Skip to main content
Log in

From Pan to Homo sapiens: evolution from individual based to group based forms of social cognition

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Mind & Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The evolution from pre-human primates to modern Homo sapiens is a complex one involving many domains, ranging from the material to the social to the cognitive, both at the individual and the community levels. This article focuses on a critical qualitative transition that took place during this evolution involving both the social and the cognitive domains. For the social domain, the transition is from the face-to-face forms of social interaction and organization that characterize the non-human primates that reached, with Pan, a hiatus due to the centripetal effects that highly individualized behavior has on a social system. The transition is to the relation-based forms of social organization that evolved in the hominins ancestral to Homo sapiens and are universal in human societies today. For the cognitive domain, this transition involves going from behavior responding mainly to phenomenal level sensory inputs to behavior formed in accordance with the concept of a relation, initially abstracted from behavior patterns, then extending the concept of a relation beyond abstraction from behavior patterns to the concept of a relation generated recursively through constructing the relation of a relation. This extension made possible the construction of systems of relations; initially genealogical systems of relations constructed culturally using the logic of recursion, and subsequently, the symbolic, computational systems of kin term relations referred to by anthropologists as kinship terminologies. The latter are “constructed realities” in the sense this term is used by cultural anthropologists. It follows that the evolution of relation-based systems of social interaction is not adequately accounted for through population model evolutionary accounts such as the Dual Inheritance Theory of human evolution since “constructed realities” constitute collectively and publicly shared cultural knowledge rather than the individually and privately possessed knowledge that is assumed in the population model framework for human evolution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

adapted from the following: filled triangle– Epstein 2002; filled square—Rightmire 2004; filled diamond—Ruff et al. 1997. Phylogenetic groups for the encephalization data are identified except for the data for Homo post H. erectus. EQ = brain mass/(11.22 body mass0.76). The stages refer to qualitatively different tool forms (see Read and van der Leeuw 2008 for details)

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. I thank an anonymous reviewer for bringing the relevance of Charles S. Peirce's notion of hypostatic abstraction and the work of F. Stjernfelt on semiotics to my attention.

  2. “Hypostatic abstraction in mathematical logic … is a formal operation that transforms a predicate into a relation; for example ‘Honey is sweet’ is transformed into ‘Honey has sweetness.’ … The abstraction of hypostasis takes the concrete physical sense of ‘taste’ found in ‘honey is sweet’ and gives it formal metaphysical characteristics in ‘honey has sweetness’” (Wikipedia contributors: 2019).

  3. In predicate form, nurturant(female) → provides(female, nurturance).

  4. The fact that human language and systems of kinship relations are both syntactically organized linguistic systems raises the question of whether there is a connection between the origin of the one or the other. The simplest genealogical system of kinship relations incorporating both the generation of genealogical relations and the reduction of generated genealogical relations through structural equations would be the mother genealogical relation, its reciprocal child genealogical relation and the genealogical structure formed recursively from mother and/or child genealogical relations, using at most two genealogical relations at a time, and modified, for a female speaker, by the reciprocal genealogical structural equation, child’s mother = self. This system would consist of the genealogical relations mother, mother’s mother, child, child’s child, mother’s child and self = child’s mother. Implementation of the “grandmother” hypothesis system around 1.5 mya would provide a behavioral basis for the use of recursive logic to go from the mother genealogical relation to the mother of a mother genealogical relation. This suggests that a simple genealogical system of kinship relations could trace back to 1.5 mya, hence before the formation of syntactically structured human languages, and so could be one of the factors leading to embedding recursion as a fundamental feature of human languages.

References

  • Al-Amily HM (2003) The book of Arabic wisdom: proverbs and anecdotes. New Internationalist, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Alloway TP, Alloway RG (2010) Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. J Exp Child Psychol 106:20–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnard A, Good A (1984) Research practises in the study of kinship. Academic Press, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger PL, Luckmann T (1966) The social construction of reality: a treatise in the sociology of knowledge. Anchor Books, Garden City

    Google Scholar 

  • Bertolo S (ed) (2001) Language acquisition and learnability. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird-David N (2017) Us, relatives: scaling and plural life in a forager world. University of California Press, Berkeley

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson P (1985) Culture and the evolutionary process. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers P (2013) Evolution of working memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110(Suppl. 2):10371–10378

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapais B (2008) Primeval kinship: how pair-bonding gave birth to human society. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Cheney DL, Seyfarth RM (2007) Baboon metaphysics: the evolution of a social mind. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chit Hlaing FKL, Read DW (2016) Why marriage? Struct Dynamics: eJournal of Anthropological and Related Sciences 9(2). Retrieved from: https://escholarship.org/uc/item/56b9b0rb.

