Europe PMC

This website requires cookies, and the limited processing of your personal data in order to function. By using the site you are agreeing to this as outlined in our privacy notice and cookie policy.

Abstract 


Performance in a behavioral task can be facilitated by associating stimulus properties with reward. In contrast, conflicting information is known to impede task performance. Here we investigated how reward associations influence the within-trial processing of conflicting information using a color-naming Stroop task in which a subset of ink colors (task-relevant dimension) was associated with monetary incentives. We found that color-naming performance was enhanced on trials with potential-reward versus those without. Moreover, in potential-reward trials, typical conflict-induced performance decrements were attenuated if the incongruent word (task-irrelevant dimension) was unrelated to reward. In contrast, incongruent words that were semantically related to reward-predicting ink colors interfered with performance in potential-reward trials and even more so in no-reward trials, despite the semantic meaning being entirely task-irrelevant. These observations imply that the prospect of reward enhances the processing of task-relevant stimulus information, whereas incongruent reward-related information in a task-irrelevant dimension can impede task performance.

Free full text 


Logo of nihpaAbout Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;
Cognition. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 Dec 1.
Published in final edited form as:
PMCID: PMC2967668
NIHMSID: NIHMS234478
PMID: 20864094

The influence of reward associations on conflict processing in the Stroop task

Abstract

Performance in a behavioral task can be facilitated by associating stimulus properties with reward. In contrast, conflicting information is known to impede task performance. Here we investigated how reward associations influence the within-trial processing of conflicting information using a color-naming Stroop task in which a subset of ink colors (task-relevant dimension) was associated with monetary incentives. We found that color-naming performance was enhanced on trials with potential reward versus those without. Moreover, in potential-reward trials, typical conflict-induced performance decrements were attenuated if the incongruent word (task-irrelevant dimension) was unrelated to reward. In contrast, incongruent words that were semantically related to reward-predicting ink colors interfered with performance in potential-reward trials and even more so in no-reward trials, despite the semantic meaning being entirely task-irrelevant. These observations imply that the prospect of reward enhances the processing of task-relevant stimulus information, whereas incongruent reward-related information in a task-irrelevant dimension can impede task performance.

Keywords: Reward, Conflict, Stroop, Interference

1. Introduction

Reward is known to be an effective motivator of behavior and a driving force for learning (for a review see Wise, 2004). Numerous studies in humans have demonstrated that reward anticipation is associated with performance improvement in diverse behavioral domains, including response speed and accuracy (e.g., Bijleveld, Custers, & Aarts, 2010; Knutson, Adams, Fong, & Hommer, 2001), visual discrimination and visual search (e.g., Engelmann & Pessoa, 2007; Kristjansson, Sigurjonsdottir, & Driver, 2010), cognitive control (Locke & Braver, 2008), negative priming (e.g., Della Libera & Chelazzi, 2006), and memory processes (e.g., Adcock, Thangavel, Whitfield-Gabrieli, Knutson, & Gabrieli, 2006; Krebs, Schott, Schutze, & Duzel, 2009; Wittmann et al., 2005).

While reward generally exerts enhancing effects on behavior, the presence of conflicting information is known to disrupt performance, as commonly demonstrated by conflict paradigms such as the Stroop color-naming task (Stroop, 1935). In this task, subjects respond to the ink color of a color word (e.g., “RED”) while ignoring its semantic meaning. Typically, subjects’ performance is facilitated in trials in which the information in the task-relevant (ink color) and task-irrelevant (word meaning) dimensions are congruent, and impeded if they are incongruent (MacLeod, 1991). According to influential parallel distributed processing models of the Stroop effect, information from both input dimensions is conveyed in parallel, and the ultimate response depends on the relative activation of the two pathways (Carter & van Veen, 2007; Cohen, Dunbar, & McClelland, 1990). In the color-naming Stroop task, it has been proposed that automatic reading of the irrelevant word meaning strongly co-activates the corresponding pathway in parallel to the processing of the relevant ink color, and, if incongruent, interferes with performance.

