2020). For instance, empirical evidence demonstrates facilitation of corticospinal excitability (CSE; an indication of activity in primary neural motor centres) when individuals simultaneously observe and imagine hand actions, which is greater than that found during AO alone (Wright et al., 2014; Cengiz et al., 2018; Kaneko et al., 2018; Bruton et al., 2020). The facilitation observed may be driven by MI rather than by the AO component of the combination, because evidence indicates that whether the imagined hand movement uses the same or different fingers to those observed, it is the imagined effector (and not that used in the observed action) that produces the facilitation effect (Meers et al., 2020). AO alone compared to baseline did not influence CSE which indicates some dissimilarity between AO and MI (Kaneko et al., 2018; Meers et al., 2020). Additionally, while early learning of coordinative and sequential actions can occur using AO (Boutin et al., 2010; Gonzalez-Rosa et al., 2015; Cuenca-Martínez et al., 2020), learning has been shown to be influenced by MI ability, in that, individuals with high imagery ability have been seen to perform significantly better following observational learning than those with low imagery ability (Lawrence et al., 2013). So, performance improvements following concurrent AO-MI use may be driven by the MI component. Notwithstanding this, practicing an action using simultaneous MI and AO can lead to better outcomes than either alone (see Scott et al., 2021; Bruton et al., 2020), and so, different action types may have unique processing components that allow them to complement each other in achieving positive effects (Vogt et al., 2013; Eaves et al., 2016). Extrapolating from the idea that there may be some level of uniqueness between action types, it might be that the existence of somewhat distinct but complementary processes also explains why training using physical practice interspersed with AO (Larssen et al., 2021; Bazzini et al., 2022) or MI (e.g., Allami et al., 2008; Malouin et al., 2013; Rozand et al., 2016) achieves better performance or movement retention outcomes than using physical practice alone. In a similar manner, behavioural evidence shows that combining action-related language with MI (Bonnet et al., 2022) or action execution (Larson and Suchy, 2015), improves performance on language comprehension tasks and motor sequence learning and control, respectively.

In support of some level of uniqueness across action types, research indicates that they may be dissociable in terms of the neural representations underlying their functioning (Lui et al., 2008; Péran et al., 2010; Macuga and Frey, 2012; Vry et al., 2012; Wang et al., 2022). In this regard, distinct neural signatures are evident across action types (Gerardin et al., 2000; Vry et al., 2012; Hardwick et al., 2018), with the amount and pattern of activation seemingly hierarchically organised, for example, in sensorimotor, SMA, and cerebellum regions, action execution displays the greatest increase in activity over resting baseline, followed by MI, and finally AO (Macuga and Frey, 2012). Further, and perhaps not surprisingly, covert action is typically associated with weaker neural activity than action execution (Sharma and Baron, 2013; Avanzino et al., 2015). Evidence relating to the neural substrate of language also indicates a motor gradation across different action types (see Courson and Tremblay, 2020). Specifically, discrepancies are evident in the shared network between mental representation of action and action-related language (Péran et al., 2010; Yang and Shu, 2014), with for example, activity in the overlapping frontoparietal network showing more involvement of parietal areas during action mental representation whereas action-related language appears more supported by frontal areas (Péran et al., 2010). Further, MI of hand-related verbs has been found to activate the SMA, an area important for the planning and sequencing of voluntary movement, more strongly than reading the same verbs (Cunnington et al., 2005; Yang and Shu, 2014).

In summary, despite accumulated evidence supporting the idea that cognitive and motor systems are elaborately interconnected, no single explanatory framework exists that fully captures the extent and/or nature of these interconnections. This is likely due, firstly, to the complexities associated with studying covert action processes, whereby overt behaviour is discouraged and so resourceful techniques and approaches are required to gain insight into different covert actions, and secondly, because the neurocognitive substrate of different action types is characteristically multidimensional, involving a large number of neuroanatomical locations and connections associated with information processing (Young et al., 1995). Given this, the primary aim of the present study is to reduce the high-dimensional neural substrate of four action-related behaviours to characterise and further elucidate the neurocognitive connections between them. To achieve this aim, a methodological approach that is novel in this field was adopted, non-metric multidimensional scaling, to transform the high-dimensional data so that they are projected onto fewer dimensions, that are graphically displayed and make any intrinsic patterns between the four action types more apparent (for a more comprehensive description of this method, see “Materials and method” section below; Pielou, 1984).

Based on existing literature, a number of predictions were made: (i) motor-cognitive simulation processes are covert and involve little or no physical action execution, and so, it is expected that some dissimilarity will be observed, as indicated by the distance between data points in ordination space on at least one dimension, between covert actions (i.e., MI, AO, and action-related language) and action execution; (ii) empirical evidence shows behavioural and neurophysiological overlap between MI and action execution (e.g., Di Rienzo et al., 2016), and so, it is expected that less dissimilarity will be observed between action execution and MI on at least one dimension, than between action execution and the covert action types of AO and action-related language; (iii) although AO promotes early learning of unfamiliar or novel actions (e.g., Gonzalez-Rosa et al., 2015), it is typically considered a perceptual-cognitive function (see Kim et al., 2017) with less neural activation in purely motor centres than other mental action types (e.g., Caspers et al., 2010; Hardwick et al., 2018), and so, it is expected that AO will be most dissimilar to action execution across dimensions; and finally; and (iv) given that links between action-related language and physical execution (e.g., Harpaintner et al., 2020) and AO (Meister and Iacoboni, 2007) have been independently found to positively influence language processing (i.e., Beauprez et al., 2020; Courson and Tremblay, 2020), it is expected that the data points in the dimensional space associated with action execution and AO will show some proximity to those of action-related language on at least one dimension. No predictions were formed in relation to combined AO-MI because it is a relatively new area for neurophysiological study. It seems likely however, that combined AO-MI will be proximal to MI and/or AO in ordination space on some dimension, thus representing similarity between the action types. Additionally, it could be anticipated that because action execution involves both perceptual (similar to AO) and predictive (similar to MI) processes for control of movement in the environment (e.g., Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Grush, 2004; Pezzulo et al., 2013; Deschrijver et al., 2017) there will be some proximity on at least one dimension between action execution and combined AO-MI.

Materials and Method

Literature search

PubMed database1 literature searches for neuroimaging articles were performed over 1 month (January 2022; up-dated in May 2022). Combined searches of terms relating to different action types and neuroimaging methods provided data for multidimensional modeling of dissimilarities between different covert action types and action execution processes. The primary search used the string (“motor imagery” OR “action observation” OR “action language” OR “verb”) AND (“fMRI” OR “neuroimaging” OR “PET”) NOT (“clinical” OR “children” OR “stroke” OR “review” OR “monkey” OR “MEG” OR “fNIRS” OR “neurofeedback” OR “primate” OR “BCI”), which returned 690 results (212 MI related; 233 AO related; 245 language related). Subsequent searches were more targeted towards AO+MI and language-related articles and used variations of this search string (e.g., “motor imagery” AND “action observation”, or “action” AND “language” AND “fMRI”) which returned 15 and 147 results, respectively. All articles (N = 852) were initially screened using information in the title and/or abstract (requiring information relating to action types and neuroimaging). Reference sections in reviewed articles and known articles (not returned in searches) were also screened for additional relevant studies. If action execution was used as a second or control condition in a study of a different action type it was included as a case of action execution in the non-metric multidimensional scaling analysis. The objective here was to limit any confounding effects associated with heterogeneity in covert and overt action tasks across studies (e.g., where differences in neural activation might be linked to experimental task). In the final selection of included studies, the studies that investigated more than one action type with the same task/movement protocol across actions were prioritised over those only studying one action type.

Inclusion—exclusion criteria

Of the 852 articles identified, those assessed for eligibility for inclusion in the final analysis were required to satisfy strict inclusion criteria, which were: (i) only studies performing whole brain analyses (not region of interest studies, as a focus on certain brain regions may bias the final model) with reporting of active regions including coordinates in standard stereotaxic space, such as MNI/Talairach, or clearly defined Brodmann areas; (ii) healthy adult participants; (iii) MI was “pure” MI in a kinaesthetic modality (i.e., not visual in nature); (iv) AO-MI was concurrent AO and MI as evidence suggests that this type is the most effective and used form for positive behavioural outcomes (e.g., sequential or coordinative AO-MI were not included; see Vogt et al., 2013); (v) articles were in English language; and (vi) neural activity during covert actions was compared to a rest or baseline state or a control task that was consistent across actions/conditions. In the selected articles, data extracted were statistically significant active brain areas during MI, AO, action-related language, and/or action execution. Note that laterality was not included as a variable in the dataset in sensorimotor-specific related areas as lateral brain activity can be strongly task and/or limb related.

