1 Introduction

Scientists, in addition to their methodological stances, hold a whole array of sociopolitical views. Moreover, scientists develop epistemic attitudes that resemble political ones. For instance, they can be more or less epistemically tolerant towards opposing scientific theories or follow leading authorities in their disciplines with different levels of trust. The legitimate question arises whether non-epistemic attitudes of scientists, such as moral values, reflect on their epistemic attitudes and general beliefs about science. In other words, one could argue that non-epistemic values influence the scientists’ views on the relationship between a scientific hypothesis and the evidence, or their perception of the scientific method (cf. Colombo et al. 2016).

The debate about the interplay between sociopolitical and epistemic values of scientists can be traced to classical contributions in the history of philosophy of science. For instance, Karl Popper urged scientists to remain epistemically open since “only if criticism meets resistance can we learn the full force of a critical argument” (Popper 1994: 92). There is a connection between Popper’s epistemic and sociopolitical views. Popper’s idea of piecemeal social engineering (e.g., 1945a: 139, 1957: 66–67) – a gradual improvement of public policies through trial and error – can be traced to his view that scientific theories are fallible, i.e., that they can be falsified and abandoned. Moreover, Popper (1945a, b) argued that this can only be achieved in a society that is open to critical discussions and acts upon them in the same way in which scientists should be epistemically open for critical arguments. The self-declared epistemological anarchist about methodological rules Paul Feyerabend (1975/1993) defended an open and critical attitude towards the scientific method arguing in favor of competing methods and values in science and criticizing the blind trust in epistemic authority. For Feyerabend, his epistemic and methodological attitudes were closely connected to his sociopolitical views. For example, according to Martin (2019), Feyerabend was inclined to argue that only theoretical pluralism, including pseudoscientific endeavors, can oppose the “stifling conformism” of contemporary science and epistemology after he had read Mill’s On Liberty. Furthermore, Martin (2019) claims that Feyerabend’s concerns were how to allow for the views of the minority to exist in science and how to control, so-to-say, the epistemic tyranny of majority beliefs.

Some notable recent contributions defend a pluralist scientific method and an open epistemic attitude in science. Straßer et al. (2015) introduced the concept of epistemic tolerance in the context of scientific disagreement, which is in line with authors who enlist epistemic tolerance as an epistemic virtue allowing for a diversity of methods and promoting scientific progress (e.g., Chang 2012; Longino 2002). Moreover, probability models and case studies from history of science support the exploration of less likely hypotheses as epistemically beneficial for the scientific community (Kitcher 1990, 1993). Kitcher (1990) argues that the optimal distribution of cognitive labor requires the investigation of diverse and opposing theories with the underlying assumption that the exploration of these views has a scientific value. Similarly, Strevens (2003) showed epistemic advantages of exploring diverse hypotheses using formal modeling. Finally, Zollman’s formal results show the importance of cognitive diversity for the accuracy of the scientific consensus and emphasize the need for openness towards rivaling theories from the perspective of the scientific community as a whole (Zollman 2007, 2010).

Epistemic authoritarianism is explored in the field of political philosophy and represents the tendency to accept the epistemic authority in the normative field (e.g., Estlund 1992). In the context of the scientific community, epistemic authoritarianism is linked to the rigid acceptance of dominant scientific paradigms and approaches in a specific field and leading authorities that dictate them. Once an epistemically authoritarian scientist adopts prevailing beliefs, her tendency to question or revise them will be limited. Such authoritarianism is the basis for other epistemic vices, such as epistemic conformity, intellectual dogmatism, closed-mindedness, or learning myopia (cf. Battaly 2014). Epistemic tolerance and epistemic authoritarianism directly influence the researchers’ decisions about rivaling theories in their field. Epistemically tolerant scientists will not instantly dismiss opposing views, bold hypotheses, and novel approaches. Researchers who are skeptical about the general inductive method of science will also take into account the limitations of science. These notions can also be understood in the light of Thomas Kuhn’s philosophy of science. On the one hand, Kuhn (1977c; 1977/2000) acknowledged the importance of the rational disagreement of researchers when it comes to choosing a scientific theory. On the other hand, Kuhn (1977a, b, c) noticed how the interpretation of data and the evaluation of hypotheses critically depend on scientists’ convergence on a common set of assumptions during the periods of normal science, i.e., periods when scientists work within one paradigm. Shared values, shared assumptions and following established research paths – in short, moving within a scientific paradigm – are indispensable for scientists’ daily work. Kuhn (e.g., Kuhn 1962/1970: 1, 137, Kuhn 1977a) also critically explained that textbooks during normal science mainly present the dominant views and sometimes even rewrite the history in favor of dominant paradigms. These textbooks influence the education and formation of junior researchers and make them less critical of dominant views (e.g., Kuhn 1962/1970: 5, 167).