  • Cioffi-Revilla C (1994) Martial and political items in the Mesopotamian exhibit of the Département des Antiquités Orientales. Louvre Museum: VIth Millennium BC to VIIth Century BC. Working paper, Long-Range Analysis of War Project, University of Colorado

  • Cowan N, Elliott EM, Saults JS, Morey CC, Mattox SM, Hismjatullina A, Conway ARA (2005) On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognit Psychol 51(1):42–100

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan N, Fristoe NM, Elliott EM, Brunner RP, Saults JS (2006) Scope of attention, control of attention, and intelligence in children and adults. Memory Cognit 234(8):1754–1768

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dasser V (1988) A social concept in Java monkey. Anim Behav 36(1):225–230

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Fiori A, Rendall D (1994) Evolution of social organization: a reappraisal for primates by using phylogenetic methods. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 91:9941–9945

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dousset L (2008) The ‘Global’ versus the ‘Local’: cognitive processes of kin determination in Aboriginal Australia. Oceania 78(3):260–279

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar R (1992) A hidden constraint on the behavioural ecology of baboons. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 31:35–49

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunsworth H, Eccleston L (2015) The evolution of difficult childbirth and helpless infants. Annu Rev Anthropol 44:55–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dziebel GV (2007) The genius of kinship: The phenomenon of human kinship and the global diversity of kinship terminologies. Cambria Press, Youngstown

    Google Scholar 

  • Eisenbeiss S (2009) Generative approaches to language learning. Linguistics 47(2):273–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ember M, Ember CR (1979) Male-female bonding: a cross-species study of mammals and birds. Behavior Science Research 14:37–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle RW (2002) Working memory capacity as executive attention. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 11:19–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engle RW, Tuholski SW, Laughlin J, Conway ARA (1999) Working memory, short-term memory and general fluid intelligence: a latent variable model approach. J Exp Psychol Gen 128:309–331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ensor B (2013) The archaeology of kinship: advancing interpretation and contributions to theory. University of Arizona Press, Tucson

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein HT (2002) Evolution of the reasoning brain. Behav Brain Sci 25:408–409

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fairbanks LA (2000) Maternal investment throughout the life span in Old World monkeys. In: Whitehead PF, Jolly CJ (eds) Old world monkeys. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 341–367

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Fedurek P, Dunbar R (2009) What does mutual grooming tell us about why chimpanzees groom? Ethology 115(6):566–575

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque E, Valeggia CR, Mendoza SP (2009) The biology of paternal care in human and nonhuman primates. Annu Rev Anthropol 38:115–130

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferraro JV, Plummer TW, Pobiner BL, Oliver JS, Bishop LC, Braun DR, Ditchfield PW, Seaman JW III, Binetti KM, Seaman JW Jr, Hertel F, Potts R (2013) Earliest archaeological evidence of persistent hominin carnivory. PLoS ONE 8(4):e62174. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0062174

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitch WT (2011) The evolution of syntax: an exaptationist perspective. Front Evol Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2011.00009

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foley RA (2016) Mosaic evolution and the pattern of transitions in the hominin lineage. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biolog Sci 371(1698):20150244. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0244

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortes M (1969) Kinship and the social order: the legacy of Lewis Henry Morgan. Aldine, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Gavrilets S (2012) Human origins and the transition from promiscuity to pair-bonding. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:9923–9928

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geertz C (1973) The interpretation of cultures. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodenough W (ed) (1964) Explorations in cultural anthropology: essays in honour of George Peter Murdock. McGraw-Hill, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough KE (1959) The Nayars and the definition of marriage. J R Anthropol Inst GB Irel 89(1):23–34