The search led to 129 articles that fully met the inclusion criteria (38 AO; 33 MI; 24 action execution; 10 AO+MI; 24 action-related language). From these articles, a maximum of 100 articles was desired for input to the final multidimensional model. So, all articles investigating more than one action type were included first (minimising heterogeneity), and once this was achieved, any deficit in count was made up by randomly selecting from the remaining articles (to reach the desired 22 articles per action type). The only caveat to this random selection process was that if a meta-analysis existed among the remaining articles this was automatically included. The final dataset comprised 98 cases, that is, 98 covert action types (22 AO; 22 MI; 22 action execution; 22 action-related language; and 10 AO + MI) as studied and published across 53 scientific journal articles (for list of articles, see Table 1; note, each action type was examined for activity in 82 brain regions; for regions, see Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Published articles by author name included in the final multidimensional model.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions examined for activity during all action types (brain regions were identified according to their label and/or their Brodmann area, as reported in the articles).

Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS)

Non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) can be considered a method of statistical fitting that generates a spatial configuration whose distances between data points accurately reflect true dissimilarities in the data (Kruskal, 1964a,b; Spiridonov et al., 2019). The procedure involves firstly examining relationships in the data and generating a rank ordered matrix of the dissimilarities between all pairs of cases for the set of cases (each case in the present study represents a particular action type described by a number of neural attributes, i.e., brain regions that are activated or not during, e.g., MI). The goal is then to plot the data in a low-dimensional space with the minimum possible stress; with stress being a measure of how well the configuration matches the dissimilarity data (i.e., goodness-of-fit; Kruskal, 1964a). This is achieved by placing the data in either an initial arbitrary configuration or in a configuration where all data are equidistant, then determining in which direction stress is reducing most quickly and moving the data point structure in this direction (see Kruskal, 1964a,b). The procedure of determining the direction of most quickly decreasing stress (i.e., the steepest gradient) and moving the configuration in this direction is repeated as many times as necessary to arrive at a minimal stress value, and hence, an accurate spatial representation of the dissimilarities in the data. To note, unlike metric multidimensional scaling which uses absolute values of the dissimilarities in the data, NMDS only considers the rank ordering of the dissimilarities so that the distances in the final configuration are rank ordered to reflect the rank ordered data in the dissimilarity matrix (Goodhill et al., 1995). The overall aim of NMDS is to arrive at a solution or graphical/spatial model that faithfully represents the relationships between cases (i.e., action types) in a low-dimensional space so that underlying patterns (i.e., the multidimensional structure) can be identified and interpreted (for further description and uses, see Shepard, 1962; Kruskal, 1964a,b; Goodhill et al., 1995; Spiridonov et al., 2019).

The value of the NMDS modeling approach in the present study is that all neural raw data (i.e., activated neural regions in comparison to baseline/controls) across different studies and different action types can be inputted into a single model that maintains the high-dimensional status of data, but projects the dis/similarity between data in a low-dimensional space. Additionally, every dimension (i.e., neural region) associated with a specific action type in a particular study is taken into account when fitting and plotting the action type’s coordinate location relative to others in the model. Unlike other dimension reduction techniques, such as principal component analysis, NMDS can map binary data (i.e., brain areas that are activated or not) and capture dis/similarity between action types in fewer dimensions where all dimensions are visible and no dimension take priority over another.

In the present study, each action type or case is described in terms of activity across 82 brain regions (i.e., 82-dimensions) as objectively measured by scientific study. The inclusion of brain regions was based on motor control, visuomotor, and motor cognition literature. In the final dataset there were 98 action types (i.e., or cases; 22 MI, 22 AO, 22 action execution, 22 action-related language, and 10 AO-MI). Inclusion of specific brain regions was based on the motor-cognitive and motor control literature (for a list of included brain regions and studies, see Table 2). Non-metric scaling (rather than metric) was appropriate for the current data as they were at the ordinal measurement level (Shepard, 1962). Using NMDS, pairwise comparisons between all action-specific neural substrates (i.e., cases) were translated into a graphical representation in which the distances between cases reflect dissimilarity. The procedure: (i) calculates a proximity matrix of dissimilarities between all pairs of cases; (ii) rank orders these from smallest to largest; (iii) generates a set of coordinates for cases and rank orders the distances; and (iv) compares the ranked distances with the ranked proximities (Shepard, 1962). In the current analysis, data were transformed into a proximity matrix (at ordinal level; total proximities 4,753) using Lance-and-Williams Euclidean distance calculations (operating PROXSCAL in Statistical Package for the Social Sciences, SPSS 28, IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). The number of dimensions for the model was set to a minimum of two and a maximum of four. The initial configuration was Simplex, whereby all data are placed equidistant in the maximum dimension. From this, a maximum of 100 iterations (or repetitions to reduces stress and improve fit) were performed on the data. Stress plots were also generated.

Results

Non-metric multidimensional model

The NMDS of action-specific brain activation produced a good fitting 3-dimensional solution (normalised raw stress = 0.03621; Tucker’s coefficient of congruence = 0.98173) after 28 iterations. Figure 1 presents the final three-dimensional NMDS ordination plot of action-specific neural substrates. The distances between data points, or action types, show how dissimilar their neurocognitive substrate are, with greater distance indicating more dissimilarity. The solution in Figure 1 is displayed as three orthogonal projections, because the three-dimensional solution resembles a cube containing the data points which is less readily interpretable.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Orthogonal projection of the 3-dimensional non-metric multidimensional scaling configuration. Note: (A) x axis is dimension 1 and y axis is dimension 2. (B) z axis is dimension 3 and y axis is dimension 2. (C) x axis is dimension 1 and z axis is dimension 3. Each data point represents a particular action type described by neural attributes, i.e., brain regions that are activated or not. Different action types are displayed in different colours: blue, motor imagery; purple, action observation; orange, action execution; light brown, action related language; yellow, combined action observation and motor imagery.

Statistical analysis of action type coordinates in the three-dimensional model

To investigate whether the differences between the locations of action type points across the three-dimensions in the final model were statistically significant, a one-way ANOVA and post-hoc tests were performed. Firstly, the mean coordinates of data points in ordination space for each action type on each of the three dimensions were calculated (see Table 3 and Figure 2). Descriptive analysis of these mean coordinates in the first dimension on the x axis revealed that MI, action execution, and combined AO-MI were located in a negative direction from the origin (0), while AO and action-related language were located in a positive direction from the origin (Table 3 and Figure 2). Analysis of mean coordinates in the second dimension on the y axis revealed that that MI, AO, and action-related language were located in a positive direction from the origin while action execution and AO-MI were located in a negative direction (Table 3). A final descriptive analysis of mean coordinates in the third dimension on the z axis revealed that MI, AO, and combined AO-MI were located in a positive direction from the origin while action execution and action-related language were located in a negative direction (Table 3).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Data point coordinates across three dimensions for each action type. Note. Mean coordinates of data points for motor imagery, action observation, action execution, action-related language, and combined action observation and motor imagery across the three dimensions in the final non-metric multidimensional scaling model. Data points relating to an action type represent its neural attributes (i.e., brain regions that are activated or not). Error bars represent 95% confidence interval. * = statisitcally significant at 0.0017.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Mean coordinates of data points for action types across three dimensions.

While data were normally distributed in each of the dimensions (all Shapiro-Wilks tests p > 0.05), Levene’s test showed that the variances for action type coordinates in the second dimension on the y axis were not equal, F(4,93) = 3.155, p = 0.018, and so the assumption of homogeneity of variance was not met. Accordingly, one-way independent analysis of variance (ANOVA) using the Welch F test (a robust test of equality of means) was used to investigate the effect of action type on the spatial configuration of coordinates in each dimension. Results revealed significant differences in mean coordinates between action types in the first dimension on the x axis, Welch’s F(4,40.25) = 10.16, p < 0.001, the second dimension on the y axis, Welch’s F(4,39.57) = 4.47, p = 0.004, and in the third dimension on the z axis, Welch’s F(4,36.49) = 13.27, p < 0.001.

