Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-wzw2p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-07T07:12:38.548Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Switching: Cultural fluency sustains and cultural disfluency disrupts thinking fast

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2023

Daphna Oyserman*
Affiliation:
Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA oyserman@usc.edu https://dornsife.usc.edu/daphna-oyserman/

Abstract

Culture-as-situated cognition theory provides insight into the system 1 monitoring algorithm. Culture provides people with an organizing framework, facilitating predictions, focusing attention, and providing experiential signals of certainty and uncertainty as system 1 inputs. When culture-based signals convey that something is amiss, system 2 reasoning is triggered and engaged when resources allow; otherwise, system 1 reasoning dominates.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Lin, Y., Arieli, S., & Oyserman, D. (2019). Cultural fluency means all is okay, cultural disfluency implies otherwise. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 84, 103822.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lu, J. G. (2023). Two large-scale global studies on COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy over time: Culture, uncertainty avoidance, and vaccine side-effect concerns. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 124(4), 683706. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000320CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mourey, J. A., Lam, B. C., & Oyserman, D. (2015). Consequences of cultural fluency. Social Cognition, 33(4), 308344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D. (2011). Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making. European Review of Social Psychology, 22(1), 164214. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2011.627187CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D. (2017). Culture three ways: Culture and subcultures within countries. Annual Review of Psychology, 68, 435463.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D., Novin, S., Flinkenflögel, N., & Krabbendam, L. (2014). Integrating culture-as-situated-cognition and neuroscience prediction models. Culture and Brain, 2(1), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., Sorensen, N., Reber, R., & Chen, S. X. (2009). Connecting and separating mind-sets: Culture as situated cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 217235. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oyserman, D., & Uskul, A. K. (2008/2015). Individualism and collectivism: Societal-level processes with implications for individual-level and society-level outcomes. In van de Vijver, F., van Hemert, D., & Poortinga, Y. (Eds.), Multilevel analysis of individuals and cultures (pp. 145173). Ebook: 9780203888032. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Yan, V. X. (2019). Making meaning: A culture-as-situated-cognition approach to the consequences of cultural fluency and disfluency. In Cohen, D. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 536565). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 527561). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (2001). Feelings as information: Implications for affective influences on information processing. In Martin, L. L. & Clore, G. L. (Eds.), Theories of mood and cognition: A user's guidebook (pp. 159176). Erlbaum.Google Scholar