More recently, observations that brain regions implicated in human cognitive control are also critically involved in reward-based learning (Miller & Cohen, 2001; Ridderinkhof, Ullsperger, Crone, & Nieuwenhuis, 2004; Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, & Carter, 2004; Ullsperger & von Cramon, 2003) have given rise to the question of how far processes related to reward and conflict interact (Holroyd & Coles, 2002; Jocham & Ullsperger, 2009). Supporting such an interaction, it has been demonstrated that reward information has the potential to disrupt the behavioral adjustments that are typically observed subsequent to incongruent trials in a flanker task (van Steenbergen, Band, & Hommel, 2009). According to this study, the commonly observed behavioral adjustments (for a review see Egner, 2007) might be counteracted by the receipt of reward, thereby suggesting a shared mechanism (van Steenbergen et al., 2009). However, these observations were limited to sequential effects, and reward was delivered incidentally (i.e., subjects’ responses were not instrumental to obtaining rewards) and thus it remains unknown how conflict processing would be modulated if reward is associated with components of the task itself.

We sought to investigate this question by associating reward with two of the four ink colors in a Stroop task. While subjects responded to the ink color, the irrelevant semantic meaning of the word could be congruent, incongruent, or neutral with regard to the ink color. In addition to these typical Stroop-paradigm categories, the irrelevant word could be semantically linked to a color that was either part of the potential-reward ink-color subset or not. However, the semantic information was entirely task-irrelevant and thus never associated with obtaining reward.

Based on the notion that cognitive control in concert with attention can differentially emphasize the pathways of potential competing inputs we hypothesized that reward associations in the relevant dimension would further promote effective stimulus processing. Specifically, we predicted general response facilitation and reduced interference in potential-reward as compared to no-reward trials. Additionally, we hypothesized that reward associations with an ink color would generalize to its semantic representation (i.e., word meaning). Consequently, incongruent word meanings that are implicitly linked to reward, might cause greater interference by emphasizing the incongruent information.

2. Experiment 1

2.1. Methods

2.1.1. Participants

Twenty healthy right-handed subjects participated (mean age ± SD: 22.5±3.2, 14 female) and gave written informed consent in accordance with the Duke Medical Center Institutional Review Board for human subjects. Subjects were paid a basic amount of 15plusanaveragerewardbonusof15.

2.1.2. Paradigm and Procedure

Subjects performed a version of the classic color-naming Stroop task in which they responded to the ink color of words while ignoring their semantic meaning. A small gray fixation square (visual angle 0.3°) was maintained in the center of a black screen (Fig. 1A). In each trial a colored capitalized word was presented above fixation for 600 ms, randomly chosen from the following set: “RED”, “YELLOW”, “BLUE”, “GREEN”, or “BROWN” (vertical 0.8°, horizontal ranging from 2.1° to 4.6°). The words were separated by a variable stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) of 1800 to 2200 ms and were written in one of four ink colors (red, yellow, blue, or green). Subjects were instructed to respond as quickly as possible by pressing the button associated with the current ink color (Color; task-relevant dimension) while ignoring the semantic meaning (Word; task-irrelevant dimension; Fig. 1B). Responses were given with the index and middle fingers of the left and right hands, with color-button assignments and color-reward associations counterbalanced across subjects. The semantic meaning of a given word could be congruent (e.g., “GREEN” written in green) or incongruent (e.g., “RED” written in green) with regard to the ink color. Furthermore, trials consisting of words with no conflicting response mapping (e.g., “BROWN” written in green) were intermixed to provide a neutral category.

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms234478f1.jpg

Stimuli and experimental conditions. (A) Subjects responded to the ink color (relevant dimension) of presented words (SOA: stimulus onset asynchrony). (B) Counterbalanced across subjects, a subset of ink colors was associated with the potential of reward (potential-reward; e.g., green and blue), while the remaining ink colors were not (no-reward; e.g., red and yellow). The word meaning (irrelevant dimension) could be congruent, incongruent reward-unrelated, incongruent reward-related, or neutral with regard to the ink color.

While responses to two of the four possible ink colors were associated with the potential for monetary reward (potential-reward), the remaining two colors represented standard Stroop trials (no-reward; Fig. 1B). Accordingly, a fast and correct response in potential-reward trials resulted in a 10-cent gain, while an incorrect or slow response resulted in a 10-cent penalty. In order to keep all subjects in a similar reward range, the response time-out was adjusted dynamically based on individual performance. This procedure led to an average gain of $2.50 per run for each subject (70:30 gain-to-loss ratio). Following a short training session, subjects completed six experimental six-minute runs, yielding a total of 480 potential-reward and 480 no-reward trials. During four 20-second breaks within each run, the updated dollar amount was displayed, serving as performance feedback.