To investigate precisely where the differences lay, a series of pairwise comparisons of action type coordinates in each dimension were performed, using Games-Howell procedures for heterogeneity of variance. Family-wise error rate was minimised by adjusting the alpha value for the number of comparisons made, using a significance level of 0.0017. The results are presented in Table 4 and Figure 2. In the first dimension on the x axis, MI mean coordinate location (M = −0.29, SD = 0.40) was significantly different to the mean coordinate location of AO (M = 0.38, SD = 0.44), with a mean difference of −0.67, p < 0.001. Additionally, AO mean coordinate location (M = 0.38, SD = 0.44) was significantly different to the mean coordinate location of action execution (M = −0.19, SD = 0.26), with a mean difference of 0.57, p < 0.001. No significant differences were found (or survived correction procedures) in the mean coordinate locations of action types in the second dimension on the y axis. In the third dimension on the z axis, AO mean coordinate location (M = 0.19, SD = 0.25) was significantly different to the mean coordinate location of action execution (M = −0.25, SD = 0.16), with a mean difference of 0.44, p < 0.001.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of pairwise comparisons of action types across three dimensions.

Given the low adjusted significance value for multiple comparisons, the magnitude of the relationships between pairs of action type coordinates, in terms of dissimilarity, was examined through group-difference effect sizes (see Table 4). These can be interpreted using Cohen’s benchmarks: 0.20—small group-difference effect, 0.50—medium effect, 0.80—large effect (and Rosenthal’s 1.30 for very large effect). In the first dimension on the x axis, MI and AO, MI and action-related language, AO and action execution, AO and AO-MI, action execution and action-related language, and action-related language and AO-MI demonstrate large group-difference effects in mean coordinate location. In the second dimension on the y axis, MI and action execution, MI and AO-MI, action execution and action-related language, and action-related language and AO-MI demonstrate large group-difference effects. Finally, in the third dimension on the z axis, MI and action execution, AO and action execution, AO and action-related language, and action execution and AO-MI demonstrate large group-difference effects. These results will be considered in the “Discussion” section.

NMDS sensitivity analysis

To identify whether the NMDS solution accurately projected the 98 cases of action types (comprising 22 MI, 22 AO, 22 action-related language, 22 action execution, and 10 AO-MI) from the proximity matrix in to the lower dimensional space, while preserving the between-cases distances from the 82 dimensions (i.e., the 82 brain regions included in the model), we examined the normalised raw stress, S-stress values, the Dispersion Accounted For, and Tucker’s coefficient of congruence.

Dimensionality

The 2-dimension NMDS solution was compared to the 3-dimension solution to assess whether the goodness-of-fit was improved and if a higher dimensional solution revealed new relationships. In this regard, the 3-dimensional model had both better fit and clearer relationship patterns (e.g., see Figure 3 for a scree plot).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Screeplot of normalised raw stress associated with the three derived dimensional solutions. Note. The x axis displays the NMDS dimension solution for two, three, and four dimensions, and y axis displays the stress values associated with each dimensional solution, that is, how well the ranking of dissimilarities between cases correlates with the ranking of distances in the ordination structure. The elbow in the scree plot indicates the point at which there is a considerable decrease in stress which is typically assumed to indicate the best level of dimensionality for the ordination structure. In the present case, three dimensions appear to offer a good solution.

Stress

Goodness-of-fit values (e.g., stress) indicating the relationship between the ranking of ordination distances and the observed dissimilarities for both the 2-dimension solution and the 3-dimension solution were assessed in conjunction with a scree plot of normalised raw stress against dimensionality (see Figure 2). The stress value in the present study conveys the proportion of unexplained variance in the action type data by three NMDS axes (Spiridonov et al., 2019). A stress value of 0 indicates a perfect fit (i.e., no variance). The scree plot indicated an elbow at three dimensions, suggesting this is the most appropriate dimensional solution (note, an elbow is a rule of thumb indicator of the appropriate dimensional solution). Three dimensions improved the normalised raw stress by 0.04225, reducing the 2-dimension normalised raw stress of 0.07881 to the 3-dimension normalised raw stress of 0.03621. Additional measures of fit for the 3-dimenisonal solution were Dispersion Accounted For (0.96379) and Tucker’s coefficient of congruence (0.98173). A value of 1 for these measures indicates a perfect fit. Overall, the 3-dimensional solution had very good fit.

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to characterise the interconnections between different covert and overt action types in terms of their activity in the brain (i.e., their neurocognitive relational connections). No single study has yet directly mapped the neurocognitive dis/similarity between motor imagery (MI), action observation (AO), action execution, and action-related language outside direct neurophysiological study, and so, the present study used the powerful statistical technique of non-metric multidimensional scaling (NMDS) to further elucidate the neurocognitive relational links between each of these action types. The value of NMDS modeling in this instance is that every dimension (i.e., neural region) associated with a specific action type in a particular study is taken into account when plotting the action type’s coordinate location relative to others in the model. As NMDS operates according to the idea that similarity relates to proximity (i.e., we consider items similar because they are close in some attribute, the colour blue might be similar to navy but dissimilar to yellow; Shepard, 1962), the relative locations of data points associated with each action type in the ordination plot were examined.

The resultant 3-dimensional model highlights some salient features regarding the intrinsic relational patterns across different action types. Specifically, the four action types of MI, AO, action execution, and action-related language were observed to cluster in four main groups across the three dimensions (Figure 1). The locations of the four clusters in the overall spatial configuration across the dimensions were found to be significantly different and so highlight dissimilarities between the various action types on three neurocognitive dimensions. Clustering indicates that each action type possesses some unique attributes in relation to their neurocognitive substrate, which is consistent with existing neurophysiological evidence (Macuga and Frey, 2012; Hardwick et al., 2018; Courson and Tremblay, 2020). However, when the average locations of each action type on each of the three dimensions were compared, only action execution and AO (in the first and third dimensions on the x and z axes, respectively) and MI and AO (in the first dimension on the x axis) showed significant dissimilarity (Table 4; Figure 2). Given the multiple comparisons performed across action types and dimensions during post hoc statistical analysis, effect sizes for group differences in location in the three-dimensional ordination space between pairs of action types (as calculated in the NMDS model) were used to further quantify and interpret interconnections. Large location difference, or dissimilarity, effect sizes were observed for each action type against all other action types on at least one dimension (see Table 4). For example, MI displayed large dissimilarity effects sizes (i.e., mean coordinates differed greatly in their dimensional location in the model) between AO and action-related language (in the first dimension on the x axis), between action execution (in the second and third dimensions on the y and z axes), and between AO-MI (in the second dimension on the y axis). Differing on at least one dimension suggests that each action type has some unique neurocognitive characteristic that contributes to behaviour. Notwithstanding this, most action types were also found to be similar (i.e., with small dissimilarity effect size) to all other action types on at least one neurocognitive dimension, the exception to this being action-related language (which had medium to large dissimilarity with all action types). Similarity across action types on at least one dimension is consistent with neurophysiological findings of overlapping brain regions (Caspers et al., 2010; Hétu et al., 2013; Hardwick et al., 2018). The finding of both unique and similar neurocognitive dimensions across action types assists in explaining why alternating or combining action types during skill learning or practice contributes to positive outcomes over and above those observed using a single action type (Larson and Suchy, 2015; Caligiore et al., 2017; Larssen et al., 2021; Scott et al., 2021; Bazzini et al., 2022). Overall, the 3-dimensional model shows that across all action types, action execution and concurrent AO-MI were most similar, as indicated by the minimal to small dissimilarity effect sizes on two of the three dimensions in the configuration (see Table 4). Additionally, the most dissimilar action types were action execution and AO, as indicated by the very large dissimilarity effect sizes in the first and third dimensions in the model configuration (Table 4). The following section will discuss specific findings in more detail.

