Skip to main content

Social Animals and the Potential for Morality: On the Cultural Exaptation of Behavioral Capacities Required for Normativity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Synthese Library ((SYLI,volume 437))

Abstract

To help bridge the explanatory gap of how normativity branched off into morality in the course of evolutionary history, I claim that morality is a form of social normativity, specifically a form of cultural normativity. Furthermore, with the origins of its behavioral capacities rooted in normative practice, morality should be considered as an exaptation, a secondary adaptation shaped through cultural selection and evolution. Cultural selection pressures differ across social groups, as well as various species. Empirical evidence has shown that animals other than humans are capable of normative behavior, and that they can also be subject to processes of cultural transmission. With an inclusive approach to defining social behaviors, I argue that non-human, socio-cultural animals can engage in moral behavior.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   89.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The application of epigenetics to morality has been made before (for example, Wilson, 1998), although future studies should also expand on the potential for a change in moral behavior (gene expression/phenotype) without affecting genotype, within non-human animal populations that engage in cultural practices.

References

  • Andrews, K. (2019). Naïve normativity: The social foundation of moral cognition. Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 6(1), 36–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, P. W., Gangestad, S. W., & Matthews, D. (2002). Adaptationism – How to carry out an exaptationist program. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 489–553.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aplin, L. M. (2019). Culture and cultural evolution in birds: A review of the evidence. Animal Behavior, 147, 179–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. (2015). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature, 518(7540), 538–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aplin, L. M., Sheldon, B. C., & McElreath, R. (2017). Conformity does not perpetuate suboptimal traditions in a wild population of songbirds. PNAS, 114(30), 7830–7837. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ecog.02481

  • Ayala, F. J. (2010). The difference of being human: Morality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science in the USA 107/2 [In the Light of Evolution IV: The Human Condition], 9015–9022.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beja-Pereira, A., Luikart, G., England, P. R., Bradley, D. G., Jann, O. C., Bertorelle, G., et al. (2003). Gene-culture coevolution between cattle milk protein genes and human lactase genes. Nature Genetics, 35(4), 311–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bekoff, M. (2007). The emotional lives of animals: A leading scientist explores animal joy, sorrow, and empathy—And why they matter. Novato, CA: New World Library.

    Google Scholar 

  • Box, H. O. (1984). Primate behavior and social ecology. London: Chapman and Hall.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brennan, G., Eriksson, L., Goodin, R. E., & Southwood, N. (2013). Explaining norms. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, S. F., Talbot, C., Ahlgren, M., Lambeth, S. P., & Schapiro, S. J. (2010). Mechanisms underlying responses to inequitable outcomes in chimpanzees, Pantroglodytes. Animal Behavior, 79(6), 1229–1237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, J. M., Brügger, R. K., and van Schaik, C. P. (2018). Evolutionary origins of morality: Insights from non-human primates. Frontiers in Sociology 3/17, 1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, C. A., & Millen, A. E. (2009). Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychological Science, 20(12), 1478–1483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cantor, M., & Whitehead, H. (2013). The interplay between social networks and culture: Theoretically and among whales and dolphins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 368(1618), 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claidière, N., & Whiten, A. (2012). Integrating the study of conformity and culture in humans and nonhuman animals. Psychological Bulletin, 138(1), 126–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, S. J., Vale, G. L., Schapiro, S. J., Lambeth, S. P., & Whiten, A. (2016). Foundations of cumulative culture in apes: Improved foraging efficiency through relinquishing and combining witnessed behaviors in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Scientific Reports, 6, 112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal, F. B. M. (2014). Natural normativity: The ‘is’ and ‘ought’ of animal behavior. In F. B. M. de Waal, P. Churchland, T. Pievani, & S. Parmigiani (Eds.), Evolved morality: The biology and philosophy of human conscience (pp. 185–204). Leiden, the Netherlands: Brill Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. (1995). Darwin’s dangerous idea. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L. (2017). Ought Ought Not Imply Can. GS/PHIL6420 Moral Psychology – York University Fall Semester 2017, graduate student paper (draft provided by L. Edwards).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fraser, B. J. (2010). Adaptation, exaptation, by-products, and spandrels in evolutionary explanations of morality. Biological Theory, 5(3), 223–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galef, B. G. (1988). Imitation in animals: History, definition and interpretation of data from the psychological laboratory. In T. R. Zentall & B. G. Galef (Eds.), Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives (pp. 3–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gefland, M. J., & Jackson, J. C. (2016). From one mind to many: The emerging science of cultural norms. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 175–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gert, B., & Gert, J. (2017). The Definition of Morality. In Zalta, E. N. (ed.) Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, online: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2017/entries/morality-definition/

