Skip to main content
Log in

Group Emotions in Collective Reasoning: A Model

  • Published:
Argumentation Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Education and cognition research today generally recognize the tri-dimensional nature of reasoning processes as involving cognitive, social and emotional phenomena. However, there is so far no theoretical framework articulating these three dimensions from a descriptive perspective. This paper aims at presenting a first model of how group emotions work in collective reasoning, and specifies their social and cognitive functions. This model is inspired both from a multidisciplinary literature review and our extensive previous empirical work on an international corpus of videotaped student debates. The cognitive function of emotions is defined in reference to the process of schematization (Grize 1996, 1997) and associated emotional framing (Polo et al. 2013). On the other hand, the social function of emotions refers to recognition-oriented behaviors that correspond to engagement into specific types of group talk (e.g. Mercer 1996), implying specific politeness rules or face-preservation systems (Brown and Levinson 1988). We believe that our multi-dimensional and multi-level approach to group reasoning, which mostly employs a linguistic perspective, can be applied to a diversity of contexts. We hope it will serve as a basis for further discussion on the role of emotions in reasoning among the interdisciplinary community of argumentation studies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. They were especially trained during 1 day (6–8 h) in order to moderate the café.

  2. The Alpine Rendez-Vous Workshop on “Tools and Technologies for Emotion Awareness in Computer-Mediated Collaboration and Learning”, held in Villard-de-Lans, in Juanuary 2013, is emblematic of this line of research.

  3. This typology was developed by Mercer and his colleagues, see the following references: Fernández et al. (2002), Mercer (1996), Mercer and Littleton (2007), Mercer et al. (1999), Mercer and Sams (2006), Wegerif et al. (2004), Wegerif and Mercer (1997).

  4. Opinion question 3 is as follows: “How should the price of water be determined? (a) drinking water should be free; (b) drinking water should be sold at a price that covers the costs of its production; (c) drinking water should be sold at a price that depends on its quality; (d) drinking water should be sold at a price that depends on family income; (e) drinking water should be sold at a price that depends on how it is used; (f) water should be free up to a reasonable amount, beyond which it should be sold at a high price”.

  5. The conventions used for all the transcripts are detailed in Appendix 1.

  6. ‘Phase’ here corresponds to ‘planned tasks sequentially organized that take part to the pedagogical activity’ (Bouchard and Rollet 2003). In this context, a phasis consists of a group discussion about a given opinion question (lasting from 3 to 5 min).

  7. Opinion question 1 is the following: “In your opinion, which potential source of drinking water is the most promising for the future? (a) the discovery of new fresh water deposits; (b) water that we don’t use today (that which, is economized); (c) desalination of seawater; (d) climate change leading to more rain; (e) new techniques for the depollution of water; (f) none of these are promising: water is going to be in short supply and will become THE conflict of the twenty-first century

  8. The English version of opinion question 2 is “Which of these things would you be the most willing to do? a) take fewer baths and showers; b) use my phone and computer for a longer time before getting a new one; c) eat less meat; d) use dry toilets; e) all of that, and even more; f) I’m not ready to make any of those efforts”.

  9. This approach is similar to the Anglo-Saxon frame analysis. As a discursive construct, ‘frames’, as schematizations, ‘induce us to filter our perceptions of the world in particular ways, essentially making some aspects of our multidimensional reality more noticeable than other aspects’ (Kuypers 2009, p. 181). As a process, ‘framing’, as well as ‘schematization’, ‘encourages the facts of a given situation to be interpreted by others in a particular manner’ (Kuypers 2006, p. 8). Nevertheless, frame analysis is related to the critical tradition in discourse analysis, which does not fit with our radically descriptive epistemological approach. Moreover, the schematization process applies at a smaller scale, the one of discourse objects. We sometimes use the term ‘emotional framing’ to refer to the global emotional position resulting from the sum of the emotional tonalities conferred to each considered discourse object, for a specific ‘team’ defending the consistent argumentative claim.

  10. The following excerpts are taken from a debate among American students about the final, main question of the café: “In your opinion, in the future, whether a person has access to drinking water will depend on…? (a) on how rich the person is; (b) on how physically able the person is to live with lower water quality; (c) on efforts made, starting now, to save water by using less and to protect water resources; (d) on where on the globe the person is born; (e) on nature’s capacity to adapt to our needs for water; (f) on scientific advances”.

  11. The concept of ‘interdiscourse’ has been used and defined in different ways and is characterized by a complex historiography (Paveau 2010), that we unfortunately have no space to detail here. This classical notion of the French discourse analysis finds its roots in from Pêcheux (1969). Amossy (2006, pp. 94–99) briefly introduces this concept as a part of the basic toolbox for the analysis of argumentation. Following her approach, we here only mention ‘interdiscourse’ as far as it corresponds to ‘a necessity, to identify and assess the function of doxic elements, to perceive the utterance in relation to the other discourses from which it is elaborated’ (Idem: 98).

References

  • Albe, Virginie. 2006. Procédés discursifs et rôles sociaux d’élèves en groupes de discussion sur une controverse socio-scientifique. Revue française de pédagogie 157: 103–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, Robin. 2010. Speaking but not listening? Accountable talk in an unaccountable context. Literacy 44(3): 103–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Allwood, Jens, Davis Traum, and Kristina Jokinen. 2000. Cooperation, Dialogue and Ethics. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies 53: 871–914.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amossy, Ruth. 2006. L’argumentation dans le discours. Paris: Armand Colin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ancombre, Jean-Claude, Oswald Ducrot. 1997. L’argumentation dans la langue. Mardaga.

  • Andriessen, Jerry, Mirjam Pardijs, Michael Baker. 2013. Getting on and getting along: Tension in the developement of collaborations. In Affective learning together, ed. Michael Baker, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen, 205–228. Routledge.

  • Asterhan, Christa. 2013. Epistemic and interpersonal dimensions of peer argumentation. In Affective learning together, ed. Michael Baker, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen, 251–271. Routledge.

  • Baker, Michael, Matthieu Quignard, Kristine Lund, Marije van Amelsvoort. 2002. Designing a computer-supported collaborative learning situation for broadening and deepening understanding of the space of debate. Proceedings of the 5th International Conference of the International Society for the Study of Argumentation: 55–61. Amsterdam.

  • Baker, Michael, Jerry Andriessen. 2009. Socio-relational, affective and cognitive dimensions of CSCL interactions: integrating theoretical-methodological perspectives. Proceedings of the 9th International Conference on Computer Supported Collaborative Learning 2: 31–33.

  • Baker, Michael, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen (Eds). 2013. Affective learning together: Social and emotional dimensions of collaborative learning. Routledge.

  • Bouchard, Robert, and Rolet, Christiane. 2003. Pour une méthodologie d’analyse didactico-interactionnelle des pratiques d’enseignement-apprentissage: à propos d’une séance de mathématiques à l’école primaire. In Construction des connaissances et langage dans les disciplines d’enseignement, ed. Martine Jaubert, Maryse Rebière, Jean-Paul Bernié. IUFM Aquitaine et Université Bordeaux 2.

  • Brown, Penelope, and Stephen C. Levinson. 1988. Politeness. Cambridge: Cambridge Univiversity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caffi, Claudia, and Richard W. Janney. 1994. Toward a pragmatics of emotive communication. Journal of Pragmatics 22(3–4): 325–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cahour, Béatrice. 2013. Characteristics, emergence and circulation in interactional learning. In Affective learning together, ed. Michael Baker, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen, 52–69. Routledge.

  • Dillenbourg, Pierre, and Patrick Jermann. 2007. Designing integrative scripts. In Scripting computer-supported collaborative learning, ed. Frank Fischer, Ingo Kollar, Heinz Mandl, Jörg M. Haake, 275–301. Springer.

  • Fernández, Manuel, Rupert Wegerif, Neil Mercer, and Sylvia Rojas-Drummond. 2002. Re-conceptualizing ‘scaffolding’ and the zone of proximal development in the context of symmetrical collaborative learning. Journal of Classroom Interaction 36(2/1): 40–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, Michael A. 1995. Coalescent Argumentation. Argumentation 9(5): 837–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, Michael A. 2001. Emotional messages. Argumentation 15(3): 239–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, Michael A. 2004. Emotion argumentation and informal logic. Informal Logic 24(3): 245–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, Erwin. 1974. Les rites d’interaction. Paris: Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gouran, Dennis S. 2004. Moving forward/looking back: The functions of argument in inducing and managing conflict in decision-making and problem-solving groups. Chicago, November: Annual Meeting of the National Communication Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grize, Jean-Blaise. 1996. Logique naturelle et communication. Paris: Presses universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grize, Jean-Blaise. (1997 [1990]). Logique et langage. Ophrys.

  • Hamblin, Charles L. 1970. Fallacies. Methuen.

  • Hekmat, Ida Raphaël Micheli, Alain Rabatel (Eds). 2013. Modes de sémiotisation et fonctions argumentatives des émotions, Semen, 35.

  • Jacques, Francis. 1988. Trois stratégies interactionnelles: conversation, négociation, dialogue. In Échanges sur la conversation, ed. Jacques Cosnier, Nadine Gelas, Catherine Kerbrat-Orecchioni, 45–68. CNRS Editions.

  • Järvenoja, Hanna, and Sanna Järvelä. 2013. Regulating Emotions Together for Motivated Collaboration. In Affective learning together, ed. Michael Baker, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen, 162–181. Routledge.

  • Keefer, Matthew W., Colleen M. Zeitz, and Lauren B. Resnick. 2000. Judging the quality of peer-led student dialogues. Cognition and Instruction 18(1): 53–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotthoff, Helga. 1993. Disagreement and concession in disputes: On the context sensitivity of preference structures. Language in Society 22(2): 193–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuypers, Jim A. 2006. Bush’s war: Media bias and justifications for war in a terrorist age. Rowman & Littlefield Publishers, Inc.

  • Kuypers, Jim A. 2009. Rhetorical criticism: Perspectives in action. Lexington Press.

  • Mercer, Neil. 1996. The quality of talk in children’s collaborative activity in the classroom. Learning and instruction 6(4): 359–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, Neil, and Claire Sams. 2006. Teaching children how to use language to solve maths problems. Language and Education 20(6): 507–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, Neil, Rupert Wegerif, and Lyn Dawes. 1999. Children’s talk and the development of reasoning in the classroom. British Educational Research Journal 25(1): 95–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer, Neil, and Karen Littleton. 2007. Dialogue and the development of children’s thinking: A sociocultural approach. Psychology Press.

  • Michaels, Sarah, Catherine O’Connor, Richard Sohmer, and Lauren B. Resnick. 1992. Guided construction of knowledge in the classroom: Teacher, talk, task, and tools. The Reading Teacher 46: 316–326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michaels, Sarah, Catherine O’Connor, and Lauren B. Resnick. 2008. Deliberative discourse idealized and realized: Accountable talk in the classroom and in civic life. Studies in Philosophy and Education 27(4): 283–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Micheli, Raphaël. 2013. Esquisse d’une typologie des différents modes de sémiotisation verbale de l’émotion. Semen 35: 17–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Micheli, Raphaël. 2010. L’émotion argumentée: l’abolition de la peine de mort dans le débat parlementaire français. Cerf.

  • Mullins, Dejana, Anne Deiglmayr, and Hans Spada. 2013. Motivation and emotion shaping knowledge. In Affective learning together, ed. Michael Baker, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen, 139–160. Routledge.

  • Muntigl, Peter, and William Turnbull. 1998. Conversational structure and facework in arguing. Journal of Pragmatics 29(3): 225–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paveau, Anne-Marie. 2010. Interdiscours et intertexte. Linguistique et littérature: Cluny, 40 ans après, Besançon, France (2008). Annales littéraires de l’université de Franche-Comté. Presses universitaires de Franche-Comté: 93–105.

  • Pêcheux, Michel. 1969. Analyse automatique du discours. Paris: Dunod.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perelman, Chaïm and Lucie Olbrechts-Tyteca. 2008 [1958]. Traité de l’argumentation: la nouvelle rhétorique. Broché.

  • Plantin, Christian. 2015. Emotion and Affect. In The international encyclopedia of language and social interaction, ed. Karen Tracy, Cornelia Ilie, and Todd Sandel, 514–523. Boston: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plantin, Christian. 2011. Les bonnes raisons des émotions: Principes et méthode pour l’étude du discours « émotionné » . Peter Lang.