The question of how one’s sociopolitical views, such as the level of conservatism, could influence one’s epistemic stances was also raised from the perspective of social psychology. It has been argued that the underlying basis of conservatism is both cognitive and motivational (Jost et al. 2003). As such, it can be found in one’s desire to arrive at a firm belief or understanding on a given topic, as opposed to uncertainty. In this sense, epistemic motives (i.e., intolerance of ambiguity, closed-mindedness, uncertainty avoidance, need for order, structure, and closure) are assumed to govern how people acquire beliefs, whether on sociopolitical or scientific topics (Jost et al. 2003). Thus, we were interested in finding out whether epistemic motives such as epistemic tolerance and authoritarianism are related to sociopolitical views as well.

More recently, Rutjens et al. (2018) showed that beliefs about science and pseudoscience depend, among other things, such as religiosity and moral convictions, on political conservatism. However, one could object that, though these findings are relevant when analyzing laypersons’ evaluation of scientific output, scientists can be regarded as epistemically privileged. Therefore, one could expect that the epistemic attitudes they obtain after years of training are superior and less sullied by their political views, than the epistemic attitudes of their students who have just started the scientific learning process. Indeed, much of the traditional philosophy of science (cf. Kuhn 1977c) emphasizes the difference between epistemic and non-epistemic values, i.e., the very assessment of scientific hypotheses and data should be based on values pertaining to the pursuit of truth rather than political or economic values. However, in the contemporary literature a different accent is placed on the discussion about the role of non-epistemic values in science (e.g., Douglas 2000, 2009; Elliott 2011). According to Douglas (2000, 2009), non-epistemic values play an indirect role by providing scientists with the means to decide what counts as sufficient evidence, whereas the direct role of the values of scientists amounts to finding reasons why a theory should be adopted. Douglas argues that the inductive risk is present not only at the “external” stage of theory acceptance but also at the “internal” stages of methodology choice, data gathering, and data interpretation (cf. 2000: 566–577). She offers a new image of the scientist, now portrayed as an active and responsible agent whose reasoning should never be detached from ethical and social influences.

Straßer et al. (2015:113) remarked that the literature in social epistemology “lacks a proper account of the epistemically appropriate response scientists should have towards opposing positions in peer disagreements.” On the contrary, we think that the first step towards such an account is an inquiry into the epistemic responses scientists do have towards opposing positions in their field and towards the scientific method in general. Accordingly, our research goal was to empirically investigate the interplay between epistemic attitudes, i.e., epistemic tolerance, epistemic authoritarianism, and skepticism about the scientific method (Fig. 1). The sociopolitical attitudes that we measured independently were the participants’ level of conservatism and their political orientation. Finally, we investigated the beliefs that researchers have about science and pseudoscience. In particular, we were interested in their degrees of belief in astrology and their tendency to question the theory of evolution.

Fig. 1
figure 1

An overview of the main theoretical concepts. Note. Bold font stands for the central attitudes of interest

1.1 Research Aims and Hypotheses

Since the epistemic attitudes that we were interested in, i.e., epistemic tolerance, epistemic authoritarianism, and skepticism towards the scientific method, have not been empirically measured before, one of our main research aims was to reasonably well operationalize these philosophical notions by proposing scales for measuring them. Given the theoretical background of these notions, we expected them to be distinct but related to the non-epistemic attitudes and beliefs that we tested (Fig. 1). Thus, the nature of our pioneering study was exploratory.

In order to construct and test our hypotheses clearly and unambiguously, we have provided quantitative effect size predictions in the form of the smallest effect size of significance for each hypothesis. The quantification of the hypotheses faced several challenges. One obstacle lies in the nature of the measures used in this research. Namely, the measures of the constructs are in the form of 5-point Likert scales, meaning that there is a problem in translating the quantitative differences into something meaningful in the real-world (Anvari and Lakens 2019). This is even more challenging because there is a limited amount of research on similar topics and no meta-analyses that could be used as guidelines for determining the expected effect sizes. Nonetheless, we have provided theoretical motivation for our hypotheses, as well as quantitative effect size predictions. These predictions were, when possible, based on results from past research on related constructs and several expected effect sizes were based on general statistical recommendations as no similar studies have been conducted thus far. Expected effect sizes were specified in the form of calculated indices of effect size (i.e., Cohen d) rather than absolute effect sizes as these measurements (i.e., Likert scales) have no intrinsic meaning (Sullivan and Feinn 2012). We have taken the recommended effect size of Cohen d = .4 as a reference point, as this is the average effect size in psychology (Brysbaert 2019), with values greater than d = .5 indicating moderate effect size (Cohen 1988). We have also calculated the power of the tests used to assess our hypotheses with the G*Power software. The motivation behind this lies in the fact that many studies in the field of behavioral sciences are underpowered (Sedlmeier and Gigerenzer 1989; Ioannidis 2005; Open Science Collaboration 2015).

As the first hypothesis, we wanted to trace a possible relationship between conservatism and epistemic attitudes. Since an overview of the psychological literature showed that epistemic motives are significantly related to social conservatism (Jost et al. 2003), it was possible that these epistemic motives also partially constitute epistemic authoritarianism and epistemic tolerance. We estimated the correlation size based on a negative correlation of r = −.11 between intellectual humility in the sociopolitical domain and conservatism (Krumrei-Mancuso & Newman 2020). These coefficients correspond with Cohen’s (1988) definition of a small effect, that could be expected in this type of study design, i.e., a non-experimental exploratory study that operationalized novel constructs. Thus, explicitly stated, we postulated the following hypotheses.

  • Hypothesis 1a. The correlation between epistemic authoritarianism and conservatism will be positive, and at least be of the size Pearson’s r = .10.

  • Hypothesis 1b. The correlation between epistemic tolerance and conservatism will be negative, and at least be of the size Pearson’s r = −.10.

Secondly, our study aimed at investigating whether skepticism towards the scientific method is more typical for right-wing researchers, or perhaps both ends of the political spectrum show certain similarities. For this reason, we proposed two competing hypotheses. The first hypothesis postulates that right-wing researchers will be more skeptical towards the scientific method. This hypothesis was made having in mind related research showing that rightists have a tendency of questioning some important scientific claims such as climate change (e.g., Dunlap and McCright 2008) or the safety of vaccines (e.g., Hamilton et al. 2015). On the other hand, in line with Feyerabend’s argumentation against a unique scientific method that is related to his left political ideas (cf. Martin 2019), the second hypothesis states that leftists would also express skepticism towards the scientific method. However, this skepticism would reflect their tendency to question the currently established scientific practices, but not the scientific endeavor en général, while rightists would show skepticism towards scientific methods and results altogether. Formally expressed, we assumed the following.

  • Hypothesis 2a. There will be at an effect of political orientation, with right-wing researchers being more skeptical towards the scientific method than left-wing researchers, with the effect size of at least of d = .4.

  • Hypothesis 2b. There will be no difference between left and right-wing researchers on the value of skepticism towards the scientific method, with both groups scoring higher than M = 3, i.e., the neutral point on the Likert scale.

As for the participant’s scientific field, we assumed that researchers working in natural sciences would tend to doubt the scientific method less and to be more epistemically authoritarian than those working in social sciences because of the nature of their methodologies. Besides obvious differences, viz. that natural scientists work in technologically equipped and highly specialized laboratories, and that most of the social scientists, especially those working in the humanities (e.g., philosophy, history, ethnology, linguistics, philology, etc.) are using rather different methods (conceptual analysis, historical method, synoptic method, discourse analysis, etc.), one can also trace differences in the way of conducting and publishing original research. According to Kuhn's (1962/1970) famous discussion about paradigmatic and pre-paradigmatic science, there is a difference between sciences that already have a developed paradigm under which their research is conducted, while in the pre-paradigmatic state a plurality of competing approaches exists alongside each other. Big developments like Darwin’s theory shift a science from the pre-paradigmatic to the paradigmatic stage (Kuhn 1962/1970). According to this theory, humanities and social science are still in the pre-paradigmatic stage. Following Kuhn, one can expect differences in responses between natural and social scientists on the epistemic tolerance, epistemic authoritarianism, and skepticism towards the scientific scales.

Moreover, Jaffe (2014) offered a quantitative analysis of bibliometric data of scientific publications thereby accounting for different research strategies in natural and social sciences. Thus, Jaffe brought to light that natural scientists adhere to a “follower” strategy resulting in publications with multiple authors, a high level of international collaboration, as well as a high citation rate from the colleagues publishing in the same target journals; whereas social scientists seem to care more about originality than following in the footsteps of already tackled topics, so they end up with publications characterized by high levels of self-citations, a low citation rate in a wide range of journals and a low level of international collaboration. Jaffe’s results are in line with bibliometric trends already reported in papers tracing the co-authorship patterns such as Frame and Carpenter (1979), Luukkonen (1992), and Persson et al. (2004), as well as more recently in Parish et al. (2018). These studies also indicate that greater international research collaboration has been observed to coincide with elevated citation impact and to represent a vital characteristic of the “hard science”, most notably physics, medicine, and biology. To sum up, when it comes to the relationship between the scientific field and the epistemic attitudes, we postulated the following.

  • Hypothesis 3a. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of the scientific field on the value of the epistemic authoritarianism scale, with researchers from natural sciences scoring higher than the ones from social sciences.

  • Hypothesis 3b. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of the scientific field on the value of epistemic tolerance, with researchers from social sciences scoring higher than the ones from natural sciences.

  • Hypothesis 3c. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of the scientific field on the value of skepticism towards the scientific method, with researchers from social sciences scoring higher than the ones in natural sciences.

Also, we postulated the career stage of researchers as relevant for their epistemic attitudes and assumed that senior researchers would be less skeptical about the scientific method than junior ones since they have received more training, their professional experience is more enriched with age, and their theoretical views are refined. Closely related to this, is the assumption concerning the degree of epistemic tolerance of junior researchers. Given their young age and susceptibility to various theoretical influences, we expected that they would be less dogmatic and more inclined to reassess dominant paradigms in their disciplines. Moreover, we assumed that epistemic authoritarianism is related to the time spent researching in a specific science field following its dominant paradigms. Thus, we started from the following two assumptions.

  • Hypothesis 4a. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of career stage on the value of epistemic authoritarianism with senior researchers being more authoritarian than junior researchers.

  • Hypothesis 4b. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of career stage on the value of skepticism towards the scientific method, with senior researchers being less skeptical than junior researchers.

Lastly, we set out to examine the difference between senior and junior researchers concerning their stances towards pseudoscience in general. Research in social psychology explored inclinations of laymen to believe in pseudoscientific statements and question science (e.g., Rutjens et al. 2018). We wanted to explore whether there is a difference in the degree of belief in pseudoscientific claims between senior and junior researchers, assuming that more experienced researchers will question science less. Thus, we postulated the following hypotheses about the beliefs in science.

  • Hypothesis 5a. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of career stage on astrological beliefs, with junior researchers holding higher beliefs than senior researchers.

  • Hypothesis 5b. There will be at least an effect of d = .4 of career stage on beliefs about evolution, with junior researchers questioning evolution more than seniors.

In order to test these hypotheses, we conducted an online survey and explored the interplay between the epistemic and the political attitudes of scientists. We propose scales for measuring the level of their 1) epistemic tolerance, 2) epistemic authoritarianism, and 3) skepticism towards the scientific method. Moreover, to investigate the relationship between epistemic attitudes and sociopolitical views, we asked scientists to position themselves on the political spectrum and included a separate scale for measuring the level of their conservatism. Finally, in order to better assess participants’ general tendency to relativize science, we included measures of their beliefs in science and pseudoscience.

2 Method

2.1 Participants

The study was conducted among 655 participants from SerbiaFootnote 1 (248 male, 352 female, and 55 genders not reported), 18 to 74 years old (M = 29; Sd = 11) through purposive and convenience sampling methods. Participants reported their education level, as well as their field of study. Based on these data, the participants were classified as either junior (75.4%, from undergraduate studies to Ph.D. studies) or senior researchers (24.6%, with a doctorate degree). Participants specified their scientific discipline, based on which we categorized them in either the field of natural or social sciences (Appendix Tables 6 and 7). This was done because of a small proportion of some disciplines in the overall sample, as well as a disproportion of junior and senior researchers in certain disciplines. For this reason, further analyses were conducted with two categories, the field of natural (48.9% of participants) or social (51.1%) sciences. The rather large scale of the sample was necessary in order to gather a sufficient number of participants from different categories, i.e., junior and senior researchers from different fields, as well as participants with different political orientations, since we wanted to analyze differences between these categories. Besides a scale measuring economic and social conservatism, an ideological measure included in the study is the participants’ self-report of their political orientation. We initially used a seven-point scale (−3 as an indication of far left, −2 left, −1 moderately left, 0 center, 1 moderately right, 2 right, 3 far right). However, because a small number of participants reported being on the right side of the spectrum, categories were narrowed down to three: left (64.2%), center (23%), and right (12.8%). Further analyses were conducted on these three categories.

2.2 Item Development

Given the lack of empirical research on epistemic attitudes, this aspect of the present study is exploratory. One of our aims was to operationalize epistemic authoritarianism and tolerance, as well as skepticism towards the scientific method. These instruments represent multidimensional measures of epistemic attitudes, as modeled on an overview of the literature of previous attempts to measure a related construct, i.e., views on the nature of science (Deng et al. 2011).

Epistemic attitudes operationalized within two scientific field-dependent dimensions are epistemic tolerance and authoritarianism. Epistemic tolerance is operationalized using seven items that indicate the awareness of scientists that the views of researchers with opposing approaches might have scientific value (e.g., The opinions of the researchers I disagree with should be taken into consideration; Even the papers of the researchers I disagree with are scientifically valuable). The items are constructed to measure the epistemic tolerance within the researchers’ specific field of expertise in the situations of rational disagreements between peers. The epistemic authoritarianism scale also consists of 7 items that were formulated to indicate researchers’ firm acceptance of dominant paradigms, approaches or theories in their discipline, as well as their reluctance to question these prevailing views (e.g., The academic authorities in my field are right; Some of the results within my scientific field are conclusively proved and should not be questioned). Participants indicated their attitudes on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from strong disagreement to strong agreement with the constructed statements (i.e., 1 = strongly disagree, 2 = mostly disagree, 3 = neither agree nor disagree, 4 = mostly agree, 5 = strongly agree). All items can be found in Appendix 2.

The scale measuring skepticism towards the scientific method is inspired by the philosophical considerations that were discussed in section 1. The content of the scale does not restrict itself to the participants’ field of study; rather, it is about attitudes towards science in general, as well as specific fields, such as mathematics.Footnote 2 By including items concerning the status of mathematics, this scale is designed to measure radical relativism alongside skepticism towards methods of both natural and social sciences. The skepticism towards the scientific method was assessed in three domains: a) scientists’ subjectivity (4 items, example: What scientists publish is more a reflection of their opinion, than the objective truth), b) stochastic progress of science (3 items, example: Progress in science is achieved solely by mere chance) and c) intelligibility of the objective truth (5 items, example: Science will never be able to adequately describe reality). We made this decision in part due to an overview of previous research on a related phenomenon (i.e., views on the nature of science), which was treated as a multidimensional construct (Deng et al. 2011). Participants indicated their attitudes towards statements about an aspect of skepticism on a 5-point Likert-type scale, which ranged from strong disagreement to strong agreement. For all items, a skeptical position was reflected by a high score. All items are listed in Appendix 2.

2.3 Procedure

Participants were directed to a set of online questionnaires on the Limesurvey platform.Footnote 3 After participants gave their informed consent, they were presented with instructions (see Appendix 2), followed by the questionnaires. The order of presentations of scales was fixed, while the order of items within the scales was randomized. The first questionnaire was a measure of skepticism towards the scientific method. This measure was constructed by the researchers and assessed beliefs about skepticism towards the scientific method (SSM scale) in three domains. The items from the epistemic tolerance and authoritarianism scales were then presented together and in a randomized order. These measures consist of 7 items each and assess epistemic authoritarianism and tolerance towards theories and researchers in one’s scientific discipline. Participants also completed two 5-point scales that indicated their attitudes towards science and pseudoscience. One scale assessed participants’ beliefs about the theory of evolution and it is an adaptation of an existing scale (Johnson and Peeples 1987). The other is an adaptation of Randall and Desrosiers’ (1980) scale concerning attitudes towards astrology. Participants’ sociopolitical views were measured with Everett’s (2013) 12-item Social and Economic Conservatism Scale (SECS) that was adapted for our study. Participants were given words or phrases and asked to rate them on a ‘feeling thermometer’ where 10 represents very negative, and 100 represents very positive feelings about an issue. In addition to translating the scale, we excluded two items concerning gun control and immigration policies, as these topics are more suitable indicators of conservatism in the USA than in Serbia. Lastly, participants were asked to provide demographic information (i.e., field of study, education level, political orientation).

3 Results

We will first present descriptive statistics and measures of reliability and representativeness for the scales we constructed. A confirmatory factor analysis was also conducted in order to test the construct validity of our scales. Further results are presented in the same order as the hypotheses are proposed. The first hypothesis is tested by calculating the Pearson correlation coefficient that was used for assessing the relationships between the other constructs as well. Additionally, we have conducted a principal component analysis in an attempt to transform the study variables into a smaller number of underlying constructs. In order to investigate group differences among our participants (hypotheses 2–5), we performed analyses of variance (ANOVAs).

3.1 Descriptive Measures and Psychometric Properties of Epistemic Attitudes

Descriptive statistics for the scales measuring epistemic attitudes appear in Table 1. Our results show that researchers are generally epistemically tolerant, and are not epistemically authoritarian or skeptical towards the scientific method. This is further seen in values of skewness and kurtosis, as well as from Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests of normality that indicate that the relevant variables are normally distributed. The internal reliability, represented by the symbol α in Table 1, for the scales for epistemic tolerance and epistemic authoritarianism is acceptable (Nunnally and Bernstein 1994). The skepticism towards the scientific method scale is divided into the subscale scores of scientists’ subjectivity, stochastic progress of science, and the intelligibility of the objective truth. Both the subscores and full-scale scores are presented. The scale as a whole has acceptable internal reliability.Footnote 4 The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure of representativeness was calculated in order to assess the proportion of common variance between variables in a particular scale. All measures of KMO are above 0.8, indicating adequate sampling (Kaiser and Rice 1974) for all scales (Epistemic tolerance: KMO = .81, χ2(21) = 781.50, p < .001; Epistemic authoritarianism: KMO = .83, χ2(21) = 747.81, p < .001; Skepticism towards the scientific method: KMO = .88, χ2(21) = 2179.90, p < .001).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and psychometric properties of epistemic attitudes

3.2 Construct Validity of Epistemic Attitudes Scales

In order to assess the construct validity of our scales in this phase, we have conducted confirmatory factor analysis. This analysis included items from three scales measuring epistemic attitudes, i.e., epistemic authoritarianism, epistemic tolerance, and skepticism towards the scientific method. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of sampling adequacy and Bartlett’s test of sphericity indicated that the data are suitable for factor analysis, KMO = .871, χ2(325) = 4339.32, p < .001. A maximum-likelihood confirmatory factor analysis was conducted with the number of factors fixed to three and the promax rotation. The model explains 32% of the variance.

The pattern matrix for the 3-factor solution is shown in Appendix 3 (Table 8). The first factor includes items from the scale measuring skepticism towards the scientific method, the second items regarding epistemic authoritarianism, and the third epistemic tolerance. Correlations between the first and second factor are moderate, as well as the second and third, while there is no correlation between the first and third factor (Appendix 3, Table 9).

3.3 Relationships between Epistemic and Sociopolitical Attitudes and Beliefs about Science

We have tested our first hypothesis and conducted an exploration of relationships between the main constructs by calculating Pearson correlation coefficients. As performing multiple tests on the same data set increases the chances of obtaining a false positive result, we have tested the correlations against a Bonferroni adjusted alpha level of .008. As Table 2 shows, there is a significant correlation of low intensity between conservatism and epistemic authoritarianism and tolerance in the expected direction and greater than r = .10, thus confirming our first hypotheses, 1a and 1b. Conservatism is the only variable that has significant correlations with all other constructs, most notably with beliefs about the theory of evolution, as well as a positive low correlation with skepticism towards the scientific method. Skepticism towards the scientific method has a moderate correlation with epistemic authoritarianism. On the other hand, it is not significantly related to epistemic tolerance. Epistemic tolerance and epistemic authoritarianism, on the other hand, are moderately related. Beliefs about science are not related to epistemic authoritarianism and tolerance, but do have significant relationships with skepticism towards the scientific method.

Table 2 Pearson correlation coefficients of epistemic and sociopolitical attitudes and beliefs about science

3.4 Principal Component Analysis of Epistemic and Sociopolitical Attitudes and Beliefs about Science

In order to examine the possibility of transforming our set of correlated variables into uncorrelated underlying constructs, we conducted a principal component analysis. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of .61 is around .6, the suggested minimum for sampling adequacy (Kaiser and Rice 1974). Bartlett’s test of sphericity was statistically significant (χ2 (15) = 468.49, p < .001). Two components with eigenvalues greater than one were extruded, and the model explains 56.74% of the variance in the data. The first component with an eigenvalue of 1.83 accounts for 30.56% of the variance in the data, while the second component had an eigenvalue of 1.57 and accounted for further 26.18% of the variance. Correlations of variables and the components can be found in Table 3.

Table 3 Results from a principal component analysis of epistemic and sociopolitical attitudes and beliefs about science

The first component has the highest correlation with beliefs about science, conservatism, as well as skepticism towards the scientific method. This may indicate how sociopolitical attitudes and beliefs about science and the scientific method are intertwined. However, skepticism towards the scientific method and conservatism correlate with the second component as well. This component has the highest correlation with epistemic authoritarianism and epistemic tolerance. This indicates that there is no clear line of demarcation between epistemic attitudes and sociopolitical views and that these components cannot be used in further analyses as a substitute for the main constructs of the study.

Since we assumed that these constructs are theoretically related, we have also conducted a factor analysis. We employed maximum likelihood as an extraction method with promax rotation, in order to allow correlations between factors. The Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin measure of .60 indicates borderline adequate sampling adequacy and Bartlett’s test of sphericity was statistically significant (χ2 (15) = 197.87, p < .001). Two factors with eigenvalues greater than one were extruded, with the first factor accounting for 20.1% of the variance, and the second for 18.4%. The pattern matrix for the 2-factor solution is shown in Table 4.

Table 4 Results from a factor analysis of epistemic and sociopolitical attitudes and beliefs about science

The structure of these results is complementary to the principal component analysis. Namely, Factor 1 is primarily related to sociopolitical attitudes and beliefs about science, with a moderate loading of skepticism towards the scientific method. Factor 2 is primarily related to epistemic attitudes, as it has the highest loadings with epistemic authoritarianism and tolerance, as well as skepticism. The correlation coefficient between Factor 1 and Factor 2 is r = −.13.

3.5 Epistemic Attitudes and Political Orientation

In order to test our hypotheses about the interaction of skepticism towards the scientific method with the political orientation of researchers, we have conducted an independent samples t-test, as well as a single sample t-test. No significant difference was found in skepticism towards the scientific method in relation to the participants’ political orientation (t(366) = −1.82, p > .05). Moreover, skepticism towards the scientific method was significantly lower (M = 2.1, SD = 0.6) than the neutral point (M = 3), (t(366) = −28.944, p < .001), meaning our hypotheses 2a and 2b were not confirmed.

Further exploration of group differences in political orientation shows that in the case of epistemic authoritarianism (F(1, 475) = 5.15, p < .01), leftists are significantly more tolerant compared to centrists (MD = 0.2, p < .05), while no difference was found compared to rightists (MD = 0.2, p > .05). Although the differences are statistically significant, the effect size is smallFootnote 5p2 = .02), and d = .3 is below the threshold, meaning this result does not hold scientific significance. The same can be said for epistemic tolerance (F(1, 475) = 5.84, p < .01), where a significant difference of small effect size (ηp2 = .02), and d = .4 was found between leftists (M = 4.5; SD = 0.5) and rightists (M = 4.3; SD = 0.5; p < .01), with no significant difference between centrist (M = 4.4; SD = 0.5) and leftists or rightists. There was also no differences in beliefs in astrology (F(1, 475) = 2.34, p > .05), but a moderate effect size (ηp2 = .10) with a test power of .99 was found in beliefs about the theory of evolution (F(1, 475) = 27.13, p < .001), with leftists (M = 4.5; SD = 0.6) scoring higher than centrists (M = 4.1; SD = 0.7, p < .001) and rightists (M = 3.9; SD = 0.8, p < .001).

3.6 Differences in Epistemic Attitudes by Scientific Field and Career Stage

In order to investigate our hypotheses 3–5, we conducted a between-subjects ANOVA on the participants’ scores on epistemic attitudes and beliefs about science with the scientific field and career stage as independent variables. The results show a significant main effect (F(1, 599) = 54.67, p < .001) of the scientific field with a moderate effect size (ηp2 = .08) on epistemic authoritarianism. The mean response for participants in social sciences (M = 2.3; SD = 0.6) was lower than the mean response for participants in the field of natural sciences (M = 2.7; SD = 0.7). The test power was .99 with the effect size of d = .7, thus confirming our hypothesis 3a. There was no significant effect of the career stage on epistemic authoritarianism (F(1, 599) = 0.02, p > .05), meaning our hypothesis 4a was not confirmed. Similar results were found in the case of epistemic tolerance (F(1, 599) = 48.83, ηp2 = .07, p < .001), as researchers from social sciences (M = 4.6; SD = 0.4) scored significantly higher than the ones from natural sciences (M = 4.3; SD = 0.6). The test power was also .99 with the effect size of d = .7, confirming our hypothesis 3b. No significant interaction was found between the scientific field and career stage in the case of epistemic authoritarianism (F(1, 599) = 1.72, p > .05), nor epistemic tolerance (F(1, 599) = 2.37, p > .05).

