Skip to main content

METHODS article

Front. Psychol., 13 June 2017
Sec. Quantitative Psychology and Measurement

Construct Validity and Reliability of a New Spanish Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Early Adolescents

  • 1Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas, Buenos Aires, Argentina
  • 2Centro de Investigaciones en Psicología y Ciencias Afines, Universidad Adventista del Plata, Libertador San Martin, Argentina

Empathy is a basic socio-emotional process of human development that involves the ability to perceive, share, and understand the emotional states of others. This process is essential to successful social functioning. However, despite its significance, empathy has been difficult to define and measure, particularly when incorporating both its emotional and cognitive aspects. The purpose of this study was to develop an Empathy Questionnaire for children aged 9–12 years based on a model of social cognitive neuroscience and to analyze its construct validity and reliability. This questionnaire aimed to integrate the following aspects: emotional contagion, self-other awareness, perspective-taking, emotional regulation, and empathic action. Three studies were conducted. Study 1 evaluated the discriminative power of the items and studied the underlying structure of the instrument using exploratory factor analysis. In Study 2, confirmatory factor analysis was performed to test the model obtained. Finally, the goal of Study 3 was to analyze the convergent and discriminant validity of the questionnaire and the internal consistency of its dimensions. The final version of the instrument contained 15 items that operationalized the previously listed dimensions. The results of the 3 studies indicated that the questionnaire had good validity and reliability. This study has important implications for research and clinical practice. Given its simplicity and brevity, this new self-report scale may work well as a screening method to evaluate the key psychological issues underlying numerous child behaviors that predict the success or failure of social relationships, individual quality of life, and mental well-being.

Introduction

Empathy is an indispensable skill for human beings, whose lives are based on complex social contexts. The social nature of human beings is such that the recognition and understanding of the mental states of others and the ability to share these mental states and adequately respond to them are equally or more important than the understanding of and response to non-social natural contexts (López et al., 2014). This social and emotional competence underlies some of the most important human interactions, from the early bonding between mother and child to other complex prosocial behaviors (Batson, 2009).

Considering the significant role that empathy plays in human development, it is essential to assess and measure it to identify when its development has been altered and when to intervene. Recent advances in social cognitive neuroscience have indicated that empathy, as an inductive process, can be described and thus be measured. This realization has led to significant implications for its conceptualization (Decety and Moriguchi, 2007; Decety and Meyer, 2008). Thus, the objective of this article is to construct a five-dimensional questionnaire of empathy for children 9–12 years based in cognitive neuroscience developments.

According to Bensalah et al. (2016) to date, four questionnaires have been used to assess empathy in middle childhood (Bryant, 1982; Litvack-Miller et al., 1997; Garton and Gringart, 2005; Reid et al., 2013). Garton and Gringart's (2005) questionnaire studies the role of empathy in collaborative problem solving in school education. Garton and Gringart's (2005) and Litvack-Miller et al.'s (1997) questionnaires, used with children aged 7–9 or 7–11 years, respectively, are independent adaptations of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI; Davis, 1980). The Index of Empathy (Bryant, 1982) for children aged 6–12 years only assess emotional empathy as a vicarious emotional response to the emotion of another person. Reid and colleagues developed the Kids' Empathic Development Scale (Reid et al., 2013). This scale assesses empathy in 7–11 year-olds that have to answer to emotional stories. It considers three factors: an affective component, a cognitive component referring to the justifications that children produce to explain their emotional inference, and a behavioral component that concerns adaptive behaviors in relation to the emotional scenario (Bensalah et al., 2016). Additionally, the Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Adolescents (Pouw et al., 2013) operationalizes the affective aspects of empathy.

Finally, Bensalah et al. (2016) have adapted for French children aged 6–11 years, the adult French version of the Basic Empathy Scale (Carré et al., 2013) of a 20-item scale developed by Jolliffe and Farrington (2006) for adolescents. Bensalah et al. postulate a three-factor model of empathy: emotional contagion, cognitive empathy, and emotional disconnection. Except in the latter case, which is based on three primary aspects of empathy suggested by Decety and Moriguchi (2007), the scales mentioned before do not offer a measure of empathy that reflect the understanding of empathy based in neuroscience.

Some current models have integrated the automatic or unconscious emotional experience of the observed or inferred emotional state with the recognition and understanding of the emotional states of others through controlled cognitive processes. Decety and Jackson (2004), for example, present a model that accounts for the affective and cognitive components of empathy. They argue that reflecting the emotions of others is a basic process that can occur without the intervention of consciousness, whereas processes that characterize human empathy, such as perspective taking and self-regulation, require controlled processing.

Empathy not only involves the emotional experience of another person's emotional state or emotional contagion, whether real or inferred, but also the recognition and a minimal understanding of the emotional state of the other person. This definition captures the multidimensional nature of empathy and explicitly references a minimum capacity to mentalize (Decety and Moriguchi, 2007). Some authors suggest that there is neural basis that supports a model of two separate systems for empathy: an emotional system and a cognitive system (Shamay-Tsoory, 2011; Cox et al., 2012).

