Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 211, June 2021, 104632
Cognition

Multiple-image arrays in face matching tasks with and without memory

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104632Get rights and content

Abstract

Previous research has shown that exposure to within-person variability facilitates face learning. A different body of work has examined potential benefits of providing multiple images in face matching tasks. Viewers are asked to judge whether a target face matches a single face image (as when checking photo-ID) or multiple face images of the same person. The evidence here is less clear, with some studies finding a small multiple-image benefit, and others finding no advantage. In four experiments, we address this discrepancy in the benefits of multiple images from learning and matching studies. We show that multiple-image arrays only facilitate face matching when arrays precede targets. Unlike simultaneous face matching tasks, sequential matching and learning tasks involve memory and require abstraction of a stable representation of the face from the array, for subsequent comparison with a target. Our results show that benefits from multiple-image arrays occur only when this abstraction is required, and not when array and target images are available at once. These studies reconcile apparent differences between face learning and face matching and provide a theoretical framework for the study of within-person variability in face perception.

Introduction

We rely on faces to verify identity in a variety of situations ranging from buying alcohol to crossing borders. It is, therefore, important to understand how accurate we are at determining whether a photo-ID shows the person using it, and to identify potential ways to improve our performance in such tasks.

A large body of literature suggests that recognising familiar and unfamiliar faces entail some qualitatively different processes (Johnston & Edmonds, 2009; Megreya & Burton, 2006) and this could have serious practical implications. On the one hand, we are very good at recognising images of familiar identities even when these images are heavily distorted or degraded (e.g. Bruce, 1982, Bruce, 1986; Burton, Wilson, Cowan, & Bruce, 1999). On the other hand, recognition of unfamiliar identities is much poorer even with images taken on the same day or in the same session (e.g. Bruce et al., 1999; Burton, White, & McNeill, 2010). This stark contrast between familiar and unfamiliar faces has been demonstrated using many different tasks and paradigms including face memory, search and sorting tasks (Jenkins, White, Van Montfort, & Burton, 2011; Klatzky & Forrest, 1984; Kramer, Hardy, & Ritchie, 2020) as well as face matching tasks where typically two images are presented side-by-side on a computer screen, and participants are asked to judge whether the photos show the same person or different people (Bruce et al., 1999; Bruce, Henderson, Newman, & Burton, 2001; Clutterbuck and Johnston, 2002, Clutterbuck and Johnston, 2004; Megreya & Burton, 2008; Ritchie et al., 2015). While matching tasks have been generally used to approximate the process of checking photo-ID, the effect of familiarity has also been documented outside the lab with findings of poor performance when matching a live unfamiliar person to a photograph (Davis & Valentine, 2009; Kemp, Towell, & Pike, 1997; Megreya & Burton, 2008; Ritchie, Mireku, & Kramer, 2020). Moreover, all of this is true for many people who are employed to check photo-ID such as checkout assistants (Kemp et al., 1997), passport officers (White, Kemp, Jenkins, Matheson, & Burton, 2014) and police officers (Burton et al., 1999).

The difference between recognising familiar and unfamiliar faces has been attributed to the types of processing involved. We have seen the faces of familiar identities in a variety of contexts, situations and conditions, providing us with rich information about the many ways a single person might look. This way, we are able to isolate everything that is diagnostic of the person and discard any superficial image differences, leading to a more abstracted and image-independent processing for familiar faces. In Bruce and Young's influential model (Bruce and Young, 1986), familiar recognition is conceptualised through the use of Face Recognition Units (FRUs) which code structural information about known faces. FRUs must therefore store an abstracted, stable representation of a familiar person that is not influenced by simple image properties such as changes in head angle or expression.

Bruce (1994) first introduced the notion of stability from variation as a key familiarisation mechanism. Since then, a number of behavioural and computer modelling studies have shown that we can create and store stable representations of faces through exposure to within-person variability – that is multiple exposures to the same person showing naturally-occurring changes in their appearance. However, the same natural within-person variability that aids the recognition of familiar faces, can be detrimental to unfamiliar recognition which relies to a much greater extent on superficial image properties. This means that irrelevant differences in the physical properties of images or simple changes in clothing or accessories can be mistakenly regarded as evidence for differences in identity (Bindemann & Sandford, 2011; Graham & Ritchie, 2019; Kramer & Ritchie, 2016). In fact, recent research has suggested that the difference between familiar and unfamiliar face recognition may be due to our ability to use or tolerate within-person variability for familiar people (Burton, 2013; Burton, Jenkins, & Schweinberger, 2011; Burton, Kramer, Ritchie, & Jenkins, 2016; Jenkins et al., 2011). It is therefore possible that exposure to this variability can help unfamiliar viewers to switch from image-based to a more abstracted processing by aggregating the variability information into a single identity representation.