The information conveyed by the irrelevant semantic meaning of the word resulted in equally distributed congruency conditions for both potential-reward and no-reward trials (Fig. 1B): congruent, incongruent reward-unrelated, incongruent reward-related, and neutral. It should be emphasized that, although the irrelevant incongruent word could be implicitly “related” to either the potential-reward or no-reward ink-color subset, the monetary incentives were exclusively dependent on the ink-color dimension. This manipulation allowed us to investigate the explicit effects of reward in the relevant dimension (potential-reward vs. no-reward), as well as indirect effects of reward associations that were entirely irrelevant to the task (incongruent reward-related vs. reward-unrelated).

The averaged response times (RT) and error rates were submitted to repeated-measures analyses of variance (rANOVAs) to verify the overall main effects of the relevant dimension (Color: potential-reward, no-reward) and the irrelevant dimension (Word: congruent, incongruent, neutral). In order to investigate differential effects of reward-related and reward-unrelated irrelevant information, additional 2by2-rANOVAs were conducted focusing on the two types of incongruent trials.

2.1.3. Results

Subjects responded faster in potential-reward as compared to no-reward trials (F(1,19)=78.28, p<.001; η2p=.805; Fig. 2A). In addition, in keeping with common findings in the Stroop task, RTs were significantly modulated by the presence of irrelevant semantic information, with fastest responses when the task-irrelevant word meaning was congruent, intermediate when it was neutral, and slowest when it was incongruent (F(1,18)=50.6, p<.001; η2p=.727; Table 1). When confining the rANOVA to incongruent trials (Fig. 2A; dotted box), we again observed generally shorter RTs in potential-reward versus no-reward trials (F(1,19)=65.96, p<.001; η2p=.776). In contrast, reward-related information in the irrelevant dimension resulted in the opposite pattern, with slower responses for incongruent words semantically related to potential-reward colors (reward-related>reward-unrelated; F(1,19)=13.08, p=.002; η2p=.408). No interaction effects were observed (p>.1).

An external file that holds a picture, illustration, etc.
Object name is nihms234478f2.jpg

RT results from experiment 1 (A) and experiment 2 (B). Differences between potential-reward (turquoise bars) and no-reward (orange bars) trials averaged across all word-meaning categories are displayed on the left. Effects of the irrelevant word-meaning dimension are displayed separately for the respective ink-color subsets on the right. RT values are depicted as the difference relative to neutral words. Error bars represent standard errors of the means (significance level ***p<.001; **p<.005; *p<.05).

Table 1

Effects of relevant and irrelevant reward associations on performance.

Color (relevant)Word (irrelevant)
congruentincongruentneutral
reward-unrelatedreward-related
Exp1: Rewarded Stroop
potential-rewardRT ms (SD)521 (71.7)544 (77.0)552 (79.9)540 (76.6)
Errors % (SD)5.7 (2.6)8.8 (5.1)10.5 (4.3)6.7 (3.4)
no-rewardRT ms (SD)607 (87.0)637 (89.7)650 (93.7)623 (80.7)
Errors % (SD)10.2 (5.1)19.7 (10.4)15.6 (8.0)12.5 (7.5)

Exp2: Rewarded Stroop
potential-rewardRT ms (SD)450 (35.5)466 (44.9)473 (41.9)458 (43.6)
Errors % (SD)8.6 (6.4)10.5 (6.7)14.1 (9.4)11.1 (8.3)
no-rewardRT ms (SD)542 (45.5)561 (46.9)572 (45.7)550 (45.3)
Errors % (SD)16 (8.4)22.8 (11.2)23.2 (10.1)17.9 (8.7)

Exp2: Extinction phase
former potential-rewardRT ms (SD)516 (60.4)535 (66.7)553 (65.7)529 (62.1)
Errors % (SD)11.4 (7.4)16.4 (10)17.2 (9.6)12.5 (6.6)
former no-rewardRT ms (SD)560 (71.1)577 (81.5)581 (67.7)565 (73.4)
Errors % (SD)17 (11.8)17.2 (10.1)20.8 (10.4)15.8 (9.9)

Exp1: Experiment 1; Exp2: Experiment2; RT: response time; SD: standard deviation

Considering no-reward trials alone, planned post-hoc t-tests comparing incongruent to neutral trials confirmed the typical incongruency Stroop effects, with significant differences between neutral trials and both incongruent trial types (neutral<reward-unrelated: t(19)=4.35, p<.001; neutral<reward-related: t(19)=3.13, p<.006). In potential-reward trials, conflicting information still led to significantly slower responses relative to neutral trials if the incongruent word was related to potential-reward colors (neutral<reward-related: t(19)=3.39, p=.003). However, this effect was absent for reward-unrelated words (neutral<reward-unrelated: t(19)=1.30, p=.211). The direct comparison between incongruent reward-related and reward-unrelated words confirmed the relative RT-slowing for the former within both potential-reward (t(19)=2.30, p=.033) and no-reward trials (t(19)=2.61, p=.017).