Hoffman (2000) suggested empathy might represent a developmental process, progressing from automatic mimicry (Iacoboni, 2009) to cognitive processing that involves the ability to imagine the experiences of another. In the same line, Decety and Jackson (2004) demonstrated that observable brain activity was associated with four subjectively experienced components of empathy: affective sharing, self-awareness, perspective taking, and emotion regulation. Decety and Moriguchi's (2007) descriptions of these four components increased the clarity of the overall conceptualization of empathy (Lietz et al., 2011).

Recent research has suggested a fifth potentially important component of empathy (Lietz et al., 2011). Gerdes and Segal (2009) have presented a 3-dimensional diagram based on the developments of Decety et al. (Decety and Jackson, 2004; Decety and Lamm, 2006). This model includes three components: (1) the affective response to the emotions and actions of others, (2) the cognitive processing of the other's perspective and one's own affective response, and (3) the conscious decision making to undertake an empathic or prosocial action that Eisenberg (2006, p. 71) called empathy related responding, which would explain the relationship between empathy and prosocial behavior. According to this model, the empathic awareness of another's discomfort (e.g., pain, distress) should lead to solidarity or altruism (Lietz et al., 2011).

In view of the theoretical considerations above, this study aimed to design a self-report instrument that would obtain a multidimensional measure of empathy for 9–12 year-old children based on the model of Decety and Jackson (2004). The model would include the empathic action dimension suggested by Gerdes and Segal (2009). Accordingly, the objective was to operationalize the following dimensions of empathy in children: emotional contagion, self-other awareness, perspective-taking, emotional regulation, and empathic action, questionnaire integrates affective and cognitive components of empathy. According to our best knowledge, there is no other questionnaire to measure empathy in children based on Decety and Moriguchi's (2007) four components model and including empathic action.

The measurement of the empathy construct is challenging, particularly for children. In addition to children's known difficulties in responding to questionnaires (i.e., less attention, language development) is the complexity of the dimensions to be implemented and the development of items for children that have content validity (Sartori and Pasini, 2007; Lietz et al., 2011). The items referred to self-awareness, for example, are very complex since the child must simultaneously look for what he felt at the moment of affective excitement (Lamm et al., 2007) and then suspend his own experience to evoke the thoughts and feelings of others. In turn, this skill is a prerequisite for perspective taking, which is a similar but distinct process because it involves that the other is different and yet to be put in their place. Emotional regulation implies changing the way people think which in turn changes the way they feel—a complex cognitive process (Ochsner et al., 2002; Lietz et al., 2011). Finally, when dealing with children, these complex processes must be expressed in short sentences with the least possible logical complexity.

We have chosen this stage of development in children because all the empathic dimensions proposed have already developed, although some of them continue to mature through late childhood and adolescence. Between 18 and 72 h after birth, newborns who hear other children cry often show distress reactions, suggesting an automatic or reactive contagion-type mechanism (Simner, 1971; Sagi and Hoffman, 1976; Martin and Clark, 1982). During the second year of life, throughout the development of the self-other differentiation, perspective-taking, and emotional regulation, a transformation from self-concern to concern for others occurs (Knafo et al., 2008). By 2 years of age, children begin to exhibit the basic behaviors of empathy by having emotional responses that correspond to other people's emotional states and by manifesting more organized prosocial behaviors (e.g., helping, sharing, or comforting) indicative of concern for others (Hoffman, 2000; Decety and Jackson, 2004). In the preschool years (4–5 years old), children are capable of taking the perspective of the other in pretend tasks that are frequently used as indicators of the development of a theory of mind (Wimmer and Perner, 1983; Wellman et al., 2001). Growth in the ability to identify oneself with the experience of others permits children between 7 and 12 years of age to show a natural inclination to feel empathy for others who suffer pain (Decety et al., 2008) and to develop more effective helping strategies (McDonald and Messinger, 2011). Surtees and Apperly (2012) found that perspective taking increases during the middle childhood period. Children's progress in emotional understanding is due to improvements both in making inferences about others' mental states and in attentional, memory, and inhibitory control abilities. Emotion regulation skills develop throughout childhood and adolescence, gradually allowing children to control emotion, affect, motivation, and urges (Bensalah et al., 2016). The mastery of emotional regulation occurs gradually across an extended period of childhood (e.g., Zimmer-Gembeck and Skinner, 2011). Cognitive control becomes more efficient between 8 and 12 years of age (Choudhury et al., 2006) and continues to improve between the ages of 12–13 and 19 years (Keulers et al., 2010; Vetter et al., 2013; Van der Graaff et al., 2014). Children aged 6–10 begin to understand that there are social group's informal norms about when, where, and how one should express emotions (Siegler, 2006). They come to appreciate the contexts in which certain emotional expressions are socially most appropriate and therefore ought to be regulated (Harris, 1983). This capacity of emotional self-regulation is related to sympathy or the affective aspect of empathy. Adequately self-regulated children would be expected to experience others' emotions vicariously yet not become overly aroused and overwhelmed by the emotion (Eisenberg, 2000).

Procedures for the Development of the Scale Items

The items were developed based on the theoretical models proposed by Decety and Jackson (2004) and by Gerdes and Segal (2009). The expressions used in each item were clear and simple and employed typical expressions for Argentine children aged 9–12 years.

Alternative expressions were suggested for some items and experts on the subject selected the best option. Thus, a preliminary scale of 50 items was formed, 10 for each dimension, which was presented to a sample of 17 children (9 girls and 8 boys) evenly representing different ages (4 children aged 9, 4 children aged 10, 5 children aged 11, and 4 children aged 12). The children were asked to assess the clarity of the instructions and the response options as well as the level of difficulty of the terms or expressions used in each sentence. Based on the children's recommendations, the instructions and the response options remained intact, but the items received revisions. Twelve statements were changed to get clearer expressions (e.g., “I find it easy to see the point of view of others” was replaced by “I find it easy to understand other people's different ways of thinking”); others items were excluded because they involved a level of abstraction too complex for this age group (e.g., “I can distinguish what I feel from what another person is feeling”), and a new one was proposed for the emotional regulation scale (e.g., “When I get angry, I find it difficult to calm down”).

The questionnaire resulting from the previous revision comprised 47 items: 7 on emotional contagion (e.g., “When I'm with someone who is sad, I also feel sad”); 10 on self-awareness, as different from the other (e.g., “Even though I am happy, I notice when a friend is angry”); 10 on perspective-taking (e.g., “Although others think differently, I can understand them”); 11 on emotional regulation (e.g., “When I get angry I find it extremely difficult to calm down”); and 9 on empathic action (e.g., “We must share with those who have less than us”).

Instrument Study

Study 1

The purpose of the first study was to evaluate the items' psychometric performance and to study the underlying structure of the instrument to assess the multidimensional construct of empathy in children.

Methods

Participants

Parental informed consent and voluntary consent of the children was received prior to participation. An intentional, non-probabilistic sample of 205 school children (112 girls, 54.6%) from the Entre Rios, Misiones, and Buenos Aires provinces was used. Children were in the fifth (39%), sixth (36.6%), and seventh (24.4%) grades of five elementary schools participating in the first study. Mean age was 11.14 years (SD = 1.19; rank 9–12 years old). The children belonged to middle socioeconomic status, being the distribution of the mother's educational level was: 0% primary education, 40% secondary education, and 60% with tertiary or higher educational studies and distribution of the father's educational level was: 0% primary education, 35% secondary education, and 75% with tertiary or higher educational studies. The distribution of the occupation of the head of the family was: 15% qualified manual labor, 20% administrative and sales, and 65% university-level professionals, financiers, business people, all high productivity.

Instruments

The instrument administered was the preliminary version of the multidimensional Empathy Questionnaire for children. It comprised 47 items with four response options: always, often, sometimes, and never.

Procedures

The discriminative power of the items was examined using a contrasting groups method; the children's responses were analyzed using a t-test to identify the difference in means for independent samples. This analysis determined whether there were statistically significant differences between children with higher empathy scores and those with lower scores (upper quartile or 75th percentile and lower quartile or 25th percentile). A Principal Component Analysis (PCA) was run to investigate the dimensionality of the instrument.

Results

All of the items were discriminative (p < 0.001; See Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Discriminative power of the items in the final version of the scale.

The value of the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of Sampling Adequacy was 0.759, and the Bartlett's test of sphericity was χ2 = 452.12; p < 0.001.

When defining the factors, only items weighing 0.40 or more were considered. After discarding factorially complex items, that is those with a similar weighing on more than one factor and those weighing less than the predetermined value in the corresponding factor, a simple structure consisting of 15 items was obtained. This structure comprised three items per factor that were answered on a 4-point scale (from always to never) and accounted for 55.46% of the variance (see Table 2). The McDonald's omega coefficients for each factor are shown at Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results of the Principal ComponentAnalysis, Factor Matrix, and Oblimin Rotation.

Study 2

This study aimed to test the model obtained in the first study through a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA).

Methods

Participants

Prior parental informed consent and voluntary participation of the children was obtained. The second study was performed with an intentional sample of 212 school children (119 girls, 55.9%) from the Entre Rios, Misiones, and Buenos Aires provinces who were in the fifth (32%), sixth (38%), and seventh (30%) grade of four elementary schools. Their mean age was 11.08 years (SD = 0.94; rank 9–12 years). The children belonged to the middle socioeconomic class, being the distribution of the mother's educational level was: 0% primary education, 37% secondary education, and 63% with tertiary or higher educational studies, and the father's educational level was: 0% primary education, 38% secondary education, and 62% with tertiary or higher educational studies. The distribution of the occupation of the head of the family was: 10% qualified manual labor, 25% administrative and sales, and 65% university-level professionals, financiers, business people, all high productivity.