A growing body of research has shown that exposure to within-person variability helps when learning a new identity and this has been supported by work using both behavioural and computer modelling data (Dowsett, Sandford, & Burton, 2016; Jones, Dwyer, & Lewis, 2017; Kramer, Young, & Burton, 2018; Longmore et al., 2017; Longmore, Liu, & Young, 2008; Matthews, Davis, & Mondloch, 2018; Murphy, Ipser, Gaigg, & Cook, 2015; Ritchie & Burton, 2017; Robins, Susilo, Ritchie, & Devue, 2018). The benefits from access to multiple images of the same identity have been shown in adults' as well as in children's face learning (Matthews et al., 2018), with some evidence that children aged 6–13 need more variability than adults to learn a new person from video footage (Baker, Laurence, & Mondloch, 2017).

The amount of within-person variability is also an important factor in face learning. Ritchie and Burton (2017), for example, showed participants photos that were either high in variability (displaying changes in head angle, lighting, camera, age, hair style, etc) from a Google Images search, or photos that were low in variability, taken from a video of a single event (changes only in head angle and expression). After learning the identities from these images, participants' performance was tested with a name-verification and a face matching task using novel images of the same identities. In both cases, participants who had learned from the high variability image set outperformed those who learned from the low variability set. These results suggest that exposure to variability is key to learning or abstracting a stable representation of a person.

Research on the benefits of within-person variability for face matching has been less consistent and conclusive. Unlike face learning, this is a purely perceptual task with no demands on memory. Some studies suggest that multiple images may help to improve performance on face matching. White, Burton, Jenkins, and Kemp (2014) presented participants with arrays of two, three, or four images of the same person and asked them to match another image to the array. The multiple-image arrays gave rise to better performance than matching to a single image. In a different paradigm, participants were presented with a physical photograph of a target and asked to search through a pile of photos to find another image of the same person. On successive trials, participants were given an additional image of the same identity and their accuracy improved as the number of target images increased (Dowsett et al., 2016). Other recent studies, however, have failed the replicate these results with no benefits reported from exposure to arrays comprising a frontal and a profile view image (Kramer & Reynolds, 2018) or when matching a live person to a four-image array compared to a single image (Ritchie et al., 2020).

Therefore, when it comes to the key role of within-person variability for successful recognition, face learning and face matching tasks present somewhat dissimilar results. Exposure to variability helps learning a new identity, whereas results with matching are unclear. One possible explanation for this difference is that learning paradigms require the face to be memorised whereas matching paradigms present all stimuli simultaneously, without a memory component to the task. It is thus possible that exposure to variability, or multiple images, is only helpful when the task requires that a representation of the face be abstracted in order to be held in memory to make subsequent comparisons. This is supported by evidence for the benefits of within-person variability in face matching when images are presented one after the other, rather than simultaneously (Menon, White, & Kemp, 2015b).

Here, we compare face recognition accuracy in a purely perceptual simultaneous matching task and a memory-dependent sequential matching task. In a series of four studies, we manipulate the amount of within-person variability available, and the presentation order of multiple image arrays and comparison images, allowing us to determine why variability seems to be consistently aiding face learning but not face matching performance. It is possible that differences in results between previous studies are due to a difference in the amount of within-person variability shown in the arrays, with studies that have found a multiple-image benefit (e.g. White, Burton, et al., 2014) perhaps displaying more variability in the arrays than those that have not found that effect (e.g. Ritchie et al., 2020). However, if the difference in the utility of variability between face learning and matching is due to the memory component of learning tasks, then we would expect variability to facilitate performance in only sequential matching tasks. Like learning tasks, sequential matching tasks may require variability to be incorporated into a stable identity representation.

Experiment 1 investigates the effect of array variability on face matching performance in a simultaneous task. Experiment 2 compares performance in simultaneous versus sequential matching tasks. Finally, Experiments 3 and 4 compare performance on two different sequential tasks – one that allows for variability to be integrated into a single mental representation and one that does not.

Section snippets

Experiment 1 – array variability

The evidence to date is mixed as to whether multiple images improve matching performance (Menon, White, & Kemp, 2015a; Menon et al., 2015b; Ritchie et al., 2020; Sandford & Ritchie, 2021; White, Burton, et al., 2014), and so it could be that these experiments used arrays of differing degrees of variability, resulting in different effects. In this first experiment, we investigated the effect of array variability on face matching performance. We constructed high and low variability arrays from an

Experiment 2 – simultaneous vs sequential matching

This experiment investigated the effect of four-image arrays in simultaneous and sequential matching. The simultaneous and sequential tasks have different task demands, being purely perceptual- and memory-based respectively. This allows us to investigate the effect of variability on these two different processes. If memory is important for the multiple-image advantage, then we should see that four-image arrays produce higher matching accuracy only in a sequential and not a simultaneous matching