  • Gould, S. J. (1997). The exaptive excellence of spandrels as a term and prototype. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science in the USA, 94, 10750–10755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of san Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptationist programme. Proceedings of the Royal Society B of London, 205, 581–598.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J., & Vbra, E. S. (1982). Exaptation – A missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology, 8(1), 4–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., Muller, M., Strimling, P., Wrangham, R., & Zuberbühler, K. (2009). Wild chimpanzees rely on cultural knowledge to solve an experimental honey acquisition task. Current Biology, 19, 1806–1810.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, T., Zuberbüher, K., Clément, F., & van Schaik, C. P. (2015). Apes have culture but may not know that they do. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heath, J. (2017). Morality, convention and conventional morality. Philosophical Explorations, 20(3), 276–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J. (2011). A cultural species: How culture drove human evolution – A multi-disciplinary framework for understanding culture, cognition and behavior. Psychological Science Agenda – Science Brief, online: https://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2011/11/human-evolution

  • Henrich, J. (2016). The secret of our success. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 69(2), 207–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, V., Proctor, D., Bonnie, K. E., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLoS One, 5(5), 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2003). Diversification and cumulative evolution in new Caledonian crow tool manufacture. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B, 270, 867–874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jaeggi, A. V., Dunkel, L. P., Van Noordwijk, M. A., Wich, S. A., Sura, A. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: Implications for culture. American Journal of Primatology, 72(1), 62–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Josephs, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Young children think you can opt out of social- conventional but not moral practices. Cognitive Development, 39, 197–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, R. (2014). The origins of moral judgement. Behavior, 151, 261–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keith, S. A. & Bull, J. W. (2017). Animal culture impacts species’ capacity to realise climate-driven range shifts. Ecography, 40, 296–304. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/ecog.02481

  • Kinnier, R., Kernes, J., & Dautheribes, T. (2000). A short list of universal moral values. Counselling and Values – Issues and Insights, 45, 4–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laiolo, P., & Tella, J. L. (2007). Erosion of animal cultures in fragmented landscapes. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment, 5(2), 68–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N. (2008). Animal Cultures. Current Biology, 18(9), R366–R370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Brown, G. R. (2002). Sense and nonsense: Evolutionary perspectives on human behaviour. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Hoppitt, W. (2003). Do animals have culture? Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(/3), 150–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laland, K. N., & Janik, V. M. (2006). The animal cultures debate. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 21(10), 542–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lorini, G. (2018). Animal norms: An investigation of normativity in the non-human social world. Law, Culture and the Humanities, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Machery, E., and Mallon, R. (2012). Evolution of Morality. In Doris, J. M. (ed.) & Moral Psychology Research Group, The moral psychology handbook (Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK), 3–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 329–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, M., & Nishida, T. (2013). Ontogeny of a social custom in wild chimpanzees: Age changes in grooming hand-clasp at Mahale. American Journal of Primatology, 75, 186–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, S. (2005). Innateness and moral psychology. In P. Carruthers, S. Laurence, & S. Stich (Eds.), The innate mind: Structure and contents (pp. 353–430). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Niu, W. (2015). Commentary on chapter 14: Conservatism versus innovation: The great ape story by Josep Call. In A. B. Kaufman & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Animal creativity and innovation: Explorations in creativity research (pp. 397–418). Cambridge, UK: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohtsuki, H., & Iwasa, Y. (2004). How should we define goodness?—Reputation dynamics in indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 231(1), 107–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, J., & Bekoff, M. (2009). Wild justice: The moral lives of animals. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, E. E., Caldwell, C. A., & Whiten, A. (2010). Comparative cultural cognition. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 23–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2008). Is morality innate? In W. Sinnott-Armstrong (ed.) Moral Psychology 1: The evolution of morality – Adaptations and innateness (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA) 367–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramsey, G. (2017). What is animal culture? In K. Andrews & J. Beck (Eds.), Routledge companion to the philosophy of animal minds (pp. 345–353). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raz, J. (1998). Multiculturalism. Ratio Juris, 11(3), 193–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rendell, L., & Whitehead, H. (2001). Culture in whales and dolphins. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 309–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapolsky, R. M. (2006). Culture in animals: The case of a non-human primate culture of low aggression and high affiliation. Social Forces, 85(1), 217–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sasaki, T., & Biro, D. (2017). Cumulative culture can emerge from collective intelligence in animal groups. Nature Communications, 8, 1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidt, M. F. H., Butler, L. P., Heinz, J., & Tomasello, M. (2016). Young children see a single action and infer a social norm: Promiscuous normativity in 3-year-olds. Psychological Science, 27(10), 1360–1370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sosis, R. (2009). The Adaptationist-Byproduct debate on the evolution of religion: Five misunderstandings of the Adaptationist program. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9, 315–332. https://richard-sosis.uconn.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/2243/2018/06/2009-Sosis-Adaptionist-Byproduct-Debate.pdf