Participants also committed less errors in potential-reward compared to no-reward trials (F(1,19)=22.87, p<.001; η2p=.546). We furthermore found a main effect of the irrelevant word-meaning dimension (F(1,18)=21.9, p<.001; η2p==.535), with highest error rates for incongruent trials, intermediate for neutral, and least for congruent. Focusing on incongruent trials only, we observed lower error rates for potential-reward versus no-reward trials (F(1,19)=20.85, p<.001; η2p=.523), accompanied by a significant interaction with the word dimension (F(1,19)=8.44, p=.009; η2p=.308), reflecting greater interference from incongruent reward-unrelated words in no-reward trials. No main effect of the irrelevant dimension alone was observed (p<.2). Planned post-hoc t-tests revealed that in no-reward trials, error rates were significantly higher for both types of incongruent information compared to neutral trials (neutral<reward-related: t(19)=2.89, p=.009; neutral<reward-unrelated: t(19)=4.75, p<.001). In potential-reward trials, however, a robust increase in error rates was only observed for incongruent reward-related words relative to neutral ones (neutral<reward-related: t(19)=4.27, p<.001; neutral<reward-unrelated: t(19)=2.04, p=.056).

2.1.4. Discussion

In summary, the observed differential pattern in experiment 1 indicates a beneficial influence of reward associations in the relevant dimension, including a performance-enhancing suppression of incongruent irrelevant information when it was semantically unrelated to reward. At the same time, conflicting irrelevant information that was implicitly linked to reward led to substantially stronger interference as compared to incongruent information that was entirely unrelated to reward.

In order to verify the observed effects and to replicate the results in an independent subject sample, a second experiment was conducted. First, in order to ensure that the observed effects can be exclusively evoked by the prospect of reward rather than a mixture of “maximizing wins” and “minimizing losses,” we excluded the concept of punishment from experiment 2. While fast and accurate responses were still rewarded, subjects were no longer penalized for being slow or incorrect. Second, in order to reduce the potential confusion about the task-relevant and therefore reward-predictive stimulus dimension, we included a color-reward training phase preceding the rewarded Stroop task. In this training phase, subjects responded to colored rectangles in the absence of semantic information and received visual feedback to strengthen the reward associations. Third, following a rewarded Stroop phase similar to experiment 1, subjects performed an unrewarded Stroop task (termed “extinction phase”) in which they were instructed that none of the colors was associated with reward any longer. This phase was included in order to investigate to what extent the behavioral influences of established reward associations would persist after removing the prospect of reward.

3. Experiment 2

3.1. Methods

3.1.1. Participants

Sixteen healthy right-handed subjects (mean age ± SD: 22.6±3.2, 9 female) participated after giving written informed consent in accordance with the Duke Medical Center Institutional Review Board for human subjects. Subjects were paid a basic amount of 15plusanaveragerewardbonusof16.

3.1.2. Paradigm and Procedure

Experiment 2 consisted of three successive phases: a color-reward training phase, a rewarded Stroop phase, and an extinction phase in which rewards were no longer given. The color-button mappings, as well as the color-reward associations, remained constant throughout the three phases but were counterbalanced across subjects.

Color-reward training phase

Colored rectangles (red, yellow, blue, green; 0.8° by 2°) were presented right above fixation for 600 ms, and subjects were asked to indicate the current color as fast as possible by pressing one of four buttons. Similar to experiment 1, fast and accurate responses to two of the four colors were associated with obtaining incentives (potential-reward) while the remaining colors were not (no-reward). Additionally, responses were followed by visual feedback (500 ms) indicating if it was fast/accurate (“+10ct”) or slow/incorrect (“±0ct“). Thus, fast and correct responses were rewarded, while slow or incorrect responses did not affect the total gain. Subjects performed one six-minute run consisting of 56 trials (SOA = 1800 to 2200 ms) and the total gain was displayed in the end.

