Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

The Asymmetrical Contributions of Pleasure and Pain to Subjective Well-Being

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Many ethicists writing about well-being have assumed that claims made about the relationship between pleasure and well-being carry similar implications for the relationship between pain and well-being. I argue that the current neuroscience of pleasure and pain does not support this assumption. In particular, I argue that the experiences of pleasure and pain are mediated by different cognitive systems, that they make different contributions to human behavior in general and to well-being in particular, and that they bear fundamentally different relationships to our motivational systems and hence desires. I further argue that though there is ample evidence that pleasure can be dissociated from appetitive motivation, there is no compelling evidence suggesting that the unpleasantness of pain can be dissociated from the aversive motivational force of pains. I consider several objections to this claim, including Jennifer Corns’ recent arguments that the unpleasantness of pain experience can be dissociated from the motivational signal of pain, before briefly drawing some lessons for ethics.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We can further subdivide preference satisfaction accounts into actual preference views, which hold that well-being is improved by satisfying the preferences we have, and informed preference views, which hold that well-being is improved by satisfying the preferences we would have under some idealized conditions. This distinction will not be relevant for my discussion.

  2. Some authors have suggested that pain asymbolia due to insula lesions represents the only “genuine” case of pain affect without sensory pain (Aydede 2005; Grahek 2001). I have argued against this view for a number of reasons. First, there is only very limited evidence reporting pain asymbolia and linking it to specific brain damage. Second, the reported symptoms of asymbolia suggest to me that something more is going on than just a loss of pain affect (Klein 2012 makes a related point about the stated symptoms of asymbolia). And finally, many of the claims suggesting that cingulotomy and morphine patients don’t have “true” dissociations are based on incorrectly assuming that their symptomology is the same as that of frontal lobotomy patients. I think the most parsimonious explanation is that the patients who report feeling pain but not being bothered by it in different conditions have something similar going on, though there are important differences between the various conditions.

  3. There has been a relatively recent discovery of nerve fibers in mammals that appear to selectively respond to gentle touch such as massage (Vrontou et al. 2013). Knowledge thus far is limited. However, even if they exist and were a rough equivalent of “pleasure sensors,” pains and pleasures would still be relying on a different set of sensory transducers.

  4. “Sufficient” as Berridge uses it, just seems to mean that, all other things being equal, causing neural activation in one of these regions in a healthy, normally functioning individual produces the effect (in this case pleasure or ‘liking’).

  5. Berridge operationalizes “liking” by using it to refer to characteristic facial expressions rats make during presumably pleasurable experiences, facial expressions they share with humans and other monkeys. On Berridge’s view, liking isn’t necessarily conscious. For that matter, on his view, pleasure isn’t necessarily conscious. For the purposes of this article, when I use the term pleasure, I am referring to conscious likings. The possible existence of unconscious likings could be problematic for my account below if it turned out that the dissociation between wanting and liking only applied to unconscious likings, but the evidence doesn’t suggest this is the case.

  6. I should note that the regions weren’t shown to be active during both pleasure and pain in the same experiments. Rather, Lecknes and Tracey were reviewing a number of different studies.

  7. Donald Price, one of the pain researchers who has written the most about the affective and sensory pain pathways, has written that, “numerous studies support the view that the sensory and immediate affective dimensions of pain are separate and unique, even though they are closely related, (Price et al. 2006, p. 334). However, he also endorses a “serial pathway” model on which the sensory pain pathway serves as crucial input for affective processing, which integrates the sensory information with other relevant information (eg. the current level of anxiety). If the affective component of pain necessarily includes sensory information, one might question whether the two dimensions are truly dissociable. However, since the relevant question on my view is whether the motivational signal of pain dissociates from the feeling of unpleasantness, the possibility that the sensory component of pain contributes to the affective component would not undermine my core contention about the difference between pleasure and pain.

  8. Of course, given that many areas in the brain do many things, it’s important to not make assumptions that two types of processes “are the same” or “involve a shared component” merely because they activate similar brain regions. However, I think what distinguishes this case is the level of precision involved: Wei and Zhuo (2006), for example, have tracked changes predictive of aversive motivation signals down to the level of cell layers in particular regions of the anterior cingulate.