The finding of significant dissimilarity between action execution and AO was predicted, and based on the consideration of AO as a perceptual-cognitive function (see Kim et al., 2017), with less neural activation in purely motor centres than other action types (e.g., Caspers et al., 2010; Hardwick et al., 2018), and the absent corticospinal excitability during AO (Meers et al., 2020). However, the extent of dissimilarity (i.e., across all three dimensions) is also somewhat surprising given that research demonstrates, for example, that using AO interventions during limb immobilisation can mitigate associated negative effects by preserving sensorimotor cortical excitability during the immobilised period (Bassolino et al., 2014) and safeguarding motor performance (De Marco et al., 2021). Additionally, AO is known to be effective in learning novel actions, particularly when these involve coordination or sequencing actions (Boutin et al., 2010; Gatti et al., 2013; Gonzalez-Rosa et al., 2015; Cuenca-Martínez et al., 2020). Indeed, AO has been shown to outperform MI in learning unfamiliar actions (Gatti et al., 2013; Gonzalez-Rosa et al., 2015). Further, when observing actions that are familiar to us, the content of mental representation during AO appears to include both visual and motoric components (Calvo-Merino et al., 2006). Consequently, it cannot be discounted in the present study that the considerable dissimilarity between AO and action execution may reflect the context in which AO was performed in the neurophysiological studies entered into the NMDS analysis. In this regard, only studies that used AO in the context of passive observation of action were included for analysis, due to the possibility that AO in the context of, for example, subsequent intention to imitate may encourage active motor processing or implicit MI. Furthermore, research indicates that the tasks and context associated with AO appear to modify the processes engaged during AO, in that, different strategies may be adopted, some of which are less motoric (or not motoric at all) than others (Hodges, 2017). It is worth noting that evidence suggests that AO activates motor centres (thus displaying neural overlap with action execution) to a greater extent when biological vs. non-biological movements are observed, or when movements are familiar rather than unfamiliar, and so, the importance of the context in which AO is performed appears to be an important consideration (Holz et al., 2008; Zentgraf et al., 2011).

It is interesting that AO was found to be most similar to MI over all other action types (as indicated by the small dissimilarity effects between AO and MI on two of the three dimensions; albeit having a significant large dissimilarity effect with MI on the first dimension). The similarity between AO and MI on two dimensions (i.e., dimensions 2 and 3 on the y and z axes, respectively) may reflect their mental status (i.e., their reliance on cognitive systems, such as memory, attention, etc.) or their role in the cognitive transformation of motor-related information via a simulation mechanism (Jeannerod, 2001, 2006). This idea is somewhat supported by the observation that on these same two dimensions MI and action execution, and AO and action execution, show dissimilarity, and so, the similarity between AO and MI (in conjunction with the dissimilarity between MI/AO and action execution) appears to relate more to cognitive aspects of action rather than motoric. The interconnection between AO and MI, and their mutual dissimilarity to action execution on two neurocognitive dimensions, supports previous research demonstrating that learning a motor skill via MI relies more on perceptual processes than does learning via action execution (Ingram et al., 2016).

Notwithstanding the similarity between AO and MI on two dimensions, it is important to note that MI and AO differ significantly on the first dimension (see Figure 2; Table 4). In this dimension, MI was most similar to action execution, as indicated by their proximity in the ordination model (and small dissimilarity effect sizes), which suggests that this first neurocognitive dimension may be motoric in nature (this is further discussed below). Given the extent of empirical evidence demonstrating positive effects of MI use on motor behaviour and neuroplastic change (Pascual-Leone et al., 1995; Debarnot et al., 2014; Di Rienzo et al., 2016), it was predicted that MI would be similar to action execution on at least one dimension, and this finding supports this.

Overall, it was expected that action execution would display some dissimilarity to all covert action types (i.e., MI, AO, action-related language), given the mental–physical gradient between them (Jeannerod, 2006; Glover and Baran, 2017). This prediction was largely satisfied as indicated by large dissimilarity effect sizes in at least two dimensions between action execution and each action type (an exception being AO-MI). As already stated, action execution was more dissimilar to AO than any other action type across the three dimensions. While execution was similar to MI on the first dimension, it is interesting that in the present study, action execution was most similar to concurrent AO-MI than any other action type, as indicated by the minimal to small dissimilarity effect sizes in location on two of the three dimensions in the configuration (see Table 4). Action execution involves both perceptual and predictive processes for control of movement in the environment (e.g., Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000; Grush, 2004; Pezzulo et al., 2013), and according to the similarities observed in the NMDS model presented here (see Figure 1) it may be that combined AO-MI also largely capture these qualities. Research demonstrates that with walking actions, gait phase dependent modulation of cortical activity in sensorimotor areas (as measured by electroencephalography; EEG) during combined AO-MI, was more similar to that during execution of the same action than was AO alone (Berends et al., 2013; Kaneko et al., 2021). Further, concurrent AO-MI (during balance control) appears to activate neural motor centres (e.g., M1, PMC, SMA, and cerebellum) to a greater extent than either MI or AO alone (Taube et al., 2015), and also enhance corticospinal excitability (Sakamoto et al., 2009). When examining primary motor cortex (M1) activity, 9% of AO-related studies and 100% of action execution studies included in the NMDS analysis indicated activity in the M1. Additionally, 27% of MI studies and 40% of concurrent AO-MI studies included in the model displayed such activity. This example of neural motor activity patterns highlights a superadditive effect (in AO-MI), where using action types concurrently, or interspersed, appears to have greater influence on brain activity and/or movement performance than using either alone (e.g., Romano-Smith et al., 2018; Larssen et al., 2021; Bazzini et al., 2022; for review see Scott et al., 2021). The greater movement skill improvement typically observed when physical performance is interspersed either AO or MI may occur because somewhat different but complementary processes are refined during covert and overt action which ultimately leads to superior behaviour (Allami et al., 2008; Malouin et al., 2013; Larssen et al., 2021).

The first dimension displays similarity (i.e., small effect sizes in location difference in the ordination model) between action execution and MI and AO-MI, and so, it may be that this dimension can be characterised as motoric in nature. When the similarity between action execution and MI in the first dimension is interpreted in conjunction with the observation of large dissimilarities between AO and AO-MI and between AO and MI and small group differences in location between MI and AO-MI and between action execution and AO-MI, it appears that in AO-MI on this dimension it is MI that is primarily driving processing, thus supporting the previous research finding that MI during AO-MI drives corticospinal excitability (see Meers et al., 2020). The apparent motoric nature of MI in this dimension may relate to the predictive forward modeling mechanism of motor control theories (i.e., efference copy; Wolpert and Ghahramani, 2000) given the offline or cognitive status of MI. It is also interesting that the motoric quality of this dimension is further implied by the location of action-related language (assumed to be a motor-related but more cognitive process) which displays large group differences in proximity to the locations of MI, action execution, and AO-MI.

It was further predicted in the present study that action-related language will show some similarity to AO and action execution on at least one dimension. This prediction was based on the observation that previous research indicates that action-related words trigger activation of the motor system (Pulvermüller et al., 2005; Pulvermüller, 2005, 2018; Jirak et al., 2010; Courson and Tremblay, 2020) and that AO or action execution prime action-related language processing (Beauprez et al., 2020; Bidet-Ildei et al., 2020). The findings relating to the configuration of the NMDS model indicate that this prediction is not supported, as action-related language shows medium to large dissimilarity with all action types across the three dimensions (see Table 4). Given that previous research findings suggest functional links between sensorimotor systems and language systems (e.g., Pulvermüller et al., 2005; Larson and Suchy, 2015; Bonnet et al., 2022), it is possible that the action-related language tasks used in the studies entered into the model in the present study influenced the outcome. Specifically, although research demonstrates the role of sensorimotor brain areas in word recognition, there are indications that context, experience, or familiarity with action influence the strength of link between action and language (e.g., with motor experience, language processing is facilitated, for example, with faster times for word recognition; Pulvermüller et al., 2005; Holt and Beilock, 2006; Beauprez et al., 2019, 2020). Accordingly, context may determine the extent of involvement or mediation of sensorimotor systems in language processing (Arbib et al., 2014; Moreno et al., 2015). Facilitative effects of motor experience may be linked to the operation of associative mechanisms (e.g., Pulvermüller, 2005) or simulation mechanisms (Jeannerod, 2006; Barsalou, 2008), which elaborate action representational content over time, and could account for the positive effects of MI training on semantic access/categorisation tasks (Bonnet et al., 2022) or AO training on verb processing (Beauprez et al., 2020). Overall, it appears that the relational connections between action-related language and other action types is complex and will require further nuanced investigation to unravel the specificities of any links that may exist. For example, it will be useful to fully understand the temporal organisation of processing associated with each action type in conjunction with action-related language processing, as this may provide insight into whether action-related language is more related to cognitive dimensions than motor dimensions, and whether action words are understood and processed prior to, simultaneously with, or after activation of motor systems (e.g., Moreno et al., 2015; Zappa et al., 2019).