  • Plantin, Christian. 2016a. Actants et acteurs, Dictionnaire de l’argumentation—Une introduction aux études d’argumentation 26–27. Lyon: ENS Editions.

  • Plantin, Christian. 2016b. Politesse argumentative, Dictionnaire de l’argumentation—Une introduction aux études d’argumentation 369. Lyon: ENS Editions.

  • Polo, Claire. 2014. L’eau à la bouche: ressources et travail argumentatifs des élèves lors de débats socio-scientifiques sur l’eau potable. Etude comparée de 10 cafés scientifiques menés au Mexique, aux USA et en France, en 2011–2012. Doctoral dissertation, Lyon 2 University, France.

  • Polo, Claire, Christian Plantin, Kristine Lund, and Gerald Niccolai. 2013. Quand construire une position émotionnelle, c’est choisir une conclusion argumentative. Semen 35: 41–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pomerantz, Anita. 1984. Agreeing and disagreeing with assessments: Some features of preferred/dispreferred turn shapes. In Structures of social action: Studies in conversation analysis, ed. J. Maxwell Atkinson, John Heritage, 57–101. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Roschelle, Jeremy, and Stephanie D. Teasley. 1995. The construction of shared knowledge in collaborative problem solving. Proceedings of the Conference on Computer Supported Collaborative Learning 69–97.

  • Ryan, Richard M., and Edward L. Deci. 2000. Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist 55(1): 68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seibold, David R., and Renee A. Meyers. 2007. Group argument a structuration perspective and research program. Small Group Research 38(3): 312–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sins, Patrick, Klas Karlgren. 2013. Identifying and overcoming tension in interdisciplinary teamwork in profesional development. In Affective learning together, ed. Michael Baker, Sanna Järvelä, Jerry Andriessen, 185–203. Routledge.

  • Stahl, Gerry. 2006. Group cognition. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toulmin, Stephen E. 1958. The uses of argument. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Traverso, Veronique. 1999. Negociación y argumentación en la conversación familiar. Escritos 17/18: 51–89. Journal of the Center of Language Sciences, UAP, Mexico.

  • van Eemeren, Frans H., and Rob Grootendorst. 2004. A systematic theory of argumentation: The pragma-dialectical approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, Douglas N. 1992. The place of emotion in argument. Pennsylvania State University Press.

  • Wegerif, Rupert, and Neil Mercer. 1997. A dialogical framework for researching peer talk. Language and Education Library 12: 49–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wegerif, Rupert, Karen Littleton, Lyn Dawes, Neil Mercer, and Denise Rowe. 2004. Widening access to educational opportunities through teaching children how to reason together. Westminster Studies in Education 27(2): 143–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, Armin. 2003. Scripts for computer-supported collaborative learning. Effects of social and epistemic cooperation scripts on collaborative knowledge construction. Doctoral Dissertation, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany.

Download references

Acknowledgments

LABEX ASLAN (ANR-10-LABX-0081) of Université de Lyon supported this research, within the program “Investissements d’Avenir” (ANR-11-IDEX-0007) of the French National Research Agency (ANR).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Claire Polo.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendices

Appendices

1.1 Appendix 1: Transcript Conventions

Here are detailed the main transcript conventions used in this article:

1 SPE blabla:

number of the turn, first three letters of speaker’s name, utterance

[:

beginning of speech overlap

::

elongated sound

<((laughing)) uterrance>:

commentaries on simultaneous coverbal behaviour

&:

continuation of a speech turn

=:

rapid succession of words/sounds

((turn)):

non-verbal turn (laugh, gesture, etc.)

xxx:

inaudible segment

/or \:

raising or falling intonation

(word):

uncertain transcription

°word°:

low voice or very low voice (°°word°°)

WORD:

augmented volume

‘:

non standard elision

(.) or (0.8):

pauses either not timed or timed in seconds (lasting 0.8 s)

(…):

discontinuity in turn taking

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Polo, C., Plantin, C., Lund, K. et al. Group Emotions in Collective Reasoning: A Model. Argumentation 31, 301–329 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-016-9407-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10503-016-9407-5

Keywords

Navigation