References

  • Aydede, M. 2005. Introduction. In Pain: New essays on its nature and the methodology of its study, ed. M. Aydede. Cambridge: Bradford Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, T.X. 1959. Toward a theory of pain: Relief of chronic pain by prefrontal leucotomy, opiates, placebos, and hypnosis. Psychological Bulletin 56: 430–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R.F., E. Bratslavsky, C. Finkenauer, and K.D. Vohs. 2001. Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology 5: 323–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berridge, K. 2004. Motivation concepts in behavioral neuroscience. Physiology & Behavior 81: 179–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berthier, M., S. Starkstein, and R. Leiguarda. 1988. Asymbolia for pain: A sensory-limbic disconnection syndrome. Annals of Neurology 24: 41–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradburn, N. 1969. The structure of psychological well being. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brooks, A.M., and G.S. Berns. 2013. Aversive stimuli and loss in the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17(6): 281–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coghill, R.C., J.G. McHaffie, and Y. Yen. 2003. Neural correlates of interindividual differences in the subjective experience of pain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100: 8538–8542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corkin, S., and N. Hebben. 1981. Subjective estimates of chronic pain before and after psychosurgery or treatment in a pain unit. Pain 1: S150.

  • Corns, J. 2014. Unpleasantness, motivational oomph, and painfulness. Mind & Language. Online version at David Bain’s Pain Project Site.

  • Cromwell, H.C., and K. Berridge. 1993. Where does damage lead to enhanced food aversion: The ventral palidum/substantia innominata or lateral hypothalmus? Brain Research 624: 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. 1978. Why you can’t make a computer that feels pain. Synthese 38: 415–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Descalzi, G., X.Y. Li, T. Chen, V. Mercaldo, K. Koga, and M. Zhuo. 2012. Rapid synaptic potentiation within the anterior cingulate cortex mediates trace fear learning. Molecular Brain 5: 6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E., and R.A. Emmons. 1985. The independence of positive and negative affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47: 1105–1117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foltz, E.L., and L.E. White. 1962. Pain ‘relief’ by frontal cingulotomy. Journal of Neurosurgery 19: 89–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grahek, N. 2001. Feeling pain and being in pain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardcastle, V. 2001. The myth of pain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Heathwood, C. 2007. The reduction of sensory pleasure to desire. Philosophical Studies 133: 23–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurka, T. 2011. The best things in life. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Janal, M.N., M. Glusman, J.P. Kuhl, and W.C. Clark. 1994. Are runners stoical? An examination of pain sensitivity in habitual runners and normally active controls. Pain 58: 109–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johansen, J.P., and H.L. Fields. 2004. Glutamatergic activation of anterior cingulate cortex produces an aversive teaching signal. Nature Neuroscience 7: 398–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., and A.B.. Krueger. 2006. Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being. The Journal of Economic Perspectives 20: 3–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, C. 2012. Imperatives, phantom pains, and hallucination by presupposition. Philosophical Psychology 25(6): 917–928.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kringelbach, M. 2010. The hedonic brain: A functional neuroanatomy of human pleasure. In Pleasures of the brain, ed. M. Kringlebach and K. Berridge, 202–221. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaGraize, S., C. Labuda, R. Rutledge, R. Jackson, and P. Fuchs. 2004. Differential effect of anterior cingulated cortex lesion on mechanical hypersensitivity and escape/avoidance behavior in an animal model of neuropathic pain. Experimental Neurology 188: 139–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LaGraize, S., J. Borzan, Y.B. Peng, and P. Fuchs. 2006. Selective regulation of pain affect following activation of the opiod anterior cingulate cortex system. Experimental Neurology 197: 22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R.J. 2002. Differential contributions of positive and negative affect to subjective well-being. In Annual meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics, vol. 18, ed. E.H. Da Silva, J.A. Matushima, and N.P. Riberio-Filho, 186–190. Rio de Janeiro: Editora Legis Summa Ltda.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, R.J., and Z. Prizmic. 2008. Regulation of emotional well-being. In The science of subjective well-being, ed. M. Eid and R. Larsen, 258–289. New York: The Guilford Press. 261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, J.T., A.P. McGraw, and J.T. Cacioppo. 2001. Can people feel happy and sad at the same time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 81: 684–696.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leknes, S., and I. Tracey. 2010. Pain and pleasure: Master’s of mankind. In Pleasures of the brain, ed. M. Kringlebach and K. Berridge, 320–335. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayerfeld, J. 1999. Suffering and moral responsibility. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mill, J.S. 1979. Utilitarianism. Indianopolis: Hackett Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J.M., S.R. Vorel, A.J. Tranguch, E.T. Kenny, P. van Mazzoni, W.G. Gorp, and H.D. Kleber. 2006. Anhedonia after a selective bilateral lesion of the globus pallidus. American Journal of Psychiatry 163: 786–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norris, C., J. Gollan, G.G. Berntson, and J.T. Cacioppo. 2010. The current status of research on the structure of evaluative space. Biological Psychology 84: 422–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostrowsky, K., M. Magnin, P. Ryvlin, J. Isnard, M. Guenot, and F. Mauguière. 2002. Representation of pain and somatic sensation in the Human Insula: A study of responses to direct electrical cortical stimulation. Cerebral Cortex 12(4): 376–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palminteri, S., D. Justo, C. Jauffret, B. Pavlicek, A. Dauta, C. Delmaire, V. Czernecki, C. Karachi, L. Capelle, A. Durr, and M. Pessiglione. 2012. Critical roles for anterior insula and dorsal striatum in punishment-based avoidance learning. Neuron 76(5): 998–1009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parfit, D. 1984. Reasons and persons. Oxford: Clarendon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ploghaus, A., C. Narain, C.F. Beckmann, S. Clare, S. Bantick, R. Wise, P.M. Matthews, J.N. Rawlins, and I. Tracey. 2001. Exacerbation of pain by anxiety is associated with activity in a hippocampal network. Journal of Neuroscience 21(24): 9896–9903.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price, D. 2000. Psychological and neural mechanisms of the affective dimension of pain. Science 288: 1769–1772.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Price, D., G.N. Verne, and J.M. Schwartz. 2006. Plasticity in brain processing and modulation of pain. Progress in Brain Research 157: 333–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pujol, J., A. Lopez, J. Deus, N. Cardoner, J. Vallejo, A. Capdevila, and T. Paus. 2002. Anatomical variability of the anterior cingulate gyrus and basic dimenions of human personality. NeuroImage 15: 847–855.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rainville, P., G.H. Duncan, D.D. Price, B. Carrier, and M.C. Bushnell. 1997. Pain affect encoded in human anterior cingulate but not somatosensory cortex. Science 277: 968–971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rainville, P., B. Carrier, R.K. Hofbauer, M.C. Bushnell, and G.H. Duncan. 1999. Dissociation of sensory and affective dimensions of pain using hypnotic modulation. Pain 82: 159–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rolls, E. 1999. The brain and emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romanelli, P., V. Esposito, and J. Adler. 2004. Ablative procedures for chronic pain. Neurosurgery Clinics of North America 15: 335–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., and E.B. Royzman. 2001. Negativity bias, negativity dominance, and contagion. Personality and Social Psychology Review 5: 296–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sawamoto, N., M. Honda, T. Okada, T. Hanakawa, M. Kanda, H. Fukuyama, and J. Konishi. 2000. Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: An event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of Neuroscience 20: 7438–7448.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schimmack, U. 2008. The structure of subjective well-being. In The science of subjective well-being, ed. M. Eid and R. Larsen, 97–123. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, W. 2002. Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron 36: 241–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shriver, A. 2006. Minding mammals. Philosophical Psychology 19: 433–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sidgwick, H. 1907. The methods of ethics. Indianapolis: Hacket Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K.S., S.V. Mahler, S. Pecina, and K.C. Berridge. 2010. Hedonic hotspots: Generating sensory pleasure in the brain. In Pleasures of the brain, ed. M.L. Kringlebach and K.C. Berridge, 27–49. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sumner, L. W. 1996. Welfare, Happiness, and Ethics. New York: Oxford University Press.