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould SJ, Vrba ES (1982) Exaptation: a missing term in the science of form. Paleobiology 8:4–15

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gusinde M (1931) The Fireland Indians, vol. 1: The Selk’nam: on the life and thought of a hunting people of the Great Island of Tierra del Fuego.Verlag der Internationalen Zeitschrift, Vienna

  • Hauser MD, Chomsky N, Fitch WT (2002) The faculty of language: what is it, who has it and how did it evolve? Science 298:1569–1579

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes K, O’Connell JF, Blurton Jones NG (1997) Hadza women’s time allocation, offspring provisioning, and the evolution of post-menopausal lifespans. Curr Anthropol 38:551–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann E, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Call J, Hare B, Tomasello M (2010) The structure of individual differences in the cognitive abilities of children and chimpanzees. Psychol Sci 21(1):102–110

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffecker JE (2007) Representation and recursion in the archaeological record. J Archaeol Method Theory 14:359–387

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jolly A (1998) Lemur social structure. Folia Primatol 69(Suppl 1):1–13

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judd CB (1925–1926) The psychology of social institutions. J Abnormal Soc Psychol 20: 151–156

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lahdenperä M, Lummaa V, Helle S, Tremblay M, Russell AF (2004) Fitness benefits of prolonged post-reproductive lifespan in women. Nature 428(6979):178–181

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lai D (2001) Alignment, structural balance, and international conflict in the Middle East, 1948–1978. Confl Manag Peace Sci 18:211–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lane D, Maxfield RM, Read D, van der Leeuw S (2009) From population to organization thinking. In: Lane D, Pumain D, van der Leeuw SE, West G (eds) Complexity perspective in innovation and social change. Springer, Berlin, pp 11–42

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Leaf M, Read D (2012) Human thought and social organization: anthropology on a new plane. Lexington Press, Lanham

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann J, Korstjens AH, Dunbar RIM (2007) Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Anim Behav 74:1617–1629

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss C (1949) Les structures élémentaire de la parenté. Mouton, The Hague

    Google Scholar 

  • Lévi-Strauss C (1983) Le regard éloigné. Plon, Paris

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowe ED (2018) Kinship, funerals, and the durability of culture in Chuuk. In: Quinn N (ed) Culture, mind and society: advances in culture theory from psychological anthropology. Springer, Cham, pp 75–108

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • MacLarnon A (2012) The anatomical and physiological basis of human speech production: adaptations and exaptations. In: Gibson KR,Tallerman M (eds) The Oxford handbook of language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199541119.013.0022

    Google Scholar 

  • Maigon HL (2007) Social systems. Philos Psychol 20(5):557–578

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malinowski B (1913) The family among the Australian Aborigines: a sociological study. Hodder Stoughton, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Maryanski AR (1987) African ape social structure: Is there strength in weak ties? Soc Netw 9:191–215

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maryanski A, Turner JH (1992) The social cage: human nature and the evolution of society. Stanford University Press, Stanford

    Google Scholar 

  • Matsuzawa T (1994) Field experiments on use of stone tools by chimpanzees in the wild. In: Wrangham RW, McGrew WC, de Waal FBM, Heltne PG (eds) Chimpanzee cultures. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, pp 351–370

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthey de l’Etang A (2020) Chapter 8: Dwight Read: towards a new paradigm followed by a discussion between the author and Dwight read. Struct Dyn 11(1):135–190

  • Miller GA (1956) The magical number seven plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol Rev 63:81–97

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitani JC, Watts DP, Muller MN (2002) Recent developments in the study of wild chimpanzee behavior. Evol Anthropol 11:9–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan LH (1871) Systems of consanguinity and affinity of the human family. Smithsonian Institution, Washington

    Google Scholar 

  • Muller MN (2002) Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees. In: Boesch C, Hohmann G, Marchant LF (eds) Behavioural diversity in chimpanzees and bonobos. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 112–123

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Muller MN, Mitani JC (2005) Conflict and cooperation in wild chimpanzees. Adv Study Behav 35:275–331