Instruments

The instrument administered was the 15-item version of the multidimensional Empathy Questionnaire for children that resulted from study 1. In the present study, McDonald's omega was as follows: Emotional Contagion = 0.78, Self-others Awareness = 0.75, Perspective-Taking = 0.72, Emotional Regulation = 0.72, and Empathic Action = 0.70.

Procedures

To verify whether the data from a new study sample fit the structure found by the PCA performed in study 1, using Maximum Likelihood Estimator a CFA was conducted to assess the fit indices: GFI (Goodness of Fit Index), CFI (Comparative Fit Index), and NFI (Bentler-Bonett Normed Fit Index), and RMSEA (Root Mean Square Error of Approximation) to assess the degree of error of the model. The CFA was performed through AMOS 16.0 (Arbuckle, 2007).

Results

Construct validity

The five-factor model fit the data very well, as we can see through the values of fit indexes: χ2(80) = 88.041, p = 0.252, χ2/df = 1.10; GFI = 0.95; NFI = 0.91, CFI = 0.98, and RMSEA = 0.02, and it is depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Five factor model of empathy. *p ≤ 0.05, **p ≤ 0.01, ***p ≤ 0.001.

Study 3

The purpose of the third study was to verify the construct validity of the instrument by testing its convergent and discriminant validity and to analyze the internal consistency of the different dimensions of empathy.

Several studies found that individuals who frequently participated in prosocial behaviors were more empathic and showed less aggressive behaviors (Carlo et al., 1992, 2011; Eisenberg et al., 2006; Garaigordobil and De Galdeano, 2006). The dimensions of empathy that were evaluated by the new instrument were therefore hypothesized to have a positive relationship with prosociality and a negative relationship with physical and verbal aggression. Moreover, evidence exists about a negative relationship between emotional instability and the dimension of emotional regulation (Singh and Mishra, 2011; Khosla, 2012; Bujor and Turliuc, 2014). Thus, a negative relationship between these constructs was hypothesized. A positive correlation between empathic action and prosocial behavior was also expected to be observed, as well as a positive relationship between perspective-taking as assessed by the IRI (Davis, 1983) and the perspective-taking dimension of the new instrument.

Methods

Participants

A total of 479 schoolchildren (257 girls, 53.7%), in the fourth (19.4%), fifth (25.7%), sixth (29%), and seventh (25.9%) grades of four primary schools participated in this study. The participants were selected using a purposive sampling framework. Their mean age was 10.77 years (SD = 1.16; rank 9–12 years). The children were from the middle socioeconomic class, being the distribution of the mother's educational level: 0% primary education, 38% secondary education, and 62% with tertiary or higher educational studies, and of the father's educational level: 0% primary education, 35% secondary education, and 65% with tertiary or higher educational studies. The distribution of the occupation of the head of the family was: 12% qualified manual labor, 28% administrative and sales, and 60% university-level professionals, financiers, business people, all high productivity.

Instruments

In addition to the multidimensional instrument of empathy for children presented in studies 1 and 2, the following questionnaires were administered.

– The Prosocial Behavior Scale (PB, Caprara and Pastorelli, 1993, Spanish version by Del Barrio et al., 2001). Prosocial behavior encompasses all those voluntary acts that contribute to the benefit of other individuals (Eisenberg and Mussen, 1989; Roche Olivar, 2011). The PB is a scale composed of 15 items that evaluate the behavior of altruism, trust, and pleasantness through three response alternatives as a function of frequency with each of the behaviors described (often, sometimes, and never). This self-report provides an overall measure of child prosociality. The internal consistency obtained in this study was α = 0.79.

– The Physical and Verbal Aggression Scale (FVA, Caprara and Pastorelli, 1993; Spanish version by Del Barrio et al., 2001). This scale is a 20-item self-report questionnaire that evaluates physically and verbally behavior that is harmful to others. The response format consists of 3 options: often, sometimes, and never. In this study, the internal consistency of the scale was α = 0.73.

– The Emotional Instability Scale (EI, Caprara and Pastorelli, 1993; Spanish version by Del Barrio et al., 2001). This scale measures the lack of self-control in social situations as the result of a limited capacity to curb impulsiveness and emotionality. This instrument is self-reported and consists of 20 items. The questionnaire uses 3 response options: often, sometimes, and never. The internal consistency of the scale in this study was α = 0.83.