There are some methodological limitations in the present study. Firstly, the data for the study was based on data previously generated across different neuroimaging studies. One drawback of this is that different tasks and experimental requirements were involved which may have impacted the extent or type of brain activation recorded. Although this potential confound is mentioned here, the present analysis included 33 articles (out the total 53) that studied more than one action type using the same experimental conditions, and so the impact of such a confound is mitigated. Secondly, a single author searched the literature for inclusion of appropriate studies and reviewed the activated brain areas described, which may have led to missed or overlooked potentially viable articles or brain activations. However, a single reviewer ensured consistency across selection procedures and so has advantages. Finally, the number of cases of AO+MI entered into the model was only half that of the number of cases entered for other action types. Unfortunately, although this does not impact the NMDS solution calculation, it reflects the limited amount of neuroimaging research on this particular action combination.

The present study highlights both similarities and dissimilarities between four action types on three dimensions, which somewhat supports the idea of complementarity. However, given the potentially important implications of using action types in combination (either simultaneously or alternating) for movement skill improvement or rehabilitation, it is important that future research strives to fully understand how best to integrate different action types across different tasks, contexts, and disciplines. In this regard, recent attempts to integrate different action types into a single method for rehabilitation of language (e.g., Durand et al., 2021) or motor function in the clinical (e.g., Bek et al., 2021; for review see Caligiore et al., 2017) and sport (e.g., see Wright et al., 2021) domains represent important avenues.

Conclusion

The primary aim of this study was to use non-metric multidimensional scaling to reduce the dimensions, and thus complexity, of the neurocognitive substrate of the four different action types of motor imagery (MI), action observation (AO), action execution, and action-related language, to identify the least number of dimensions that distinguish or relate them. Understanding their relational connections contributes to a better understanding of the neurocognitive mechanisms driving their functioning, and the extent to which these mechanisms might be shared by the different action types. Taking the results of the current study together, three main findings are reported. First, action execution and AO (in the first and third dimensions on the x and z axes, respectively) and MI and AO (in the first dimension on the x axis) showed significant dissimilarity, with action execution and AO displaying the most dissimilarity across all other action types. Second, in the three-dimensional model, across all action types, action execution and concurrent AO-MI were most similar. Finally, action-related language demonstrated the greatest dissimilarity with all action types across the three dimensions in the model. Overall, each action type showed some unique neurocognitive characteristics (with dissimilarity to all other action types on at least one dimension) that likely contributes to the optimal functioning of the perceptual-cognitive-motor system, allowing humans to perform, observe, learn, imagine, and reason about action-related information. The findings herein position four different action types in a shared three dimensional space that highlights the extent of their neurocognitive commonalities and dissimilarities. By doing so, it is anticipated that this will assist in understanding how best to exploit the various interconnections between these different covert actions in the rehabilitation of any one that is dysfunctional. Recent work exploiting the combination of AO and MI for application in clinical (e.g., Scott et al., 2021) and sport domains (see Wright et al., 2021), and the combination of action observation, execution, and imagery in assisting fluency in action-related language (e.g., Durand et al., 2021) are promising directions. As yet a single comprehensive theory of motor cognition that accounts for the accumulated evidence (and for different but overlapping hypotheses or theories) has yet to emerge. Future research might concentrate efforts on further cultivating and testing a comprehensive theory of action-related functions, with particular focus on whether they are best described and explained by motoric or non-motoric conceptualisations.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

HO’S is the sole intellectual contributor to the work.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov

References

Allami, N., Paulignan, Y., Brovelli, A., and Boussaoud, D. (2008). Visuo-motor learning with combination of different rates of motor imagery and physical practice. Exp. Brain Res. 184, 105–113. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1086-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbib, M. A., Gasser, B., and Barrès, V. (2014). Language is handy but is it embodied? Neuropsychologia 55, 57–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avanzino, L., Gueugneau, N., Bisio, A., Ruggeri, P., Papaxanthis, C., and Bove, M. (2015). Motor cortical plasticity induced by motor learning through mental practice. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 9:105. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baeck, J. S., Kim, Y. T., Seo, J. H., Ryeom, H. K., Lee, J., Choi, S. M., et al. (2012). Brain activation patterns of motor imagery reflect plastic changes associated with intensive shooting training. Behav. Brain Res. 234, 26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 577–609. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x99002149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 59, 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassolino, M., Campanella, M., Bove, M., Pozzo, T., and Fadiga, L. (2014). Training the motor cortex by observing the actions of others during immobilization. Cereb. Cortex 24, 3268–3276. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumann, A., Tödt, I., Knutzen, A., Gless, C. A., Granert, O., Wolff, S., et al. (2022). Neural correlates of executed compared to imagined writing and drawing movements: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:829576. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.829576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baumgaertner, A., Buccino, G., Lange, R., McNamara, A., and Binkofski, F. (2007). Polymodal conceptual processing of human biological actions in the left inferior frontal lobe. Eur. J. Neurosci. 25, 881–889. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05346.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bazzini, M. C., Nuara, A., Scalona, E., De Marco, D., Rizzolatti, G., Avanzini, P., et al. (2022). The proactive synergy between action observation and execution in the acquisition of new motor skills. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:793849. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.793849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauprez, S. A., Blandin, Y., Almecija, Y., and Bidet-Ildei, C. (2020). Physical and observational practices of unusual actions prime action verb processing. Brain Cogn. 138:103630. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.103630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauprez, S. A., Laroche, B., Perret, C., and Bidet-Ildei, C. (2019). How action context modulates the action-language relationship: a topographic ERP analysis. Brain Topogr. 32, 794–807. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00722-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bek, J., Holmes, P. S., Craig, C. E., Franklin, Z. C., Sullivan, M., Webb, J., et al. (2021). Action imagery and observation in neurorehabilitation for Parkinson’s disease (ACTION-PD): development of a user-informed home training intervention to improve functional hand movements. Parkinsons Dis. 2021:4559519. doi: 10.1155/2021/4559519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berends, H. I., Wolkorte, R., Ijzerman, M. J., and van Putten, M. J. (2013). Differential cortical activation during observation and observation-and-imagination. Exp. Brain Res. 229, 337–345. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3571-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berlingeri, M., Crepaldi, D., Roberti, R., Scialfa, G., Luzzatti, C., Paulesu, E., et al. (2008). Nouns and verbs in the brain: grammatical class and task specific effects as revealed by fMRI. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 25, 528–558. doi: 10.1080/02643290701674943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beudel, M., Zijlstra, S., Mulder, T., Zijdewind, I., and de Jong, B. M. (2011). Secondary sensory area SII is crucially involved in the preparation of familiar movements compared to movements never made before. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32, 564–579. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidet-Ildei, C., Beauprez, S.-A., and Badets, A. (2020). A review of literature on the link between action observation and action language: advancing a shared semantic theory. New Ideas Psychol. 58:100777. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2019.100777

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bonnet, C., Bayram, M., El Bouzaïdi Tiali, S., Lebon, F., Harquel, S., Palluel-Germain, R., et al. (2022). Kinesthetic motor-imagery training improves performance on lexical-semantic access. PloS One 17:e0270352. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0270352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boutin, A., Fries, U., Panzer, S., Shea, C. H., and Blandin, Y. (2010). Role of action observation and action in sequence learning and coding. Acta Psychol. (Amst) 135, 240–251. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brihmat, N., Tarri, M., Quidé, Y., Anglio, K., Pavard, B., Castel-Lacanal, E., et al. (2018). Action, observation or imitation of virtual hand movement affect differently regions of the mirror neuron system and the default mode network. Brain Imaging Behav. 12, 1363–1378. doi: 10.1007/s11682-017-9804-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruton, A. M., Holmes, P. S., Eaves, D. L., Franklin, Z. C., and Wright, D. J. (2020). Neurophysiological markers discriminate different forms of motor imagery during action observation. Cortex 124, 119–136. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caligiore, D., Mustile, M., Spalletta, G., and Baldassarre, G. (2017). Action observation and motor imagery for rehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and an integrative hypothesis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 72, 210–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvo-Merino, B., Grèzes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, R. E., and Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr. Biol. 16, 1905–1910. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Casiraghi, L., Alahmadi, A., Monteverdi, A., Palesi, F., Castellazzi, G., Savini, G., et al. (2019). I see your effort: force-related bold effects in an extended action execution-observation network involving the cerebellum. Cereb. Cortex 29, 1351–1368. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caspers, S., Zilles, K., Laird, A. R., and Eickhoff, S. B. (2010). ALE meta-analysis of action observation and imitation in the human brain. Neuroimage 50, 1148–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cengiz, B., Vurallı, D., Zinnuroğlu, M., Bayer, G., Golmohammadzadeh, H., Günendi, Z., et al. (2018). Analysis of mirror neuron system activation during action observation alone and action observation with motor imagery tasks. Exp. Brain Res. 236, 497–503. doi: 10.1007/s00221-017-5147-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courson, M., and Tremblay, P. (2020). Neural correlates of manual action language: comparative review, ALE meta-analysis and ROI meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 116, 221–238. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.06.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F., and Grafton, S. T. (2006). Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31, 1257–1267. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cuenca-Martínez, F., Suso-Martí, L., León-Hernández, J. V., and La Touche, R. (2020). Effects of movement representation techniques on motor learning of thumb-opposition tasks. Sci. Rep. 10:12267. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-67905-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cunnington, R., Windischberger, C., and Moser, E. (2005). Premovement activity of the pre-supplementary motor area and the readiness for action: studies of time-resolved event-related functional MRI. Hum. Mov. Sci. 24, 644–656. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2005.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Debarnot, U., Sperduti, M., Di Rienzo, F., and Guillot, A. (2014). Experts bodies, experts minds: how physical and mental training shape the brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:280. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J. (1996). Do imagined and executed actions share the same neural substrate? Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 3, 87–93. doi: 10.1016/0926-6410(95)00033-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Grauwe, S., Willems, R. M., Rueschemeyer, S. A., Lemhöfer, K., and Schriefers, H. (2014). Embodied language in first- and second-language speakers: neural correlates of processing motor verbs. Neuropsychologia 56, 334–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Marco, D., Scalona, E., Bazzini, M. C., Nuara, A., Taglione, E., Lopomo, N. F., et al. (2021). Observation of others’ actions during limb immobilization prevents the subsequent decay of motor performance. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U S A 118:e2025979118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2025979118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Vega, M., León, I., Hernández, J. A., Valdés, M., Padrón, I., Ferstl, E. C., et al. (2014). Action sentences activate sensory motor regions in the brain independently of their status of reality. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1363–1376. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desai, R. H., Conant, L. L., Binder, J. R., Park, H., and Seidenberg, M. S. (2013). A piece of the action: modulation of sensory-motor regions by action idioms and metaphors. Neuroimage 83, 862–869. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deschrijver, E., Wiersema, J. R., and Brass, M. (2017). The influence of action observation on action execution: dissociating the contribution of action on perception, perception on action and resolving conflict. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 17, 381–393. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0485-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Rienzo, F., Debarnot, U., Daligault, S., Saruco, E., Delpuech, C., Doyon, J., et al. (2016). Online and offline performance gains following motor imagery practice: a comprehensive review of behavioural and neuroimaging 160 studies. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:315. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durand, E., Masson-Trottier, M., Sontheimer, A., and Ansaldo, A. I. (2021). Increased links between language and motor areas: a proof-of-concept study on resting-state functional connectivity following personalized observation, execution and mental imagery therapy in chronic aphasia. Brain Cogn. 148:105659. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2020.105659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eaves, D. L., Riach, M., Holmes, P. S., and Wright, D. J. (2016). Motor imagery during action observation: a brief review of evidence, theory and future research opportunities. Front. Neurosci. 10:514. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elli, G. V., Lane, C., and Bedny, M. (2019). A double dissociation in sensitivity to verb and noun semantics across cortical networks. Cereb. Cortex 29, 4803–4817. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frith, C., and Dolan, R. (1996). The role of the prefrontal cortex in higher cognitive functions. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 5, 175–181. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(96)00054-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garbin, G., Collina, S., and Tabossi, P. (2012). Argument structure and morphological factors in noun and verb processing: an fMRI study. PloS One 7:e45091. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerardin, E., Sirigu, A., Lehéricy, S., Poline, J. B., Gaymard, B., Marsault, C., et al. (2000). Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. Cereb. Cortex 10, 1093–1104. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.11.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gatti, R., Tettamanti, A., Gough, P. M., Riboldi, E., Marinoni, L., Buccino, G., et al. (2013). Action observation versus motor imagery in learning a complex motor task: a short review of literature and a kinematics study. Neurosci. Lett. 540, 37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glover, S., and Baran, M. (2017). The motor-cognitive model of motor imagery: evidence from timing errors in simulated reaching and grasping. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 43, 1359–1375. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000389

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gonzalez-Rosa, J. J., Natali, F., Tettamanti, A., Cursi, M., Velikova, S., Comi, G., et al. (2015). Action observation and motor imagery in performance of complex movements: evidence from EEG and kinematics analysis. Behav. Brain Res. 281, 290–300. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodhill, G. J., Simmen, M. W., and Willshaw, D. (1995). An evaluation of the use of multidimensional scaling for understanding brain connectivity. Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 348, 265–280. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1995.0068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grèzes, J., and Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation and verb generation of actions: a meta-analysis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 12, 1–19. doi: 10.1002/1097-0193(200101)12:1<1::aid-hbm10>3.0.co;2-v

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: motor control, imagery and perception. Behav. Brain Sci. 27, 377–442. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x04000093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanakawa, T., Immisch, I., Toma, K., Dimyan, M. A., Gelderen, P. V., Hallett, M., et al. (2003). Functional properties of brain areas associated with motor execution and imagery. J. Neurophysiol. 89, 989–1002. doi: 10.1152/jn.00132.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hardwick, R. M., Caspers, S., B Eickhoff, S., and Swinnen, S. P. (2018). Neural correlates of action: comparing meta-analyses of imagery, observation and execution. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 94, 31–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harpaintner, M., Sim, E. J., Trumpp, N. M., Ulrich, M., and Kiefer, M. (2020). The grounding of abstract concepts in the motor and visual system: an fMRI study. Cortex 124, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.10.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., and Pulvermüller, F. (2004). Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron 41, 301–307. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00838-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hernández, M., Fairhall, S. L., Lenci, A., Baroni, M., and Caramazza, A. (2014). Predication drives verb cortical signatures. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 26, 1829–1839. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hesslow, G. (2012). The current status of the simulation theory of cognition. Brain Res. 1428, 71–79. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hétu, S., Grégoire, M., Saimpont, A., Coll, M. P., Eugène, F., Michon, P. E., et al. (2013). The neural network of motor imagery: an ALE meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 37, 930–949. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.03.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Higuchi, S., Holle, H., Roberts, N., Eickhoff, S. B., and Vogt, S. (2012). Imitation and observational learning of hand actions: prefrontal involvement and connectivity. Neuroimage 59, 1668–1683. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodges, N. J. (2017). Observations on action-observation research: an autobiographical retrospective across the past two decades. Kinesiol. Rev. 6, 240–260. doi: 10.1123/kr.2017-0016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holt, L. E., and Beilock, S. L. (2006). Expertise and its embodiment: examining the impact of sensorimotor skill expertise on the representation of action-related text. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 13, 694–701. doi: 10.3758/bf03193983

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holz, E. M., Doppelmayr, M., Klimesch, W., and Sauseng, P. (2008). EEG correlates of action observation in humans. Brain Topogr. 21, 93–99. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0066-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., and Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. Behav. Brain Sci. 24, 849–937. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x01000103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hommel, B. (2019). Theory of event coding (TEC) V2.0: representing and controlling perception and action. Atten. Percept. Psychophys. 81, 2139–2154. doi: 10.3758/s13414-019-01779-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hout, M. C., Papesh, M. H., and Goldinger, S. D. (2013). Multidimensional scaling. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 4, 93–103. doi: 10.1002/wcs.1203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingram, T. G., Kraeutner, S. N., Solomon, J. P., Westwood, D. A., and Boe, S. G. (2016). Skill acquisition via motor imagery relies on both motor and perceptual learning. Behav. Neurosci. 130, 252–260. doi: 10.1037/bne0000126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ingram, T. G. J., Solomon, J. P., Westwood, D. A., and Boe, S. G. (2019). Movement related sensory feedback is not necessary for learning to execute a motor skill. Behav. Brain Res. 359, 135–142. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iseki, K., Hanakawa, T., Shinozaki, J., Nankaku, M., and Fukuyama, H. (2008). Neural mechanisms involved in mental imagery and observation of gait. Neuroimage 41, 1021–1031. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behav. Brain Sci. 17:2. 187–202. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00034026