  • Tennie, C., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Ratcheting up the ratchet: On the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2405–2415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tiberius, V. (2015). Moral psychology: A contemporary introduction. In P. K. Moser (Ed.), Routledge contemporary introductions to philosophy. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Leeuwen, E. J. C., Acerbi, A., Kendal, R. L., Tennie, C., & Haun, D. B. M. (2016). A Reappreciation of ‘Conformity’. Animal Behavior, 122, e5–e10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Schaik, C. P., Ancrenaz, M., Borgen, G., Galdikas, B., Knott, C. D., Singleton, I., et al. (2003). Orangutan cultures and the evolution of material culture. Science, 299, 102–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vincent, S., Ring, R., & Andrews, K. (2018). Normative practices of other animals. In A. Zimmerman, K. Jones, & M. Timmons (Eds.), The routledge handbook of moral epistemology (pp. 57–83). London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver, I. C. G., Cervoni, N., Champagne, F. A., D’Alessio, A. C., Sharma, S., Seckl, J. R., et al. (2004). Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 847–854.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, M. M., & Laland, K. N. (2012). Social information, conformity and the opportunity costs paid by foraging fish. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 66, 797–809.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, H. (2017). Gene-culture coevolution in whales and dolphin. PNAS, 114(30), 7814–7821. https://www.pnas.org/content/pnas/114/30/7814.full.pdf

  • Whitehead, H., Rendell, L., Osborne, R. W., & Würsig, B. (2004). Culture and conservation of nonhumans with reference to whales and dolphins: Review and new directions. Biological Conservation, 120, 427–437.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehead, H., Laland, K. N., Rendell, L., Thorogood, R., & Whiten, A. (2019). The reach of gene–culture coevolution in animals. Nature Communications, 10(2405), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. (2012). Social learning, traditions and culture. In J. Mitani, J. Call, P. Kappeler, P. Palombit, & J. Silk (Eds.), The evolution of primate societies (pp. 681–699). Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. (2019). Cultural evolution in animals. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 50(1), 27–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, E. (1998). The biological basis of morality. The Atlantic 4, online https://www.theatlantic.com/magazine/archive/1998/04/the-biological-basis-of-morality/377087/

  • Wrangham, R. W. (2009). Catching fire: How cooking made us human. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamamoto, S., Humle, T., & Tanaka, M. (2013). Basis for cumulative cultural evolution in chimpanzees: Social learning of a more efficient tool-use technique. PLoS One, 8(1), 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yasui, S., & Idani, G. (2017). Social significance of trunk use in captive Asian elephants. Ethology Ecology and Evolution, 29(4), 330–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I wish to express my sincere gratitude to those who have helped me make this paper possible. I am extremely grateful for the mentorship and guidance I received from Kristin Andrews, without whom I would not have found my passion for such research, nor the knowledge of where to start. I am also deeply appreciative of the encouragement from the editors, Helen De Cruz and Johan De Smedt, who provided me with direct support, especially during the final stages of this work. Finally, I am thankful for all those I was able to speak to about these ideas (in classes, at conference presentations, or otherwise), as well as for those who read previous drafts and were patient with my thinking process/writing progress.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Estelle Palao .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2021 Springer Nature Switzerland AG

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Palao, E. (2021). Social Animals and the Potential for Morality: On the Cultural Exaptation of Behavioral Capacities Required for Normativity. In: De Smedt, J., De Cruz, H. (eds) Empirically Engaged Evolutionary Ethics. Synthese Library, vol 437. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-68802-8_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics