Skip to main content
Log in

How Our Ancestors Raised Children to Think as Modern Humans

  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article argues that social selection pressures in recent human evolution were primarily responsible for the emergence of modern cognition. These selection pressures took three specific forms: (1) Increased security and stability, which reduced allostatic load on developing children, facilitating expanded working memory development; (2) increased opportunities for mother-infant joint engagement, which created positive selection for more sophisticated forms of cognition; and (3) increased pressure on ritualized behavior associated with both mother-infant joint engagement and the construction and maintenance of an unprecedentedly complex adult social world. These pressures directly affected the development of the behavioral trait of effortful control in children. Effortful control is closely linked with executive functions, including working memory. Effortful control also shows characteristics that make it a prime candidate for rapidly spreading, culturally driven changes that were prevalent late in human evolution.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adler DS, Bar-Oz G, Belfer-Cohen A, Bar-Yosef O (2006) Ahead of the game: Middle and Upper Paleolithic hunting behaviors in the Southern Caucasus. Current Anthropology 47: 89–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alcorta CS (2006) Religion and the life course: Is adolescence an “experience expectant” period for religious transmission? In: Where God and Science Meet, Vol. 2 (McNamara P, ed), 55–79. Bridgeport, CT: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allman J, Hakeem A, Watson K (2002) Two phylogenetic specializations in the brain. The Neuroscientist 8: 335–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ambrose SH (2002) Small things remembered: Origins of early microlithic industries in Subsaharan Africa. In: Thinking Small: Global Perspectives on Microlithic Technologies (Elston R, Kuhn S, eds), 9–29. Archaeological papers of the American Anthropological Association #12. Arlington, VA: American Anthropological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin D (1991) Infants’ contribution to the achievement of joint reference. Child Development 62: 875–890.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bar-Yosef O (2002) The upper paleolithic revolution. Annual Review of Anthropology 31: 363–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beauregard M, Levesque J, Bourgouin P (2001) Neurocorrelates of conscious self-regulation of emotion. Journal of Neuroscience 21(RC 165): 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell C (1997) Ritual: Perspectives and Dimensions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger TD, Trinkaus E (1995) Patterns of trauma among the Neandertals. Journal of Archaeological Science 22: 841–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow AE, MacLean K, Proctor J (2004) The role of joint attention in the development of infants’ play with objects. Developmental Science 7: 518–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blurton Jones N (2002) The lives of hunter-gatherer children: Effects of parental behavior and parental reproductive strategy. In: Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behavior (Pereira ME, Fairbanks LA, eds), 309–326. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bocherens H, Billiou D, Mariotti A, Toussaint M, Patou-Mathis M, Bonjean D, Otte M (2001) New isotopic evidence for dietary habits of Neandertals from Belgium. Journal of Human Evolution 40: 497–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bocherens H, Drucker DG, Billiou D, Patou-Mathis M, Vandermeersch B (2005) Isotopic evidence for diet and subsistence pattern of the Saint-Cesaire I Neanderthal: Review and use of multi-source mixing model. Journal of Human Evolution 49: 71–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick MM, Nystrom LE, Fissell K, Carter CS, Cohen JD (1999) Conflict monitoring versus selection-for-action in anterior cingulate cortex. Nature 402: 179–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner JS (1975) The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language 2: 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burger J, Kirchner M, Bramanti B, Haak W, Thomas MG (2007) Absence of the lactase-persistence-associated allele in early Neolithic Europeans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: 3736–3741.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casey BJ, Durston S, Fossella JA (2001) Evidence for a mechanistic model of cognitive control. Clinical Neuroscience Research 1: 267–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caspari R, Lee SL (2004) Older age becomes common late in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 101: 10895–10900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catlin G (1867) O-kee-pa: A Religious Ceremony and Other Customs of the Mandans. London: Trubner.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chagnon N (1968) Yanamamo: The Fierce People. New York: Holt, Rinehart.