– The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI, Davis, 1980). The IRI operationalize a cognitive and affective multidimensional empathy construct. IRI measures four dimensions: (a) fantasy, the tendency to identify with characters in films and in literature; in other words, it assesses the subject's imaginative capacity to place him or herself in fictitious situations, (b) perspective-taking, or the ability to understand another person's point of view, (c) emotional distress, feelings of anxiety and restlessness shown by the person when observing situations in which another person experiences negative experiences, and (d) empathic concern, feelings of compassion, concern, and care toward others. In the present study, we administered an adapted version for children of IRI carried out in Argentina (Richaud de Minzi, 2007). In this study, we only considered the dimension Perspective Taking, since the objective of this study was to evaluate the convergent validity of the new scale of empathy proposed in this work, and this dimension is the only one that would have a greater theoretical correspondence with it. In this study sample, the Cronbach's alpha was 0.64. The response format consists of 5 options: the statement describes me very well, well, somewhat, a little, and not at all.

– The Multidimensional Scaling of Children Prosociality (Lemos, 2015). This scale evaluates the following prosocial behaviors through 23 items: assistance, giving and sharing, comforting, positive confirmation of the other, and cooperation. The possible responses to the items are always, sometimes, rarely, and never. The internal consistency of the overall scale that was obtained for this study sample was α = 0.90.

Procedures

The instruments were administered collectively in two different situations to avoid fatigue of the children. Three scales were completed in the first session (Prosocial Behavior Scale, Physical and Verbal Aggression Scale, and Emotional Instability Scale) and two in the second (Interpersonal Reactivity Index and Multidimensional Scaling of Children Prosociality). The order of administration was maintained constant for all school groups.

To provide further evidence of the instrument's validity, Pearson's r correlations were performed to assess the strength and the direction of the relationship between the dimensions of empathy represented in the new Questionnaire and the following variables: IRI perspective-taking, prosocial behavior, emotional instability, and physical and verbal aggression.

Results

Convergent validity

Significant and positive correlations were observed between the results of the two prosocial behavior scales (Caprara and Pastorelli, 1993; Lemos, 2015) and the dimensions of the new Empathy Questionnaire: emotional contagion, self-other awareness, perspective-taking, emotional regulation, and empathic action. A significant positive correlation between perspective-taking assessed by the IRI and perspective-taking assessed by the new Empathy Questionnaire was also obtained (see Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlations between emotional contagion, self-other awareness, perspective-taking, emotional regulation, and empathic action with prosocial behavior, emotional instability, aggression, and IRI perspective-taking.

Discriminant validity

Significant negative correlations were found between the results obtained in the physical and verbal aggression scale and the emotional contagion, self-other awareness, perspective-taking, emotional regulation, and empathic action dimensions of the new Empathy Questionnaire. A significant negative correlation was also obtained between emotional instability and emotional regulation (see Table 3).

Reliability

The McDonald's omega of the dimensions were 0.75 for emotional contagion, 0.76 for self-other awareness, 0.72 for perspective-taking, 0.72 for emotional regulation, and 0.70 for empathic action. The individual item-total correlations for the five-factor structure are provided in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Item-total correlations.

General Discussion

The aim of the present study was to study the reliability and construct validity of a new empathy questionnaire for children of 9–12 years old.

As the concept of Emotional contagion appears very early in development (Decety and Jackson, 2004) the children were not expected to experience any difficulties responding to the corresponding items. A similar ease of responding was also expected for the items relating to self-consciousness, self-other-awareness, perspective-taking, and emotional regulation, processes that are expected to have developed at the age of study.

In the case of Empathic action, it is interesting to note this factor did not appear clearly in a previous study in which a preliminary version of the scale was developed (Richaud et al., 2015). Some of the empathic action items showed factorial complexity, while others had low factorial loads. Many of the items developed to operationalize empathic action involved a set of generalized beliefs and causal attributions about the misfortune of others as well as the determination of whether it was appropriate to assist them. For example, if it appears that the misfortunes of others are the result of their own voluntary decisions or their ineffectiveness (example: “It is wrong to give things to beggars; they ask for things because they do not want to work.”), the individual may be less likely to elicit empathic action. However, if the misfortune is attributed to causes beyond the control of the individual (example: “I think we all have to help kids in need”), the individual is more likely to have an empathic and caring disposition.

At first, this lack of clarity regarding the empathic action factor was attributed to a problem with its development, as the ability to enact an empathic action implied the internalization of certain values (e.g., solidarity, help, or altruism) that required the comprehension of abstract concepts not fully realized until adolescence. However, at 7 years old, children have already been shown to feel empathy for others suffering from pain (Decety et al., 2008) and accordingly to develop strategies of help and comfort.

Therefore, a better hypothesis may be that when items refer to a particular or a contextualized situation, individuals' responses may change and adapt based on their causal attribution to the situation. The contextual modulation of empathy makes an individual's behavior more sensitive to the conditions of different environments. To modify empathic actions according to the environment, our brains must access the available contextual information to predict the social meanings involved (e.g., others' intentions, feelings, and behavior) on the basis of previous experiences and the relevance of the particular situation (Hein and Singer, 2008; Singer and Lamm, 2009).

