Skip to main content
Log in

Embodied technology and the dangers of using the phone while driving

  • Published:
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Contemporary scientific research and public policy are not in agreement over what should be done to address the dangers that result from the drop in driving performance that occurs as a driver talks on a cellular phone. One response to this threat to traffic safety has been the banning in a number of countries and some states in the USA of handheld cell phone use while driving. However, research shows that the use of hands-free phones (such as headsets and dashboard-mounted speakers) also accompanies a drop, leading some to recommend regulation of both kinds of mobile phones. In what follows, I draw out the accounts of the driving impairment associated with phone use implicit in research and policy and develop an alternative account grounded in philosophical considerations. Building on work in a school of thought called postphenomenology, I review and expand concepts useful for articulating human bodily and perceptual relations to technology. By applying these ideas to the case of driving while talking on the phone, I offer an account of the drop in driving performance which focuses on the embodied relationships users develop with the car and the phone, and I consider implications for research and policy.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. See www.distraction.gov/state-laws.

  2. For example, despite remaining neutral on the issue of talking on the phone while driving, the official position of The International Association of for the Wireless Telecommunications Industry (CITA) is in favor of the regulation of texting while driving. See www.ctia.org/advocacy/policy_topics/topic.cfm/TID/17.

  3. Information on the regulation of cell phone use while driving, both in terms of countrywide laws and statewide laws in the USA, can be found at www.cellular-news/car_bans.

  4. For introductions to postphenomenology see, e.g., Verbeek (2005); Ihde (2009); Rosenberger 2009; and the 2008, 31(1) special issue of the journal Human Studies.

  5. Merleau-Ponty puts the approach of phenomenology this way: “Not because we reject the certainties of common sense and a natural attitude to things—they are, on the contrary, the constant theme of philosophy—but because, being the presupposed basis of any thought, they are taken for granted, and go unnoticed, and because in order to arouse them and bring them into view, we have to suspend for a moment our recognition of them” (1962, xiii). By suspending judgments, and by focusing on the description of experience, unnoticed assumptions at play in established discussions may be revealed.

  6. Ihde’s account of the embodiment and transparency of technology is explicitly indebted to Martin Heidegger’s account of tool use and his notions of “present-to-hand” and “ready-to-hand,” developed in his work Being and Time (1953). I do not, however, wish to engage Heidegger’s work here since I do not want the concepts developed later to become embroiled in the larger ontological framework within which Heidegger’s notions of ready-to-hand and present-to-hand are inextricable centerpieces.

  7. The general idea that one’s field of awareness is organized by that which is significant is a theme explored throughout phenomenology (with a particularly helpful and sustained examination by Gurwitch (1964)). The term “field composition” is specific to my own work as I attempt to analyze this phenomenon in terms of its particular manifestation in the relationships between users, technologies, and the world. The term “sedimentation,” in contrast, is used throughout phenomenological thought, with a meaning generally similar to its deployment here, though of course with subtle differences between thinkers. In general, the term points to the ever-presence of past experiences actively informing the immediate experience of present encounters. It is with work of Merleau-Ponty (1962) that sedimentation is saddled as well with the connotation of bodily habituation, a connotation I hope to explicitly retain. Here I use the term sedimentation in my own technical way: to refer to the particular level of habit, the particular degree to which the past provides meaning to the present, in a given human–technological relation.

  8. I first developed a short version of this account of phone use in (Rosenberger 2010).

  9. Ihde (1990, 74) adds the example of parallel parking, “when well embodied, one feels rather than sees the distance between car and curb—one’s body is ‘extended’ to the parameters of the driver–car ‘body’”.

  10. Another example of the strength of the habits associated with driving is the experience of a driver who sits in the passenger seat and finds her or himself compelled to press upon the floor as if operating pedals (especially when uncomfortable with the driving style of the person behind the wheel).

  11. Evidence for the claim that this body of scientific research is guided by a general theoretical framework centered on notions of the limitations of the human brain—couched in terms of concepts such as workload, information processing, attention capacity, and cognitive resources—can be found in the following selection of quotes:

    “Extreme levels of workload may also mean that processing resources for new information are limited” (Alm and Nillson 1995, 714).

    “It would seem that participants were finding task performance cognitively more effortful in the during call period, and were having to invest greater attentional resources in task performance” (Haigney et al. 2000, 119).

    “The purpose of this study was, therefore, to explore the ability of drivers to cope with different levels of cognitive or mental workload that were introduced as secondary tasks and their effect on attention resource allocation” (Patten et al. 2004, 342).

    “In essence, drivers appear to be attempting to free-up resources for the secondary task by simplifying the primary task” (Jamson and Merat 2005, 93).

    “This inferior performance demonstrates that, although participants can switch attention between tasks, there appears to be an upper limit to available cognitive processing within the context of maintaining vehicle control” (Liu and Lee 2005, 379).