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: a unifying mechanism for motor cognition. Neuroimage 14, S103–S109. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Jirak, D., Menz, M. M., Buccino, G., Borghi, A. M., and Binkofski, F. (2010). Grasping language–a short story on embodiment. Conscious. Cogn. 19, 711–720. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaneko, N., Masugi, Y., Usuda, N., Yokoyama, H., and Nakazawa, K. (2018). Modulation of hoffmann reflex excitability during action observation of walking with and without motor imagery. Neurosci. Lett. 684, 218–222. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2018.07.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaneko, N., Yokoyama, H., Masugi, Y., Watanabe, K., and Nakazawa, K. (2021). Phase dependent modulation of cortical activity during action observation and motor imagery of walking: an EEG study. Neuroimage 225:117486. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117486

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilintari, M., Narayana, S., Babajani-Feremi, A., Rezaie, R., and Papanicolaou, A. C. (2016). Brain activation profiles during kinesthetic and visual imagery: an fMRI study. Brain Res. 1646, 249–261. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilintari, M., Raos, V., and Savaki, H. E. (2014). Involvement of the superior temporal cortex in action execution and action observation. J. Neurosci. 34, 8999–9011. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0736-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kilteni, K., Andersson, B. J., Houborg, C., and Ehrsson, H. H. (2018). Motor imagery involves predicting the sensory consequences of the imagined movement. Nat. Commun. 9:1617. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-03989-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kim, T., Frank, C., and Schack, T. (2017). A systematic investigation of the effect of action observation training and motor imagery training on the development of mental representation structure and skill performance. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:499. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraeutner, S. N., MacKenzie, L. A., Westwood, D. A., and Boe, S. G. (2016). Characterizing skill acquisition through motor imagery with no prior physical practice. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 42, 257–265. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruskal, J. B. (1964a). Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a nonmetric hypothesis. Psychometrika 29, 1–27. doi: 10.1007/BF02289565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruskal, J. B. (1964b). Nonmetric multidimensional scaling: a numerical method. Psychometrika 29, 115–129. doi: 10.1007/BF02289694

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhnke, P., Kiefer, M., and Hartwigsen, G. (2020). Task-dependent recruitment of modality-specific and multimodal regions during conceptual processing. Cereb. Cortex 30, 3938–3959. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J. P., Mahnkopf, C., Holzknecht, C., Siebner, H., Ulmer, S., Jansen, O., et al. (2003). Effector-independent representations of simple and complex imagined finger movements: a combined fMRI and TMS study. Eur. J. Neurosci. 18, 3375–3387. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03066.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Land, W. M., Volchenkov, D., Bläsing, B. E., and Schack, T. (2013). From action representation to action execution: exploring the links between cognitive and biomechanical levels of motor control. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7:127. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larson, J. C. G., and Suchy, Y. (2015). The contribution of verbalization to action. Psychol. Res. 79, 590–608. doi: 10.1007/s00426-014-0586-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larssen, B. C., Ho, D. K., Kraeutner, S. N., and Hodges, N. J. (2021). Combining observation and physical practice: benefits of an interleaved schedule for visuomotor adaptation and motor memory consolidation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 15:614452. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2021.614452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawrence, G., Callow, N., and Roberts, R. (2013). Watch me if you can: imagery ability moderates observational learning effectiveness. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:522. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lindenberg, R., Uhlig, M., Scherfeld, D., Schlaug, G., and Seitz, R. J. (2012). Communication with emblematic gestures: shared and distinct neural correlates of expression and reception. Hum. Brain Mapp. 33, 812–823. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lui, F., Buccino, G., Duzzi, D., Benuzzi, F., Crisi, G., Baraldi, P., et al. (2008). Neural substrates for observing and imagining non-object-directed actions. Soc. Neurosci. 3, 261–275. doi: 10.1080/17470910701458551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macuga, K. L., and Frey, S. H. (2012). Neural representations involved in observed, imagined and imitated actions are dissociable and hierarchically organized. Neuroimage 59, 2798–2807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malouin, F., Jackson, P. L., and Richards, C. L. (2013). Towards the integration of mental practice in rehabilitation programs. A critical review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:576. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meers, R., Nuttall, H. E., and Vogt, S. (2020). Motor imagery alone drives corticospinal excitability during concurrent action observation and motor imagery. Cortex 126, 322–333. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meister, I. G., and Iacoboni, M. (2007). No language-specific activation during linguistic processing of observed actions. PloS One 2:e891. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moreno, I., de Vega, M., León, I., Bastiaansen, M., Glen Lewis, A., Magyari, L., et al. (2015). Brain dynamics in the comprehension of action-related language. A time-frequency analysis of mu rhythms. Neuroimage 109, 50–62. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mouthon, A., Ruffieux, J., Mouthon, M., Hoogewoud, H. M., Annoni, J. M., Taube, W., et al. (2018). Age-related differences in cortical and subcortical activities during observation and motor imagery of dynamic postural tasks: an fMRI study. Neural Plast. 2018:1598178. doi: 10.1155/2018/1598178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nedelko, V., Hassa, T., Hamzei, F., Schoenfeld, M. A., and Dettmers, C. (2012). Action imagery combined with action observation activates more corticomotor regions than action observation alone. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 36, 182–188. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0b013e318272cad1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nedelko, V., Hassa, T., Hamzei, F., Weiller, C., Binkofski, F., Schoenfeld, M. A., et al. (2010). Age-independent activation in areas of the mirror neuron system during action observation and action imagery. A fMRI study. Restor. Neurol. Neurosci. 28, 737–747. doi: 10.3233/RNN-2010-0542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Shea, H., and Moran, A. (2017). Does motor simulation theory explain the cognitive mechanisms underlying motor imagery? A critical review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:7. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paravlic, A. H. (2022). Motor imagery and action observation as appropriate strategies for home-based rehabilitation: a mini-review focusing on improving physical function in orthopedic patients. Front. Psychol. 13:826476. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.826476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pascual-Leone, A., Nguyet, D., Cohen, L. G., Brasil-Neto, J. P., Cammarota, A., Hallett, M., et al. (1995). Modulation of muscle responses evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation during the acquisition of new fine motor skills. J. Neurophysiol. 74, 1037–1045. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.74.3.1037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Péran, P., Démonet, J. F., Cherubini, A., Carbebat, D., Caltagirone, C., Sabatini, U., et al. (2010). Mental representations of action: the neural correlates of the verbal and motor components. Brain Res. 1328, 89–103. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.02.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pezzulo, G., Candidi, M., Dindo, H., and Barca, L. (2013). Action simulation in the human brain: twelve questions. New Ideas Psychol. 31, 270–290. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.01.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pielou, E. C. (1984). The Interpretation of Ecological Data a Primer on Classification and Ordination. New York, NY, USA: Wiley.