Rewarded Stroop phase

The task was identical to the one in experiment 1 with two exceptions: Subjects performed four instead of six six-minute runs, resulting in 320 potential-reward and 320 no-reward trials. Moreover, subjects were no longer penalized for incorrect or slow responses. A staircase procedure analogous to experiment 1 guaranteed a winning rate of 70%, which translated to an average win of $4 per run.

Extinction phase

Prior to performing two additional runs of the Stroop task, subjects were explicitly instructed that none of the colors would be associated with reward anymore. We refer to colors that were formerly associated with potential-reward and no-reward as former potential-reward and former no-reward, respectively. The stimuli and timing parameters were identical to the rewarded Stroop task, except for the interim feedback which was dropped.

3.2. Results

3.2.1. Color-reward training phase

Subjects responded significantly faster to potential-reward colors (mean±SD: potential-reward 467±57ms; no-reward 530±69; t(15)=4.3, p=.001) while maintaining similar error rates (mean±SD: potential-reward 17±13%; no-reward 20±16%; p>.5), indicating that subjects learned to associate specific colors with the prospect of reward.

3.2.2. Rewarded Stroop phase

Similar to experiment 1, responses were faster in potential-reward compared to no-reward trials (F(1,15)=51.29, p<.001; η2p=.774; Fig. 2B and Table 1). Also as in experiment 1, RTs were longest for incongruent trials, intermediate for neutral, and fastest for congruent ones (F(1,14)=16.76, p<.001; η2p=.528). Focusing on incongruent trials only, the observed main effects were also comparable to experiment 1 (potential-reward<no-reward: F(1,15)=48.73, p<.001; η2p=.765; reward-related>reward-unrelated: F(1,15)=7.55, p=.015; η2p=.335).

Planned post-hoc t-tests revealed significantly longer RTs for incongruent compared to neutral words in no-reward trials (neutral<reward-related: t(15)=4.1, p=.001; neutral<reward-unrelated: t(15)=2.3, p=.034; reward-related>reward-unrelated: t(15)=2.1, p=.045). In contrast, in potential-reward trials, only incongruent reward-related words led to a significant RT difference relative to neutral ones (neutral<reward-related: t(15)=3.3, p=.005; neutral<reward-unrelated: t(15)=1.9, p=.072; reward-related>reward-unrelated: t(15)=2.0, p=.065).

Error rates were significantly reduced in potential-reward compared to no-reward trials (F(1,15)=10.85, p=.005; η2p=.420) and again highest for incongruent, intermediate for neutral, and lowest for congruent words (F(1,14)=10.34, p<.001; η2p==.565). Within incongruent trial types, error rates were again lower in potential-reward as compared to no-reward trials (F(1,15)=10.94, p=.005; η2p=.422), but there was no significant difference between incongruent reward-related and reward-unrelated words (p>.1).

3.2.3. Extinction phase

Subjects still responded significantly faster to colors that formerly indicated potential reward (former potential-reward < no-reward: F(1,15)=14.75, p=.002; η2p=.496), despite the explicit removal of actual reward. However, the reward-driven effect on accuracy did no longer reach significance (F(1,15)=3.5, p>.08). In line with typical Stroop effects, responses were slower (F(1,14)=10.86, p=.001; η2p=.420) and less accurate (F(1,14)=7.63, p=.004; η2p=.337) on incongruent as compared to neutral and congruent trials.

Focusing on the incongruent trials, responses were still faster in trials consisting of former potential-reward colors (F(1,15)=12.1, p=.003; η2p=.447), but there were no significant differences in accuracy (p>.4). Furthermore, there were no significant differences between incongruent reward-related and reward-unrelated words in either RT or accuracy (all p>.2).

4. General Discussion

In the present study, using a version of the Stroop task with reward associations for a subset of the ink colors, we systematically manipulated reward anticipation in the task-relevant dimension (ink color), thereby also implicitly imparting reward associations in the task-irrelevant dimension (word meaning). In line with previous research demonstrating the performance-enhancing effect of reward (e.g., Bijleveld et al., 2010; Engelmann & Pessoa, 2007), we found that reward anticipation linked to the word’s ink color generally led to performance facilitation, as reflected by faster responses and lower error rates. In addition, although the typical conflict-induced slowing of responses was observed in all no-reward trials, this effect was partially suppressed in potential-reward trials. In contrast, reward associations in the irrelevant word-meaning dimension inflicted costs on performance if they were incongruent to the relevant ink color. More specifically, incongruent words semantically related to potential-reward colors interfered more strongly with performance than words semantically unrelated to reward, despite the word meaning always being entirely task-irrelevant.