The relationship between empathy and prosociality has been well established (Bartal et al., 2011; Richaud, 2014; Williams et al., 2014). However, prosociality scales that contained general and not context-dependent statements (e.g., We must help those who suffer) were used in these studies. Because of these considerations, the authors of this study decided to retain items of empathic action on the scale that related to general situations to obtain the fifth dimension of the questionnaire.

In this study, an empathy scale for children was developed that was based on a multidimensional model that posits a series of stages in the development of empathy (Decety and Jackson, 2004; Decety and Moriguchi, 2007). In addition, the psychometric properties of the scale were analyzed. This study provides an original and significant contribution to the literature, as the operationalization of this model is unprecedented in Argentina.

Strengths and Limitations

The sound psychometric properties of the new scale confirm the notion that cognitive and affective aspects of empathy are closely intertwined. Accordingly, it would be inappropriate to exclude either of these aspects if the purpose is to achieve a comprehensive overview of the phenomenon of empathy. Based on the results obtained in this study, further analysis of the interactions between the five dimensions included in this model as well as their correlations with other processes not covered in this article may lead to a better understanding of the overall empathic experience.

This study has important implications for research and clinical practice. Given its simplicity and brevity, this new self-report scale may work well as a screening method to evaluate the key psychological issues underlying numerous child behaviors that predict the success or failure of social relationships, individual quality of life, and mental well-being.

The limitations of this study include the sampling criteria used for the selection of participants, as convenience sampling has several disadvantages compared to probability samples. Therefore, it may be appropriate to test the generalizability of these results in future studies by using samples that reflect a broader range of sociodemographic characteristics and a greater geographic diversity of the participants. The inclusion of a social desirability assessment is also recommended, as children have a tendency to provide socially acceptable answers to please others (Lemos, 2005, 2006); this is a general limitation of objective self-administered questionnaires (Lemos, 2013), so it would be advisable to complement the evaluation of the construct with other measures and informants (teachers, parents).

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of APA Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by ethics committee of the Centro Interdisciplinario de Investigación Matemática y Experimental (CIIPME).

Author Contributions

MR designed the project, lead the data collection, wrote the Introduction and Discussion sections. VL and BM designed the project, lead the data collection, computed the analyses, wrote the Method, Results, and Discussion sections. LO lead the data collection and computed preliminary analyses.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Arbuckle, J. L. (2007). Amos™ 18 User's Guide. Crawfordville, FL: Amos Development Corporation.

Google Scholar

Bartal, I. B. A., Decety, J., and Mason, P. (2011). Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science 334, 1427–1430. doi: 10.1126/science.1210789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D. (2009). “These things called empathy: eight related but distinct phenomena,” in Social Neuroscience of Empathy, eds J. Decety and W. Ickes (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 3–16.

Google Scholar

Bensalah, L., Stefaniak, N., Carre, A., and Besche-Richard, C. (2016). The Basic Empathy Scale adapted to French middle childhood: structure and development of empathy. Behav. Res. 48, 1410–1420. doi: 10.3758/s13428-015-0650-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bryant, B. K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents. Child Dev. 53, 413–425. doi: 10.2307/1128984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bujor, L., and Turliuc, M. N. (2014). Personality, family correlates, and emotion regulation as wellbeing predictors. Proc. Soc. Behav. Sci. 159, 142–146. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2014.12.346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caprara, G. V., and Pastorelli, C. (1993). Early emotional instability, prosocial behavior, and aggression: some methodological aspects. Eur. J. Pers. 7, 19–36. doi: 10.1002/per.2410070103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlo, G., Eisenberg, N., and Knight, G. P. (1992). An objective measure of adolescents' prosocial moral reasoning. J. Res. Adolesc. 2, 331–349. doi: 10.1207/s15327795jra0204_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carlo, G., Mestre, M. V., Samper, P., Tur, A., and Armenta, B. E. (2011). The longitudinal relations among dimensions of parenting styles, sympathy, prosocial moral reasoning, and prosocial behaviors. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 35, 116–124. doi: 10.1177/0165025410375921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carré, A., Stefaniak, N., D'Ambrosio, F., Bensalah, L., and Besche- Richard, C. (2013). The Basic Empathy Scale in adults (BES-A): factor structure of a revised form. Psychol. Assess. 25, 679–691. doi: 10.1037/a0032297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Choudhury, S., Blakemore, S.-J., and Charman, T. (2006). Social cognitive development during adolescence. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 1, 165–174. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, C. L., Uddin, L. Q., Di Martino, A., Castellanos, F. X., Milham, M. P., and Kelly, C. (2012). The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 7, 727–737. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsr051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1980). A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog Select. Doc. Psychol. 10, 85.