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray EA, Wise SP, Graham KS (2017) The evolution of memory systems: ancestors, anatomy, and adaptations. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishida T (1968) The social group of wild chimpanzees in the Mahali mountains. Primates 9:167–224

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell JF, Hawkes K, Blurton Jones NG (1999) Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. J Hum Evol 36(5):461–485

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parker ST, McKinney ML (1999) The evolution of cognitive development in monkeys, apes, and humans. The Johns Hopkins University Press, Baltimore

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul R (2018) Culture from the perspective of dual inheritance. In: Quinn N (ed) Culture, mind and society: advances in culture theory from psychological anthropology. Springer, Cham, pp 47–74

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce CS (1956[1902]) The essence of mathematics. In: Newman JR (ed) The world of mathematics, vol. 3.Simon & Schuster, New York pp 1773–1783

  • Peirce CS (1976) The new elements of mathematics, vol. 4, ed. C. Eisele. The Hague: Mouton.

  • Pinker S (1989) Learnability and cognition: the acquisition of argument structure. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Pizlo Z, Stefanov F (2013) Solving large problems with a small working memory. J Probl Solving 6(1):34–43

    Google Scholar 

  • Polyani M (1946[1964]) Science, faith and society. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

  • Quinn N (2018) An anthropologist’s view of American marriage: limitations of the tool kit theory of culture. In: Quinn N (ed) Culture, mind and society: advances in culture theory from psychological anthropology. Springer, Cham, pp 139–184

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Read D (1984) An algebraic account of the American kinship terminology. Curr Anthropol 25: 4l7–440

  • Read D (2007) Kinship theory: a paradigm shift. Ethnology 46(4):329–364

    Google Scholar 

  • Read D (2008) Working memory: a cognitive limit to non-human primate recursive thinking prior to hominid evolution. Evol Psychol 6(4):676–714

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read D (2011) Mathematical representation of cultural constructs. In: Kronenfeld DB, Bennardo G, de Munck VC, Fischer MD (eds) A companion to cognitive anthropology. Blackwell, Oxford, pp 229–253

    Google Scholar 

  • Read D (2012) How culture makes us human. Series: Big Ideas in Little Books. Walnut Creek: Left Coast Press

  • Read D (2015) Formal models of kinship. In: International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences, 2nd edn, vol 13. J. D. Wright Editor-in-chief, Oxford: Elsevier, pp 53–60

  • Read, D. 2017. Quantitative differences between the working memory of chimpanzees and humans gives rise to qualitative differences: subitizing and cranial development. Human Complex Systems. UCLA. https://escholarship.org/uc/item/5d06v437.

  • Read D (2019) From past to present: the deep history of kinship. In: Saqalli M, Vander Linden M (eds) Integrating qualitative and social science factors in archaeological modelling. Springer, Berlin, pp 1–20

    Google Scholar 

  • Read D, Andersson C (2019) Cultural complexity and complexity evolution. Adapt Behav 27:1–50

  • Read D, Fischer MD, Chit Hlaing FKL (2014) The cultural grounding of kinship: a paradigm shift. L’Homme 210(2):63–89

    Google Scholar 

  • Read D, Lane D, van der Leeuw S (2009) The innovation innovation. In: Lane D, Pumain D, van der Leeuw S, West G (eds) Complexity perspectives on innovation and social change. Springer, Berlin, pp 43–84

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Read D, van der Leeuw S (2008) Biology is only part of the story. Philos Trans R Soc B 363:1959–1968

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Read D, van der Leeuw S (2015) The extension of social relations in time and space during the Palaeolithic and beyond. In: Wenban-Smith F, Coward F, Hosfield R, Pope M (eds) Settlement, society, and cognition in human evolution. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 31–53

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rightmire GP (2004) Brain size and encephalization in early to mid-Pleistocene Homo. Am J Phys Anthropol 124:109–123

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff CB, Trinkhaus E, Holliday TW (1997) Body mass and encephalization in Pleistocene Homo. Nature 387:173–176

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushmore J, Caillaud D, Matamba L, Stumpf RM, Borgatti SP, Altizer S (2013) Social network analysis of wild chimpanzees provides insights for predicting infectious disease risk. J Anim Ecol 88(4):923–1115