The observation of response facilitation for the potential-reward ink colors is consistent with the notion that reward anticipation has an overall enhancing effect on task performance. In terms of parallel distributed processing models of conflict processing (MacLeod, 1991), reward in the relevant dimension seems to selectively enhance the processing of the currently relevant stimulus property, thereby reducing influence from interfering information and increasing the probability and speed of the correct response. In experiment 2, the stronger reduction of interference, as well as the reduction of facilitation in potential-reward trials, might be related to the additional practice in the color-naming task.

Interestingly, the general response facilitation observed in potential-reward trials persisted in the subsequent extinction phase, despite the explicit instruction that none of the colors was any longer predictive of reward. The latter observation further supports the idea that reward selectively increases the processing of behaviorally relevant stimuli. Once established, this preferential stimulus processing might persist in a relatively automatized fashion even if the original higher cognitive goal is no longer explicitly reinforced.

Beyond the performance-improving effects of relevant reward associations (ink color) in both experiments, we observed an opposite effect of reward when presented in the irrelevant dimension (word meaning), with slower responses to incongruent reward-related words. This pattern suggests that the strong reward association with the relevant stimulus dimension generalized to the irrelevant one, thereby inducing greater interference in incongruent trials (i.e., when the prepotent response to the highly salient word is incongruent with the required response). This reward-related increase in interference was very robust in no-reward trials in both experiments. However, in potential-reward trials, interference from incongruent reward-related words appeared to be slightly smaller in the second compared to the first experiment, possibly due to practice effects. Notably, the increased interference from reward-related words reported here relied on a newly learned stimulus-reward association rather than highly overlearned relationships or even automatized processes such as word reading itself. More generally, the observation of a performance decrement by reward information in the irrelevant dimension supports the notion that such associations might not always be beneficial for behavioral performance (Padmala & Pessoa, 2010; Pessoa, 2009).

With respect to the neural processes that shape these associations, the differential processing patterns might rely on dopaminergic pathways that are known to be involved in both reward (Schott et al., 2008; Schultz, 1997; Wise, 2004; Zellner & Ranaldi, 2009) and conflict processing (Holroyd & Coles, 2002; Jocham & Ullsperger, 2009). By enhancing the color-reward associations, dopamine may support cognitive control processes that facilitate the processing of the relevant stimulus property and thereby help reduce conflict-induced distraction (Locke & Braver, 2008; Montague, Hyman, & Cohen, 2004; Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg et al., 2004). At the same time, implicit reward associations with task-irrelevant stimulus properties - here, the word meaning - may induce an increase in salience of these properties. This increased salience may disrupt task performance by enhancing the incorrect stimulus-response mapping or by drawing some attentional resources away from the processing of the relevant dimension (Pessoa, 2009). Importantly, at least in the present study, the mechanism that brings about these reward-related modulations with regard to the irrelevant dimension must involve a relatively abstract color representation that is shared by processes extracting information about the word’s ink color and its semantic meaning, thereby leading to a transfer of reward association from the relevant to the irrelevant stimulus dimension.

In summary, the current observations support the idea that task-relevant reward acts as an overall enhancer of behavioral performance. Specifically, the anticipation of reward seems to promote effective stimulus processing, including a reduction of interference from conflicting information. However, these beneficial effects can come at a cost: If irrelevant reward-related information is incongruent with the relevant dimension, it appears to disrupt task performance, presumably by emphasizing an already prepotent but incorrect response to the highly salient word.

In this regard, the color-naming version of the Stroop task we used here appears to be especially well suited to provoke such robust interference effects due to word reading being a highly trained skill as compared to color-naming. In contrast, in the reverse Stroop task, which requires responses to the word instead of to the ink color, the interference effects from the task-irrelevant color dimension tend to be relatively weak or even absent (MacLeod, 1991). Accordingly, future studies will have to verify how this commonly observed processing asymmetry interacts with reward-related stimulus saliency. Reward associations in the reverse Stroop task may facilitate responses to reward-related words. In turn, the concomitantly increased saliency of irrelevant reward-related colors might boost the typically weak interference effects in this task.