    “Research has demonstrated that the adverse effects of driving while talking are most likely not related to the motor control issues of manipulating a hand-held phone … or driving experience …. Rather, it is believed that the effects are a result of competition for limited cognitive resources” (Beede and Kass 2006, 416).

    “Whether the explanation lies at the sensory level … or at the attention resources management level … the results are in line with the theory of inherent limited capacity of human attention …, which predicts that the attentional resources allocated to one task (talking) come at the expense of the other (driving)” (Rosenbloom 2006).

    “We interpret this diversion of attention as reflecting a capacity limit on the amount of attention or resources that can be distributed across the two tasks. This capacity limit might be thought of as a biological constraint that limits the amount of systematic neural activity that can be distributed across parts of the cortex. The specific biological substrate that imposes the capacity limitation is not currently known” (Just et al. 2008, 76).

    “If the human brain were not limited in attending to multiple tasks at the same time, driver distraction would not be an issue” (Regan et al. 2009, 3).

References

  • Alm, H., & Nillson, L. (1994). Changes in driver behaviour as a function of handsfree phones—a simulator study. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 26(4), 441–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, H., & Nillson, L. (1995). The effects of a mobile telephone task on driver behaviour in a car following situation. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 27(5), 707–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beede, K. E., & Kass, S. J. (2006). Engrossed in conversation: the impact of cell phones on simulated driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 38, 415–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brookhuis, K. A., de Vries, G., & de Waard, D. (1991). The effects of mobile telephoning on driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 27, 707–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caird, J. K., Willness, C. R., Steel, P., & Scialfa, C. (2008). A meta-analysis of the effects of cell phones on driver performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 40, 1282–1293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Consiglio, W., Driscoll, P., Witte, M., & Berg, W. P. (2003). Effect of cellular telephone conversations and other potential interference on reaction time in breaking response. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35, 495–500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. M., & Strayer, D. L. (2008). Effects of simulator practice and real-world experience on cell-phone-related driver distraction. Human Factors, 50(6), 893–902.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charlton, S. G. (2009). Driving while conversing: cell phones that distract and passengers who react. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 41, 160–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crundall, D., Bains, M., Chapman, P., & Underwood, G. (2005). Regulating conversation during driving: a problem for mobile phones? Transportation Research Part F: Psychology and Behaviour., 8, 197–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drews, F. A., Pasupathi, M., & Strayer, D. L. (2008). Passenger and cell-phone conversations in simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Applied, 14(4), 392–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drews, F. A., & Strayer, D. L. (2009). Cell phones and driver distraction. In M. A. Regan, J. D. Lee, & K. L. Young (Eds.), Driver distraction: theory, effects, and mitigation (pp. 169–190). CRC: Boca Raton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gurwitch, A. (1964). The field of consciousness. Pittsburgh: Duquesne University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haigney, D. E., Taylor, R. G., & Westerman, S. J. (2000). Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone use and driving performance: task demand characteristics and compensatory processes. Transportation Research Part F., 3, 113–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hancock, P. A., Lesch, M., & Simmons, L. (2003). The distraction effects of phone use during a crucial driving maneuver. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35, 501–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1953). Being and time, trans. J. Stambaugh, 1996. Albany: SUNY.

  • Horberry, T., Anderson, J., Regan, M. A., Triggs, T. J., & Brown, J. (2006). Driver distraction: the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driver performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 38, 185–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horrey, H. J., & Wickens, C. D. (2004). Driving and side task performance: the effects of display clutter, separation, and modality. Human Factors, 46(4), 611–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hunton, J., & Rose, J. M. (2005). Cellular telephones and driving performance: the effects of attentional demands on motor vehicle risk. Risk Analysis, 24(5), 855–866.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (1990). Technology and the lifeworld. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (2007). Listening and voice: phenomenologies of sound (2nd ed.). Albany: SUNY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ihde, D. (2009). Postphenomenology and technoscience: the Peking University Lectures. Albany: SUNY.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishigami, Y., & Klein, R. M. (2009). Is hands-free phone safer than handheld phone? Journal of Safety Research, 40, 157–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jamson, A. H., & Merat, N. (2005). Surrogate in-vehicle information systems and driver behaviour: effects of visual and cognitive load in simulated rural driving. Transportation Research Part F., 8, 79–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Timothy, A. K., & Cynkar, J. (2008). A decrease in brain activation associated with driving and listening to someone speak. Brain Research, 1205, 70–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kunar, M. A., Carter, R., Cohen, M., & Horowitz, T. D. (2008). Telephone conversation impairs sustained visual attention via a central bottleneck. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(6), 1135–1140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Laberge-Nadeau, C., Maag, U., Bellavance, F., Lapierre, S. D., Desjardins, D., Messier, S., et al. (2003). Wireless telephones and the risk of road crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 35, 449–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamble, D., Kauranen, T., Laakso, M., & Summala, H. (1999). Cognitive load and detection thresholds in car following situations: safety implications for using mobile (cellular) telephones while driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 31, 617–623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, J., Pashler, H., & Boer, E. (2006). Central interference in driving: is there any stopping the psychological refractory period? Psychological Sciences., 17, 228–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, B.-S., & Lee, Y.-H. (2005). Effects of car-phone use and aggressive disposition during critical driving maneuvers. Transportation Research Part F., 8, 369–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mazzae, E. N., M. Goodman, W. R. Garrott, & T. A. Ranney. (2005). NHTSA’s research program on wireless phone driver interface effects. In: Proceedings of the 19th international technical conference on the enhanced safety of vehicles, paper no. 05–0375. Washington, DC: Highway Traffic Safety Administration.