Google Scholar

Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Jackson, H. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 3, 71–100. doi: 10.1177/1534582304267187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Lamm, C. (2006). Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Sci. World J. 6, 1146–1163. doi: 10.1100/tsw.2006.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Meyer, M. (2008). From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: a social developmental neuroscience account. Dev. Psychopathol. 20, 1053–1080. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., Michalska, K. J., and Akitsuki, Y. (2008). Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children. Neuropsychologia 46, 2607–2614. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Decety, J., and Moriguchi, Y. (2007). The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions. Biopsychosoc. Med. 1, 22–65. doi: 10.1186/1751-0759-1-22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Barrio, V., Moreno, C., and López, R. (2001). Evaluación de la agresión e inestabilidad emocional en ni-os espa-oles y su relación con la depresión [Evaluation of aggression and emotional instability in Spanish children and its relation with depression]. Clín. Salud 13, 33–50.

Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 51, 665–697. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.51.1.665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N. (2006). “Empathy-related responding and prosocial behaviour,” in Empathy and fairness: Novartis Foundation Symposium 278, eds G. Bock and J. Goode (Chichester, UK: Wiley & Sons), 71–88.

Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., and Mussen, P. (1989). The Roots of Prosocial Behavior in Children. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., and Sadovsky, A. (2006). “Empathy-related responding in children,” in Handbook of Moral Development, eds M. Killen and J. G. Smetana (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers), 517–549.

Google Scholar

Garaigordobil, M., and De Galdeano, P. G. (2006). Empatía en niños de 10 a 12 años. Psicothema 18, 180–186.

Google Scholar

Garton, A. F., and Gringart, E. (2005). The development of a scale to measure empathy in 8- and 9-year old children. Aust. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 5, 17–25.

Google Scholar

Gerdes, K., and Segal, E. (2009). A social work model of empathy. Adv. Soc. Work 10, 114–127.

Google Scholar

Harris, P. L. (1983). Children's understanding of the link between situation and emotion. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 36, 490–509. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(83)90048-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, G., and Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: the empathic brain and its modulation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 18, 153–158. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, D. D. (2000). Visual Intelligence: How We Create What We See. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Company.

Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 60, 653–670. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jolliffe, D., and Farrington, D. P. (2006). Development and validation of the Basic Empathy Scale. J. Adolesc. 29, 589–611. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2005.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keulers, E. H. H., Evers, E. A. T., Stiers, P., and Jolles, J. (2010). Age, sex, and pubertal phase influence mentalizing about emotions and actions in adolescents. Dev. Neuropsychol. 35, 555–569. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.494920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khosla, M. (2012). Emotion regulation and well-being. Appl. Res. Qual. Life 7, 323–325. doi: 10.1007/s11482-011-9162-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knafo, A., Zahn-Waxler, C., Van Hulle, C., Robinson, J. L., and Rhee, S. H. (2008). The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion 8, 737–752. doi: 10.1037/a0014179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., and Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 19, 42–58. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemos, V. (2005). Construcción y validación de una escala para evaluar la deseabilidad social infantil (EDESI) [Construction and validation of a scale to evaluate childish social desiderability]. Interdisciplinaria 22, 77–96.

Lemos, V. (2006). La deseabilidad social en la evaluación de la personalidad infantil [Social desiderability in the evaluation of childish personality]. Suma Psicol. 13, 7–14. doi: 10.14349/sumapsi2006.57

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemos, V. (2013). La operacionalización de constructos psicológicos en la infancia: dificultades y propuestas de superación [Operationalization of psychological constructs in childhood: difficulties and proposals of overcoming]. Anuario Psicol. 43, 189–199.

Lemos, V. (2015). “Estudio de la validez de constructo, convergente, factorial y predictiva del cuestionario de conducta prosocial para niños de 9 a 12 años [Study of construct, convergent, factorial, and predictive validity of prosocial behavior questionnaire for 9-12 years children],” in Avances en Investigación en Recursos Socio Cognitivos y Afectivos en la Ni-ez Media y Tardía. V. N. Lemos (Org.). Simposio Presentado en la XV Reunión Nacional y IV Encuentro Internacional de la Asociación Argentina de Ciencias del Comportamiento (Tucumán).

Lietz, C. A., Gerdes, K. E., Sun, F., Geiger, J. M., Wagaman, M. A., and Segal, E. A. (2011). The Empathy Assessment Index (EAI): a confirmatory factor analysis of a multidimensional model of empathy. J. Soc. Social Work Res. 2, 104–124. doi: 10.5243/jsswr.2011.6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Litvack-Miller, W., McDouglas, D., and Romney, D. M. (1997). The structure of empathy during middle childhood and its relationship to prosocial behavior. Genet. Soc. Psychol. Monogr. 123, 303–324.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

López, M., Arán, V., and Richaud de Minzi, M. C. (2014). Empatía: desde la percepción automática hasta los procesos controlados [Empathy: form automatic perception to controlled processes]. Avances de Psicología Latinoamericana 32, 37–51. doi: 10.12804/apl32.1.2014.03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, G. B., and Clark, R. D. (1982). Distress crying in neonates: species and peer specificity. Dev. Psychol. 18, 3–9. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.18.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, N. M., and Messinger, D. S. (2011). “The development of empathy: how, when, and why,” in Moral Behavior and Free Will: A Neurobiological and Philosophical Approach, eds A. Acerbi, J. A. Lombo, and J. J. Sanguineti (If Press), 341–368.