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by NIH grant R01-MH060415 to M.G.W. and by DFG grant BO 3345/1-1 to C.N.B.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • Adcock RA, Thangavel A, Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Knutson B, Gabrieli JD. Reward-motivated learning: mesolimbic activation precedes memory formation. Neuron. 2006;50(3):507–517. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Bijleveld E, Custers R, Aarts H. Unconscious reward cues increase invested effort, but do not change speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Cognition. 2010;115(2):330–335. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Carter CS, van Veen V. Anterior cingulate cortex and conflict detection: an update of theory and data. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(4):367–379. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen JD, Dunbar K, McClelland JL. On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. Psychol Rev. 1990;97(3):332–361. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Della Libera C, Chelazzi L. Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards. Psychol Sci. 2006;17(3):222–227. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Egner T. Congruency sequence effects and cognitive control. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(4):380–390. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Engelmann JB, Pessoa L. Motivation sharpens exogenous spatial attention. Emotion. 2007;7(3):668–674. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Holroyd CB, Coles MG. The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. Psychol Rev. 2002;109(4):679–709. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Jocham G, Ullsperger M. Neuropharmacology of performance monitoring. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2009;33(1):48–60. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW, Hommer D. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens. J Neurosci. 2001;21(16):RC159. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Krebs RM, Schott BH, Schutze H, Duzel E. The novelty exploration bonus and its attentional modulation. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47(11):2272–2281. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Kristjansson A, Sigurjonsdottir O, Driver J. Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2010;72(5):1229–1236. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Locke HS, Braver TS. Motivational influences on cognitive control: behavior, brain activation, and individual differences. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8(1):99–112. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • MacLeod CM. Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: an integrative review. Psychol Bull. 1991;109(2):163–203. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Miller EK, Cohen JD. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Montague PR, Hyman SE, Cohen JD. Computational roles for dopamine in behavioural control. Nature. 2004;431(7010):760–767. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Padmala S, Pessoa L. Interactions between cognition and motivation during response inhibition. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(2):558–565. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Pessoa L. How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn Sci. 2009;13(4):160–166. [Europe PMC free article] [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. 2004;306(5695):443–447. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Ridderinkhof KR, van den Wildenberg WP, Segalowitz SJ, Carter CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: the role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 2004;56(2):129–140. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Schott BH, Minuzzi L, Krebs RM, Elmenhorst D, Lang M, Winz OH, Seidenbecher CI, Coenen HH, Heinze HJ, Zilles K, Duzel E, Bauer A. Mesolimbic functional magnetic resonance imaging activations during reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ventral striatal dopamine release. J Neurosci. 2008;28(52):14311–14319. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Schultz W. Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1997;7(2):191–197. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1935;18:643–662. [Google Scholar]
  • Ullsperger M, von Cramon DY. Error monitoring using external feedback: specific roles of the habenular complex, the reward system, and the cingulate motor area revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci. 2003;23(10):4308–4314. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • van Steenbergen H, Band GP, Hommel B. Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation: Evidence for a Role of Affect in Executive Control. Psychol Sci 2009 [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Wise RA. Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(6):483–494. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Wittmann BC, Schott BH, Guderian S, Frey JU, Heinze HJ, Duzel E. Reward-related FMRI activation of dopaminergic midbrain is associated with enhanced hippocampus-dependent long-term memory formation. Neuron. 2005;45(3):459–467. [Abstract] [Google Scholar]
  • Zellner MR, Ranaldi R. How conditioned stimuli acquire the ability to activate VTA dopamine cells: A proposed neurobiological component of reward-related learning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009 [Abstract] [Google Scholar]

Citations & impact 


Impact metrics

Jump to Citations

Citations of article over time

Smart citations by scite.ai
Smart citations by scite.ai include citation statements extracted from the full text of the citing article. The number of the statements may be higher than the number of citations provided by EuropePMC if one paper cites another multiple times or lower if scite has not yet processed some of the citing articles.
Explore citation contexts and check if this article has been supported or disputed.
https://scite.ai/reports/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.018

Supporting
Mentioning
Contrasting
23
291
7

Article citations


Go to all (129) article citations

Similar Articles 


To arrive at the top five similar articles we use a word-weighted algorithm to compare words from the Title and Abstract of each citation.


Funding 


Funders who supported this work.

NIMH NIH HHS (3)