  • McCartt, A. T., Hellinga, L. A., & Bratiman, K. A. (2006). Cell phones and driving: review of research. Traffic Injury Prevention, 7, 89–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McEvoy, S. P., MR, Stevenson, McCartt, A. T., Woodward, M., Haworth, C., Palamara, P., et al. (2005). Role of mobile phones in motor vehicle crashes resulting in hospital attendance: a case-crossover study. BMJ. doi:10.1136/bmj.38537.397512.55. published 12 July 2005.

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). The phenomenology of perception, trans. C. Smith. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parkes, A. M. (1991). Drivers’ business decision making ability whilst using carphones. In E. Lovessey (Ed.), Contemporary ergonomics: proceedings of the 14th international technical conference on the enhanced safety of vehicles (pp. 427–432). London: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patten, C. J. D., Kircher, A., Östlund, J., & Nilsson, L. (2004). Using mobile telephones: cognitive workload and attention resource allocation. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 36, 341–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Redelmeier, D. J., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1997). Association between cellular-telephone calls and motor vehicle collisions. The New England Journal of Medicine, 336(7), 453–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regan, M. A., Young, K. L., & Lee, J. D. (2009). Introduction. In M. A. Regan, J. D. Lee, & K. L. Young (Eds.), Driver distraction: theory, effects, and mitigation (pp. 3–7). CRC: Boca Raton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenberger, R. (2009). Quick-freezing philosophy: an analysis of imaging technologies in neurobiology. In: J.-K. B. Olsen, E. Selinger, and S. Riis (Eds.), New waves in philosophy of technology. New York: Palgrave Macmillan, pp. 65–82.

  • Rosenberger, R. (2010). The spatial experience of telephone use. Environment, Space, Place. 2(2): 63–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbloom, T. (2006). Driving performance while using cell phones: an observational study. Journal of Safety Research, 37, 207–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sagberg, F. (2001). Accident risk of car drivers during mobile telephone use. International Journal of Vehicle Design, 26(1), 57–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shinar, D., Tractinsky, N., & Compton, R. (2005). Effects of practice, age, and task demands, on interference from a phone task while driving. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, 315–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychological Science, 12(6), 462–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, D. L., Drews, F. A., & Crouch, D. J. (2006). A comparison of the cell phone driver and the drunk driver. Human Factors, 48(2), 381–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone-induced driver distraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(3), 128–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stutts, J. C., J. Feaganes, E. Rodgman, C. Hamlett, D. Reinfurt, K. Gish, M. Mercadante, & L. Staplin. (2003). The causes and consequences of distraction in everyday driving. In: Proceedings of the 47th annual conference of the Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine, pp. 235–252. Barrington: Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine.

  • Törnros, J. E. B., & Bolling, A. K. (2005). Mobile phone use—effects of handheld and handsfree phones on driving performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, 902–909.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Törnros, J., & Bolling, A. (2006). Mobile phone use—effects of conversation on mental workload and driving speed in rural and urban environments. Transportation Research Part F., 9, 298–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treffner, P. J., & Barrett, R. (2004). “Hands-free mobile phone speech while driving degrades coordination and control. Transportation Research Part F., 7, 229–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verbeek, P.-P. (2005). What things do: philosophical reflections on technology, agency, and design. State College: Penn State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Viotani, J. M., & Marshall, J. R. (1996). Cellular phones and traffic accidents: an epidemiological approach. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 28, 265–270.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virginia Tech Transportation Institute. (2005). 100-Car naturalistic driving study tracks drivers for a year. Press Release, June 10th. www.ctr.vt.edu

  • Young, K. L., & Regan, M. (2007). Driver distraction: a review of the literature. In I. J. Faulks, M. Regan, M. Stevenson, J. Brown, A. Porter, & J. D. Irwin (Eds.), Distracted driving (pp. 379–405). Sydney: Australisian College of Road Safety.

Download references

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to Michael Hoffmann, Sabrina Hom, Nancy Nersessian, Bryan Norton, and Victor Wanningen for comments on earlier drafts of this piece.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Robert Rosenberger.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Rosenberger, R. Embodied technology and the dangers of using the phone while driving. Phenom Cogn Sci 11, 79–94 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9230-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-011-9230-2

Keywords

Navigation