    Google Scholar 

  • Churchill SE (1993) Weapon technology, prey size selection, and hunting methods in modern hunter-gatherers: Implications for hunting in the Palaeolithic and Mesolithic. In: Hunting and Animal Exploitation in the Later Palaeolithic and Mesolithic of Eurasia (Peterkin GL, Bricker HM, Mellars PA, eds), 11–24. Archeological Papers of the American Anthropological Association #4. Arlington, VA: American Anthropological Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2001) Executive functions of the frontal lobes and the evolutionary ascendancy of Homo sapiens. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 11: 255–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2005) Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 15: 5–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2009) The Rise of Homo Sapiens: The Evolution of Modern Thinking. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham WA, Johnson MK, Raye CL, Gatenby JC, Gore JC, Banaji M (2004) Separable neural components in the processing of Black and White faces. Psychological Science 15: 806–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis CE, D’Esposito M (2003) Persistent activity in the prefrontal cortex during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Science 7: 415–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Lawson G, Vanhaeren M, Tillier A-M, Soressi M, Bresson F, Maureille B, Nowell A, Lakarra J, Backwell L, Julien M (2003) Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music: An alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory 17: 1–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Esposito G, Kirkby BS, van Horn JD, Ellmore TM, Berman KF (1999) Context-dependent, neural system-specific neurophysiological concomitants of ageing: Mapping PET correlates during cognitive activation. Brain 122: 963–979.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickson BD (1990) The Dawn of Belief. Tucson, AZ: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ding YC, Chi HC, Grady DL, Morishima A, Kidd JR, Kidd KK, et al. (2002) Evidence of positive selection acting at the human dopamine receptor D4 gene locus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 99: 309–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Defleur A, Dutour O, Valladas H, Vandermeersch V (1993) Cannibals among the Neanderthals. Nature 362: 214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Defleur A, White T, Valensi P, Slimak L, Cregut-Bonnoure E (1999) Neanderthal cannibalism at Moula-Guercy, Ardeche, France. Science 286: 128–131.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Waal FBM (1990) Peacemaking among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan J, Seitz RJ, Kolodny J, Bor D, Herzog H, Ahmed, A, Newell FN, Emslie H (2000) A neural basis for general intelligence. Science 289: 457–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eiden RD, Leonard KE, Morrisey S (2001) Paternal alcoholism and toddler noncompliance. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research 25: 1621–1633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg N, Cumberland D, Spinrad TL, Fabes RA, Shepard SA, Reiser M, et al. (2001) The relations of regulation and emotionality to children’s externalizing and internalizing problem behavior. Child Development 72: 1112–1134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans GW, Schamberg MA (2009) Childhood poverty, chronic stress, and adult working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 106: 6545–6549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans PD, Gilbert SL, Mekel-Bobrov N, Vallender EJ, Anderson JR, Vaez-Azizi LM, Tishkoff SA, Hudson RR, Lahn BT (2005) Microcephalin, a gene regulating brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans. Science 309: 1717–1720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan J, Wu Y, Fossella J, Posner MI (2001) Assessing the heritability of attentional networks. BioMed Central Neuroscience 2: 14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farah MJ, Shera DM, Savage JH, Betancourt L, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, Malmud EK, Hurt H (2006) Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research 110: 166–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feblot-Augustins J (1999) Raw material transport patterns and settlement systems in the European lower and middle Paleolithic: Continuity, change, and variability. In: The Middle Paleolithic Occupation of Europe (Roebroeks W, Gamble C, eds), 193–214. Leiden, Netherlands: University of Leiden Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari PF, Visalberghi E, Paukner A, Fogassi L, Ruggiero A, Suomi SJ (2006) Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLOS Biology 4: e302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field TM, Woodson R, Greenberg R, Cohen D (1982) Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science 218: 179–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy P, Target M (2002) Early intervention and the development of self-regulation. Psychoanalytic Quarterly 22: 307–335.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagne JR, Saudino KJ (2005) A behavioral genetic analysis of inhibitory control at 24 months of age. Behavior Genetics 35: 801.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gagne JR, Vendlinski MK, Goldsmith HH (2009) The genetics of childhood temperament. In: Handbook of Behavior Genetics (Kim Y-K, ed), 251–267. New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble C (1999) The Upper Paleolithic Societies of Europe. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glucklich A (2001) Sacred Pain. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith HH, Buss KA, Lemery KS (1997) Toddler and childhood temperament: Expanded content, stronger genetic evidence, new evidence for the importance of environment. Developmental Psychology 33: 891–905.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodall J (1986) The Chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grayson DK, Delpech F (2003) Ungulates and the middle-to-upper Paleolithic transition at Grotte, XVI (Dordogne, France). Journal of Archaeological Science 30: 1633–1648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guenther M (1999) From totemism to shamanism: Hunter-gatherer contributions to world mythology and spirituality. In: Cambridge Encyclopedia of Hunters and Gatherers (Lee RB, Daly R, eds), 426–433. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haviland JM, Lelwica M (1987) The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants’ responses to three emotional expressions. Developmental Psychology 23: 97–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawks J, Wang ET, Cochran GM, Harpending HC, Moyzis RK (2007) Recent acceleration of human adaptive evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA 104: 20753–20758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayden B (2003) Shamans, Sorcerers and Saints: A Prehistory of Religion. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henshilwood CF (2007) Fully symbolic sapiens behaviour: Innovation in the Middle Stone Age at Blombos Cave, South Africa. In: Rethinking the Human Revolution (Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C, eds), 123–132. Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hester R, Murphy K, Garavan H (2004) Beyond common resources: The cortical basis for resolving task interference. NeuroImage 23: 202–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson P (2004) The Cradle of Thought. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffecker JF (2002) Desolate Landscapes: Ice-Age Settlement in Europe. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ingvar DH (1994) The will of the brain: Cerebral correlates of willful acts. Journal of Theoretical Biology 171: 7–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kane MJ, Engle RW (2002) The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 9: 637–671.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karreman A, van Tuijl C, van Aken MAG, Dekovic M (2006) Parenting and self-regulation in preschoolers: A meta-analysis. Infant and Child Development 15: 561–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz R (1982) Boiling Energy: Community Healing Among the Kalahari !Kung. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller H, Scholmerich A, Eibl-Eibesfeldt I (1988) Communication patterns in adult-infant interactions in western and non-western cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 19: 427–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly AM, Hester R, Foxe JJ, Shpaner M, Garavan H (2006) Flexible cognitive control: Effects of individual differences and brief practice on a complex cognitive task. NeuroImage 31: 866–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein RG, Edgar B (2002) The Dawn of Human Culture. New York: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kogan N, Carter AS (1996) Mother-infant re-engagement following the still-face: The role of maternal availability in infant affect regulation. Infant Behavior and Development 19: 359–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kugiumutzakis G (1988) Neonatal imitation in the intersubjective companion space. In: Intersubjective Communication and Emotion in Early Ontogeny (Braten S, ed), 63–88. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn SL, Stiner MC (2006) What’s a mother to do? The division of labor among Neandertals and humans in Eurasia. Current Anthropology 42: 953–980.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn SL, Stiner MC (2007) Body ornamentation as information technology: Towards an understanding of the significance of early beads. In: Rethinking the Human Revolution (Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C, eds), 45–54. Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley MC, Clarkson C, Ulm S (2008) Behavioural complexity in Eurasian Neanderthal populations: A chronological examination of the archaeological evidence. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 18: 289–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lemery-Chalfant K, Doelger L, Goldsmith HH (2008) Genetic relations between effortful and attentional control and symptoms of pathology in middle childhood. Infant and Child Development 17: 365–427.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua, LJ (2003) Associations among emotionality, self-regulation, adjustment problems and positive adjustment in middle childhood. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 24: 595–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua LJ, Bush NR, Long AC, Kovacs EA, Trancik AM (2008) Effortful control as a moderator of the relation between contextual risk factors and growth in adjustment problems. Development and Psychopathology 20: 509–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lengua LJ, Honorado E, Bush N (2007) Cumulative risk and parenting as predictors of effortful control and social competence in preschool children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 28: 40–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis-Williams D (2002) The Mind in the Cave. London: Thames & Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman DE, Devlin M, Pearson OM (2001) Articular surface area responses to mechanical loading: Effects of exercise, age, and skeletal location. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 116: 266–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman DE, Pearson OM (2001) Trade-off between modeling and remodeling responses to loading in the mammalian limb. Bulletin of the Museum of Comparative Zoology 156: 269–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lutkehaus NC, Roscoe PB (1995) Gender Rituals: Female Initiation in Melanesia. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marean CW (2007) Heading north: An Africanist perspective on the replacement of Neanderthals by Modern humans. In: Rethinking the Human Revolution (Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C, eds), 367–379. Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marquet J-C, Lorblanchet M (2003) A Neanderthal face? The proto-figurine from La Roche-Cotard, Langeais (Indre-et-Loire, France). Antiquity 77: 661–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClenon J (2002) Wondrous Healing: Shamanism, Human Evolution and the Origin of Religion. DeKalb, IL: Northern Illinois University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDonald KB (2008) Effortful control, explicit processing and the regulation of human evolved predispositions. Psychological Review 115: 1012–1031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall I, Brown FH, Fleagle JG (2005) Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia. Nature 433: 733–736.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mekel-Bobrov N, Gilbert SL, Evans PD, Vallender EJ, Anderson JR, Hudson RR, et al. (2005) Ongoing adaptive evolution of ASPM, a brain size determinant in Homo sapiens. Science 309: 1720–1722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mellars P (1996) The Neanderthal Legacy. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff AN, Moore MK (1977) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198: 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mirsky J (1937) The Eskimo of Greenland. In: Cooperation and Competition Among Primitive Peoples (Mead M, ed), 51–86. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Moll H, Carpenter M, Tomasello M (2007) Fourteen-month olds know what others experience only in joint engagement with them. Developmental Science 10: 826–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myowa-Yamakoshi M, Tomonaga M, Tanaka M, Matsuzawa T (2004) Imitation in neonatal chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Developmental Science 7: 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy E, Molnar P (2004) Homoimitans or homo provocans? The phenomenon of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development 27: 57–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble KG, McCandiliss BD, Farah MJ (2007) Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science 10: 464–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollard KS, Salama SR, King B, Kern AD, Dreszer T, Katzman S, Siepel A, Pedersen JS, Bejerano G, Baertsch R, Rosenbloom KR, Kent J, Haussler D (2006) Forces shaping the fastest evolving regions in the human genome. PLoS Genetics 2(10): e168. DOI: 10.1371/journal.pgen.0020168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Fan J (2004) Attention as an organ system. In: Topics in Integrative Neuroscience: From Cells to Cognition (Pomerantz JR, Crair MC, eds), 31–61. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Power C (1998) Old wives’ tales: The gossip hypothesis and the reliability of cheap signals. In: Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases (Hurford JR, Studdert-Kennedy M, Knight C, eds), 111–129. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rappaport RA (1999) Ritual and Religion and the Making of Humanity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ratner HH, Foley MA, Gimpert M (2002) The role of collaborative planning in children’s source monitoring errors and learning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 81: 44–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy V (2008) How Infants Know Minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richeson JA, Baird AA, Gordon HL, Heatherton TF, Wyland CL, Trawalter S, Shelton JN (2003) An fMRI investigation of the impact of interracial contact on executive function. Nature Neuroscience 6: 133–1328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross HS, Lollis SP (1987) Communication within infant social games. Developmental Psychology 23: 241–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossano MJ (2007) Did meditating make us human? Cambridge Archaeological Journal 17: 47–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rossano MJ (2009) Ritual behavior and the origins of modern cognition. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 19: 243–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Evans DE (2000) Temperament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78: 122–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart MK, Ahadi SA, Hershey KL (1994) Temperament and social behavior in childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 40: 21–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothbart MK, Rueda MR (2005) The development of effortful control. In: Developing Individuality in the Human Brain: A Tribute to Michael I. Posner (Mayr U, Awh E, Keele S, eds), 167–188. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Shea J (1997) Middle Paleolithic spear point technology. In: Projectile Technology (Knecht H, ed), 79–106. New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Smuts BB, Watanabe JM (1990) Social relationships and ritualized greetings in adult male baboons (Papio cynocephalus anubis). International Journal of Primatology 11: 147–172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Soffer O (1994) Ancestral lifeways in Eurasia—the Middle and Upper Paleolithic records. In: Origins of Anatomically Modern Humans (Nitecki M, Nitecki D, eds), 101–109. New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sorensen MV, Leonard WR (2001) Neandertal energetics and foraging efficiency. Journal of Human Evolution 40: 483–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stiner, MC (1991) The faunal remains at Grotta Guattari: A taphonomic perspective. Current Anthropology 32: 103–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano T, Kopp F, Grossman T, Reid VM (2006) Eye contact influences neuralprocessing of emotional expressions in 4-month-old infants. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1: 87–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stringer C, Gamble C (1993) In Search of the Neanderthals. London: Thames & Hudson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1988) The role of joint attentional processes in early language development. Language Sciences 10: 69–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Farrar MJ (1986) Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57: 1454–1463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Haberl K (2003) Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month olds know what’s new for other persons. Developmental Psychology 39: 906–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M, Todd J (1983) Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language 4: 197–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Townsend JB (1999) Shamanism. In: Anthropology of Religion (Glazier SD, ed), 429–469. Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ (1994) Brain development, infant communication and empathy disorders: Intrinsic factors in child mental health. Development and Psychopathology 6: 599–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trinkaus E, Thompson DD (1987) Femoral diaphyseal histomorphometric determination for the Shanidar 3, 4, 5, and 6 Neandertals and Neandertal longevity. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 72: 123–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick EZ (2003) Things still to be done on the still-face effect. Infancy 4:475–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tronick E, Als H, Adamson L (1979) The structure of face-to-face communicative interactions. In: Before Speech (Bullowa M, ed), 349–370. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tronick E, Als H, Adamson L, Wise S, Brazelton TB (1978) The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 17: 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren M, D’Errico F (2005) Grave goods from the Saint-Germain-la-Riverere burial: Evidence for social inequality in the upper Paleolithic. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 24: 117–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vanhaeren M, D’Errico F, Stringer C, James SL, Todd JA, Mienis HK (2006) Middle Paleolithic shell beads in Israel and Algeria. Science 312: 1785–1788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Roosmalen MGM, Klein LL (1988) The spider monkeys, genus Ateles. In: Ecology and Behavior of Neotropic Primates (Mittermeier R, Rylands AB, Coimbra-Filho AF, da Fonesca GAB, eds), 445–537. Washington, DC: World Wildlife Fund.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vitebsky P (2000) Shamanism. In: Indigenous Religions (Harvey G, ed), 55–67. London: Cassell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Welch MC (2001) The prefrontal function and the development of the executive function in childhood. In: Handbook of Brain and Behavior in Human Development (Kalverboer AF, Gramsbergen A, eds), 767–790. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • White TD, Asfaw B, DeGusta D, Tilbert H, Richards GD, Suwa G, Howell FC (2003) Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423: 742–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehouse H (1996) Rites of terror: Emotion, metaphor and memory in Melanesian cults. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute 2: 703–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitham JC, Maestripieri D (2003) Primate rituals: The function of greetings between male guinea baboons. Ethology 109: 847–859.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkelman M (1990) Shamans and other “magico-religious” healers: A cross-cultural study of their origins, nature, and social transformation. Ethos 18:308–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winkelman M (2002) Shamanism and cognitive evolution. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 12: 71–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2003) The role of working memory in the evolution of managed foraging. Before Farming 2: 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2004) The expert Neanderthal mind. Journal of Human Evolution 46: 467–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2007) Did a small but significant enhancement in working memory capacity power the evolution of modern thinking? In: Rethinking the Human Revolution (Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C, eds), 79–90. Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynn T, Coolidge F, Bright M (2009) Hohlenstein-Stadel and the evolution of human conceptual thought. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 19: 73–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamagata S, Takahashi Y, Kijima Y, Mackawa H, Ono Y, Ando J (2005) Genetic and environmental etiology of effortful control. Twin Research and Human Genetics 8: 300–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matt J. Rossano.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rossano, M.J. How Our Ancestors Raised Children to Think as Modern Humans. Biol Theory 5, 142–153 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1162/BIOT_a_00033

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/BIOT_a_00033

Keywords

Navigation