Ochsner, K. N., Bunge, S. A., Gross, J. J., and Gabrieli, J. D. (2002). Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 14, 1215–1229. doi: 10.1162/089892902760807212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pouw, L. B. C., Rieffe, C., Oosterveld, P., Huskens, B., and Stockmann, L. (2013). Reactive/proactive aggression and affective/cognitive empathy in children with ASD. Res. Dev. Disabil. 34, 1256–1266. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reid, C., Davis, H., Horlin, C., Anderson, M., Baughman, N., and Campbell, C. (2013). The Kids' Empathic Development Scale (KEDS): a multi-dimensional measure of empathy in primary school-aged children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 31, 231–256. doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richaud, M. C. (2014). Algunos aportes sobre la importancia de la empatía y la prosocialidad en el desarrollo humano [Some contributions on the importance of empathy and prosociality in human development]. Rev. Mexicana Invest. Psicol. 6, 171–176.

Google Scholar

Richaud, M. C., Lemos, V., and Oros, L. (2015). “Empathy in children: theory and assessment,” in Psychology and Neurobiology of Empathy, eds D. Watt and J. Panksepp (New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc.), 149–170.

Google Scholar

Richaud de Minzi, M. C. (2007). “Parental styles and attachment in relation with self-control, social skills and coping in children at risk for poverty,” in New Developments in Parent-Child Relations, ed D. M. Devor (Hauppauge, NY: Nova Editorial Publishers, Inc.), 87–110.

Roche Olivar, R. (2011). Psicología y Educación para la Prosocialidad. Buenos Aires: Ciudad Nueva.

Sagi, A., and Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Dev. Psychol. 12, 175–176. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.12.2.175

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sartori, R., and Pasini, M. (2007). Quality and quantity in test validity: How can we be sure that psychological tests measure what they have to? Qual. Quant. 41, 359–374. doi: 10.1007/s11135-006-9006-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G. (2011). The neural bases for empathy. Neuroscientist 17, 18–24. doi: 10.1177/1073858410379268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegler, R. (2006). How Children Develop, Exploring Child Develop Student Media Tool Kit & Scientific American Reader to Accompany How Children Develop. New York, NY: Worth Publishers.

Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn's response to the cry of another infant. Dev. Psychol. 5, 136–150. doi: 10.1037/h0031066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, T., and Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 81–96. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04418.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, S., and Mishra, R. (2011). Emotion regulation strategies and their implications for well-being. Soc. Sci. Intern. 27, 179–198.

Google Scholar

Surtees, A. D. R., and Apperly, I. A. (2012). Egocentrism and automatic perspective taking in children and adults. Child Dev. 83, 452–460. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01730.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van der Graaff, J., Branje, S., De Wied, M., Hawk, S., Van Lier, P., and Meeus, W. (2014). Perspective taking and empathic concern in adolescence: gender differences in developmental changes. Dev. Psychol. 50, 881–888. doi: 10.1037/a0034325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vetter, N. C., Altgassen, M., Phillips, L., Mahy, C. E. V., and Kliegel, M. (2013). Development of affective theory of mind across adolescence: disentangling the role of executive functions. Dev. Neuropsychol. 38, 114–125. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2012.733786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Williams, A., O'Driscoll, K., and Moore, C. (2014). The influence of empathic concern on prosocial behavior in children. Front. Psychol. 5:425. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wimmer, H., and Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J., and Skinner, E. A. (2011). The development of coping across childhood and adolescence: an integrative review and critique of research. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 35, 1–17. doi: 10.1177/0165025410384923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: multidimensional empathy, new questionnaire, evaluation, psychometrics, validity, childhood

Citation: Richaud MC, Lemos VN, Mesurado B and Oros L (2017) Construct Validity and Reliability of a New Spanish Empathy Questionnaire for Children and Early Adolescents. Front. Psychol. 8:979. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00979

Received: 10 February 2017; Accepted: 29 May 2017;
Published: 13 June 2017.

Edited by:

Pietro Cipresso, IRCCS Istituto Auxologico Italiano, Italy

Reviewed by:

Antonio Zuffiano, Liverpool Hope University, United Kingdom
Noelia Sánchez-Pérez, Universidad de Murcia, Spain

Copyright © 2017 Richaud, Lemos, Mesurado and Oros. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Maria C. Richaud, richaudmc@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.