  • Sun, Y., Y. Gao, Z. Zhao, B. Huang, J. Yin, G. Taylor, and Z. Chen. 2008. Involvement of P311 in the affective, but not in the sensory component of pain. Molecular Pain 4: 23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vrontou, S., A.M. Wong, K.K. Rau, H.R. Koerber, and D.J. Anderson. 2013. Genetic identification of C fibres that detect massage-like stroking of hairy skin in vivo. Nature 493: 669–673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wager, T.D., L.Y. Atlas, M.A. Lindquist, M. Roy, C. Woo, and E. Kross. 2013. An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. New England Journal of Medicine 368: 1388–1397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, P., F. Li, X. Shen, and J.Z. Tsien. 2010. Conditional knockout of NMDA receptors in dopamine neurons prevents nicotine-conditioned place preference. PLoS ONE 5: e8616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wei, F., and M. Zhuo. 2006. Potentiation of sensory responses in the anterior cingulate cortex following digit amputation in the anaesthetized rat. Journal of Physiology 532: 823.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyvell, C., and K. Berridge. 2000. Intra-accumbens amphetamine increases the conditioned incentive salience of sucrose reward: enhancement of reward ‘wanting’ without enhanced ‘liking’ or response reinforcement. The Journal of Neuroscience 20: 8122–8130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeidan, F., K.T. Martucci, R.A. Kraft, N.S. Gordon, J.G. McHaffie, and R.C. Coghill. 2011. Brain mechanisms supporting the modulation of pain by mindfulness meditation. The Journal of Neuroscience 31(14): 5540–5548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Adam Shriver.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Shriver, A. The Asymmetrical Contributions of Pleasure and Pain to Subjective Well-Being. Rev.Phil.Psych. 5, 135–153 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-013-0171-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-013-0171-2

Keywords

Navigation