Experiment 3 – sequential presentation varying the order of array and comparison image

We hypothesise that the variability advantage found above in a sequential matching task relies on the task having a memory component. This is also the case for the variability advantage found elsewhere in the face learning literature, as learning tasks require memory. If this is the case, then we should see this advantage only when we present the array first in a sequential matching task as this will require participants to abstract a unified identity representation from the variability that is

Experiment 4 – applying the array order manipulation to a new task

This experiment further examined the effect of the presentation order of the array and the comparison image. Here we used an adaptation of the sequential matching paradigm used in Dowsett et al. (2016). This allowed us to investigate whether the variability advantage is still found in a different face matching paradigm which includes a memory component.

General discussion

Across the four experiments presented here, we see a clear pattern of results whereby multiple-image arrays lead to improved face matching performance in sequential matching tasks. This effect is only present when the array is presented before and not after the target image. We do not find the multiple image advantage for simultaneous face matching tasks. These results reconcile the differences between the face learning literature which shows that exposure to within-person variability and

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Andrew Dowsett for contributing to the work presented here, Amy S. Hought for data collection for Experiment 2, Ellen Wheeler for data collection for Experiment 3, and Lily Bridgewater for data collection for Experiment 4.

References (54)

  • V. Bruce

    Influences of familiarity on the processing of faces

    Perception

    (1986)
  • V. Bruce

    Stability from variation: The case of face recognition

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1994)
  • V. Bruce et al.

    Verification of face identities from images captured on video

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied

    (1999)
  • V. Bruce et al.

    Matching identities of familiar and unfamiliar faces caught on CCTV images

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied

    (2001)
  • V. Bruce et al.

    Understanding face recognition

    British Journal of Psychology

    (1986)
  • A.M. Burton

    Why has research in face recognition progressed so slowly? The importance of variability

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2013)
  • A.M. Burton et al.

    Mental representations of familiar faces

    British Journal of Psychology

    (2011)
  • A.M. Burton et al.

    Identity from variation: Representations of faces derived from multiple instances

    Cognitive Science

    (2016)
  • A.M. Burton et al.

    The Glasgow face matching test

    Behavior Research Methods

    (2010)
  • A.M. Burton et al.

    Face recognition in poor-quality video: Evidence from security surveillance

    Psychological Science

    (1999)
  • R. Clutterbuck et al.

    Exploring levels of face familiarity by using an indirect face-matching measure

    Perception

    (2002)
  • R. Clutterbuck et al.

    Matching as an index of face familiarity

    Visual Cognition

    (2004)
  • D. Cousineau

    Confidence intervals in within-subjects designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson’s method

    Tutorial in Quantitative Methods for Psychology

    (2005)
  • J.P. Davis et al.

    CCTV on trial: Matching video images with the defendant in the dock

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2009)
  • A.J. Dowsett et al.

    Face learning with multiple images leads to fast acquisition of familiarity for specific individuals

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2016)
  • J.D. Dunn et al.

    Search templates that incorporate within-face variation improve visual search for faces

    Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications

    (2018)
  • J. de Fockert et al.

    Rapid extraction of mean identity from sets of faces

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2009)
  • Cited by (21)

    • Stable individual differences in unfamiliar face identification: Evidence from simultaneous and sequential matching tasks

      2023, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      This discrepancy could be because recognition memory paradigms have a longer delay between targets and recall than in the current study. Whereas tasks with memory demands require perceivers to build a representation in memory, perceptual matching tasks do not (Ritchie et al., 2021). Our findings suggest that the ability to identify faces when stimuli are presented simultaneously, a protocol that does not require building a mental representation, predicts the ability to identify faces when short-term memory demands are introduced (i.e., in sequential tasks).

    • Perceptual similarity modulates effects of learning from variability on face recognition

      2022, Vision Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Manipulation of perceptual similarity between the images and the person seen in real life, may modify this effect. Another reason that was recently proposed for this discrepancy between studies that examined effects of learning from variability is that effects of variability are only found when a memory component is involved in the task (Ritchie et al., 2021). This was based on reported findings that when images are presented simultaneously, high variability does not improve face identification.

    • Children's ability to recognize their parent's face improves with age

      2022, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
    • Learning faces from variability: Four- and five-year-olds differ from older children and adults

      2022, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the context of Bruce and Young's (1986) model, 4- and 5-year-olds may have difficulty in building FRUs that facilitate recognition of familiar faces. Although our task did not require children to build a representation of identity (see Ritchie et al., 2021), it is possible that benefiting from exposure to multiple images in a perceptual task is a prerequisite skill for building a representation in memory. Children might need to be able to match identity across instances before they are able to integrate those instances into a robust representation in memory.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text