    Google Scholar 

  • Sahlins M (1962) Culture and nature on a Fijian island. University of Michigan Press, Ann Arbor

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz T (1981) The acquisition of culture. Ethos 9(1):4–17

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shils E (1981) Tradition. University of Chicago Press, Chicago

    Google Scholar 

  • Smaldino P (2014) The cultural evolution of emergent group-level traits. Behav Brain Sci 37:243–295

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spradley JP, Mann BJ (1975) The cocktail waitress: women's work in a man's world. Wiley, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Stjernfelt F (2012) The evolution of semiotic self-control. In: Schilhab T, Stjernfelt F, Deacon T (eds) The symbolic species evolved. Springer, Dordrecht, pp 39–64

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Strauss C (2015) Language and culture in cognitive anthropology. In: Sharafian F (ed) The Routledge handbook of language and culture. Routledge, New York, pp 386–400

    Google Scholar 

  • Strum SC, Latour B (1987) Redefining the social link: from baboons to humans. Soc Sci Inf 26:783–802

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Terray E (2010) La vision du monde de Claude Lévi-Strauss. l’Homme 193:23–44

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2014) A natural history of human thinking. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (2019) Becoming human: a theory of ontogeny. Belknap Press, Cambridge

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Tonkinson R (1991[1978]) The Mardu Aborigines: living the dream in Australia’s desert. Case Studies in Cultural Anthropology. Holt, Rinehart and Winston, New York

  • Trautmann T (2001) The whole history of kinship terminology in three chapters: before Morgan, Morgan, and after Morgan. Anthropol Theory 1(2):268–287

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tylor E (1871) Primitive culture: research into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom, vol 1. John Murray, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigilant L, Hofreiter M, Siedel H, Boesch C (2001) Paternity and relatedness in wild chimpanzee communities. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98(23):12890–12895

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Völter, C.J., R. Mundry, J. Call and A.M. Seed. (2019) Chimpanzees flexibly update working memory contents and show susceptibility to distraction in the self-ordered search task. Proc R Soc B 286:20190715. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.0715

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Voorhees B,Read D,Gabora L (2020) Identity, kinship, and the evolution of cooperation. Current Anthropology

  • Walker HL, Herndon JG (2008) Menopause in nonhuman primates? Biol Reprod 79(3):398–406

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walker RS, Hill KR, Flinn MV, Ellsworth RM (2011) Evolutionary history of hunter-gatherer marriage practices. PLoS ONE 6(4):e19066. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019066

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weaver AH, Holliday TW, Ruff CB, Trinkaus E (2001) The fossil evidence for the evolution of human intelligence in Pleistocene Homo. In: Nowell A (ed) In the mind’s eye: multidisciplinary approaches to the evolution of human cognition. International Monographs in Prehistory, Ann Arbor, pp 154–171

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wich SA, Sterck EHM, Utami SS (1999) Are orang-utan females as solitary as chimpanzee females? Folia Primatol 70:23–28

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wikipedia contributors (2019) Hypostatic abstraction. In: Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia. https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Hypostatic_abstraction&oldid=913837187. Accessed 6 Apr 2020

  • Wimsatt WC, Griesemer JR (2007) Reproducing entrenchments to scaffold culture: the central role of development in cultural evolution. In: Sansom R, Brandon RN (eds) Integrating evolution and development: from theory to practice. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 227–323

    Google Scholar 

  • Witowski S (1972) Gutmann scaling of semantic distinctions. In: Reining P (ed) Kinship studies in the Morgan Centennial Year. Smithsonian Institution, Washington, pp 167–188

    Google Scholar 

  • Wobber V, Herrmann E, Hare B, Wrangham R, Tomasello M (2014) Differences in the early cognitive development of children and great apes. Dev Psychobiol 56(3):547–573. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yerkes RM (1927) A program of anthropoid research. Am J Psychol 39:181–199

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeman JJ (1982) Peirce on abstraction. The Monist 65(2):211–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The author declares that he has received no funding for this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Dwight Read.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Read, D. From Pan to Homo sapiens: evolution from individual based to group based forms of social cognition. Mind Soc 19, 121–161 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-020-00230-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-020-00230-8

Keywords

Navigation