Google Scholar

Plata Bello, J., Modroño, C., Marcano, F., and González-Mora, J. L. (2014). The mirror neuron system and motor dexterity: what happens? Neuroscience 275, 285–295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Popp, M., Trumpp, N. M., Sim, E. J., and Kiefer, M. (2019). brain activation during conceptual processing of action and sound verbs. Adv. Cogn. Psychol. 15, 236–255. doi: 10.5709/acp-0272-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prinz, W. (2014). Action representation: crosstalk between semantics and pragmatics. Neuropsychologia 55, 51–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2005). Brain mechanisms linking language and action. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 576–582. doi: 10.1038/nrn1706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F., Hauk, O., Nikulin, V. V., and Ilmoniemi, R. J. (2005). Functional links between motor and language systems. Eur. J. Neurosci. 21, 793–797. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2005.03900.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Meaning and the brain: The neurosemantics of referential, interactive and combinatorial knowledge. J. Neurol. 25, 423–459. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pulvermüller, F. (2018). Neural reuse of action perception circuits for language, concepts and communication. Prog. Neurobiol. 160, 1–44. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2017.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ramsey, R., Kaplan, D. M., and Cross, E. S. (2021). Watch and learn: the cognitive neuroscience of learning from others’ actions. Trends Neurosci. 44, 478–491. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2021.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Matelli, M., Bettinardi, V., Paulesu, E., Perani, D., et al. (1996). Localization of grasp representations in humans by PET: 1. Observation versus execution. Exp. Brain Res. 111, 246–252. doi: 10.1007/BF00227301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rizzolatti, G., and Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 11, 264–274. doi: 10.1038/nrn2805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Romano-Smith, W. G., Wright, D., and Wakefield, C. (2018). Simultaneous and alternate action observation and motor imagery combinations improve aiming performance. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 38, 100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosenbaum, D. A. (2021). The ultimate tool: the body, planning of physical actions and the role of mental imagery in choosing motor acts. Top. Cogn. Sci. 13, 777–799. doi: 10.1111/tops.12561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rousseau, C., Barbiero, M., Pozzo, T., Papaxanthis, C., and White, O. (2021). Actual and imagined movements reveal a dual role of the insular cortex for motor control. Cereb. Cortex 31, 2586–2594. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhaa376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rozand, V., Lebon, F., Stapley, P. J., Papaxanthis, C., and Lepers, R. (2016). A prolonged motor imagery session alter imagined and actual movement durations: potential implications for neurorehabilitation. Behav. Brain Res. 297, 67–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.09.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saiote, C., Tacchino, A., Brichetto, G., Roccatagliata, L., Bommarito, G., Cordano, C., et al. (2016). Resting-state functional connectivity and motor imagery brain activation. Hum. Brain Mapp. 37, 3847–3857. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sakamoto, M., Muraoka, T., Mizuguchi, N., and Kanosue, K. (2009). Combining observation and imagery of an action enhances human corticospinal excitability. Neurosci. Res. 65, 23–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2009.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sauvage, C., Jissendi, P., Seignan, S., Manto, M., and Habas, C. (2013). Brain areas involved in the control of speed during a motor sequence of the foot: real movement versus mental imagery. J. Neuroradiol. 40, 267–280. doi: 10.1016/j.neurad.2012.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Savaki, H. E., Kavroulakis, E., Papadaki, E., Maris, T. G., and Simos, P. G. (2022). Action observation responses are influenced by movement kinematics and target identity. Cereb. Cortex 32, 490–503. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhab225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schack, T., and Ritter, H. (2009). The cognitive nature of action - functional links between cognitive psychology, movement science and robotics. Prog. Brain Res. 174, 231–250. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(09)01319-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuil, K. D., Smits, M., and Zwaan, R. A. (2013). Sentential context modulates the involvement of the motor cortex in action language processing: an fMRI study. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:100. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scott, M. W., Wood, G., Holmes, P. S., Williams, J., Marshall, B., Wright, D. J., et al. (2021). Combined action observation and motor imagery: an intervention to combat the neural and behavioural deficits associated with developmental coordination disorder. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 127, 638–646. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuster, C., Hilfiker, R., Amft, O., Scheidhauer, A., Andrews, B., Butler, J., et al. (2011). Best practice for motor imagery: a systematic literature review on motor imagery training elements in five different disciplines. BMC Med. 9:75. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-9-75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sharma, N., and Baron, J. C. (2013). Does motor imagery share neural networks with executed movement: a multivariate fMRI analysis. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:564. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shepard, R. N. (1962). The analysis of proximities: multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. Psychometrika 27, 125–140. doi: 10.1007/BF02289630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simos, P. G., Kavroulakis, E., Maris, T., Papadaki, E., Boursianis, T., Kalaitzakis, G., et al. (2017). Neural foundations of overt and covert actions. Neuroimage 152, 482–496. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiridonov, A., Balakauskas, L., Stankevič, R., Kluczynska, G., Gedminienė, L., and Stančikaitė, M. (2019). Holocene vegetation patterns in southern Lithuania indicate astronomical forcing on the millennial and centennial time scales. Sci. Rep. 9, 14711–14715. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-51321-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taube, W., Mouthon, M., Leukel, C., Hoogewoud, H. M., Annoni, J. M., Keller, M., et al. (2015). Brain activity during observation and motor imagery of different balance tasks: an fMRI study. Cortex 64, 102–114. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, L., Chen, H., Zhao, W., Wu, J., Zhang, Q., De, A., et al. (2020). The role of motor system in action-related language comprehension in L1 and L2: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 201:104714. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.104714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tremblay, P., and Small, S. L. (2011). From language comprehension to action understanding and back again. Cereb. Cortex 21, 1166–1177. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhQ199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Villiger, M., Estévez, N., Hepp-Reymond, M. C., Kiper, D., Kollias, S. S., Eng, K., et al. (2013). Enhanced activation of motor execution networks using action observation combined with imagination of lower limb movements. PloS One 8:e72403. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0072403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vogt, S., Di Rienzo, F., Collet, C., Collins, A., and Guillot, A. (2013). Multiple roles of motor imagery during action observation. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:807. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00807

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vry, M., Saur, D., Rijntjes, M., Umarova, R., Kellmeyer, P., Schnell, S., et al. (2012). Ventral and dorsal fiber systems for imagined and executed movement. Exp. Brain Res. 219, 203–216. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3079-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Z., Zhang, Z., and Sun, Y. (2022). Different neural information flows affected by activity patterns for action and verb generation. Front. Psychol. 13:802756. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.802756

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willems, R. M., Toni, I., Hagoort, P., and Casasanto, D. (2010). Neural dissociations between action verb understanding and motor imagery. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 2387–2400. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wolpert, D. M., and Ghahramani, Z. (2000). Computational principles of movement neuroscience. Nat. Neurosci. 3, 1212–1217. doi: 10.1038/81497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, D. J., Frank, C., and Bruton, A. (2021). Recommendations for combining action observation and motor imagery interventions in sport. J. Sport Psychol. Action 13, 155–167. doi: 10.1080/21520704.2021.1971810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, D. J., Williams, J., and Holmes, P. S. (2014). Combined action observation and imagery facilitates corticospinal excitability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:951. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00951

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., Shu, H., Bi, Y., Liu, Y., and Wang, X. (2011). Dissociation and association of the embodied representation of tool-use verbs and hand verbs: an fMRI study. Brain Lang. 119, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, J., and Shu, H. (2014). Passive reading and motor imagery about hand actions and tool-use actions: an fMRI study. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 453–467. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3753-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Young, M. P., Scannell, J. W., O’Neill, M. A., Hilgetag, C. C., Burns, G., Blakemore, C., et al. (1995). Non-metric multidimensional scaling in the analysis of neuroanatomical connection data and the organization of the primate cortical visual system. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 348, 281–308. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1995.0069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zappa, A., Bolger, D., Pergandi, J. M., Mallet, P., Dubarry, A. S., Mestre, D., et al. (2019). Motor resonance during linguistic processing as shown by EEG in a naturalistic VR environment. Brain Cogn. 134, 44–57. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zapparoli, L., Invernizzi, P., Gandola, M., Verardi, M., Berlingeri, M., Sberna, M., et al. (2013). Mental images across the adult lifespan: a behavioural and fMRI investigation of motor execution and motor imagery. Exp. Brain Res. 224, 519–540. doi: 10.1007/s00221-012-3331-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zentgraf, K., Munzert, J., Bischoff, M., and Newman-Norlund, R. D. (2011). Simulation during observation of human actions–theories, empirical studies, applications. Vis. Res. 51, 827–835. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Z., Sun, Y., Humphreys, G. W., and Song, Y. (2017). Different activity patterns for action and language within their shared neural areas: an fMRI study on action observation and language phonology. Neuropsychologia 99, 112–120. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: motor cognition, motor imagery, action observation, covert action, simulation, embodied, multidimensional model, action-related langauge

Citation: O’Shea H (2022) Mapping relational links between motor imagery, action observation, action-related language, and action execution. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 16:984053. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2022.984053

Received: 01 July 2022; Accepted: 07 October 2022;
Published: 04 November 2022

Edited by:

Florent Lebon, Action et Plasticité Sensomotrice, France

Reviewed by:

Ambra Bisio, University of Genoa, Italy
Marcela Perrone-Bertolotti, UMR5105 Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition (LPNC), France

Copyright © 2022 O’Shea. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Helen O’Shea, helen.oshea@ul.ie

Download