The analysis conducted on skepticism towards the scientific method shows a significant interaction effect of the scientific field and career stage (F(1, 599) = 3.86, p < .05, ηp2 = .01), albeit of small effect size with d = .5. Junior researchers in the field of social sciences (М = 2.4; SD = 0.5) are more skeptical than seniors from the same field (M = 2.1; SD = 0.6), while in natural sciences no differences between junior (М = 2.0; SD = 0.5) and senior (М = 2.0; SD = 0.6) researchers were detected, partially confirming hypotheses 3c and 4b.Footnote 6 This interaction is presented in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Interaction between the scientific field and career stage with skepticism towards the scientific method as a dependent variable

When it comes to beliefs in astrology, seniority (F(1, 599) = 0.00, p > .05) and scientific field (F(1, 599) = 0.05, p > .05) do not play an important role, meaning hypothesis 5a was not confirmed. On the other hand, a significant interaction between the participants’ scientific field and the career stage was found in the case of beliefs about evolution (F (2, 599) = 2.77, p < .05, ηp2 = .01), with junior researchers in the field of social sciences (M = 4.1; SD = 0.8) holding somewhat weaker beliefs (d = .5) about the theory of evolution than senior researchers from the same field (M = 4.5; SD = 0.7). No difference between junior (M = 4.2; SD = 0.7) and senior (M = 4.3; SD = 0.8) researchers in the field of natural sciences was found (Fig. 3). Thus, hypothesis 5b was confirmed for the researchers coming from social sciences.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Interaction between the scientific field and the career stage with beliefs about the theory of evolution as a dependent variable

Finally, a summary of the main findings is presented in Table 5. The table includes a brief description of the hypotheses, as well as the evaluation of the findings with regard to each hypothesis.

Table 5 Summary of the main findings

4 Discussion

As mentioned, the principal aim of our research was to operationalize and empirically test epistemic attitudes, with epistemic tolerance construed as an epistemic virtue, epistemic authoritarianism as an epistemic vice, and skepticism towards the scientific method as a Janus-faced epistemic attitude, rather than virtuous at all times. The first step concerns the confirmation of the quality of the newly proposed scales and the interpretation of the descriptive statistics of all scales presented in the previous section.

When it comes to descriptive statistics, from skewness and kurtosis values, as well as from Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests of normality, we see that none of the relevant variables are normally distributed. This does not necessarily indicate a problem with the used scales but rather reflects upon the underlying nature of the measured phenomena, as well as the characteristics of the scientific community. Our sample of researchers is shown to group around high values in cases of epistemic tolerance and beliefs about the theory of evolution. On the other hand, the participants’ scores group around low values in cases of epistemic authoritarianism, skepticism towards the scientific method, and beliefs in astrology. It is important to note that the standardized skewness of conservatism did not deviate from the normal distribution. The high negative value of kurtosis indicates a higher number of cases with extreme values than expected in a normal distribution. Therefore, we were able to ensure the diversity of the sample with regard to conservatism, which cannot be said for the political orientation. Namely, there was a small number of participants who classified themselves as being on the far-right side of the political spectrum. This could be a specific characteristic of the scientific community, though it should be noted that we used convenient sampling methods.

As indicated by the internal reliabilities and measures of representativeness, the three proposed scales for epistemic attitudes performed well. The adequateness of the scales is further demonstrated by the results of the confirmatory factor analysis that reproduced the factors of the three scales with the corresponding items. The fact that epistemic authoritarianism and tolerance are moderately correlated, with skepticism being related to epistemic authoritarianism but not tolerance, as well as the results of the principal component analysis, suggest that these constructs cannot be theoretically reduced to one another.Footnote 7

An overview of the main relationships found in the study can be seen in Fig. 4. The relationships between conservatism and epistemic attitudes (epistemic tolerance, epistemic authoritarianism, and skepticism towards the scientific method) are present but weak. This, alongside the fact that our second hypothesis was not confirmed, i.e., that the political orientation does not affect the epistemic attitudes, implies that, contrary to common expectations, political attitudes do not strongly affect professional beliefs and decisions of scientists. This finding goes in the direction that science could be free of political views (cf. Mattes 2019). Moreover, the effect of the scientific field on epistemic attitudes was more relevant than the effect of sociopolitical views. Natural scientists were, on average, more epistemically authoritarian and less epistemically tolerant. This is to be expected given the nature of their field, i.e., the methods used by natural scientists are stricter in comparison to the ones of social scientists. Nonetheless, it is worth noticing that both natural and social scientists are, to a large extent, tolerant, even though social scientists exhibit higher levels of epistemic tolerance.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Overview of the main findings. Note. A full line indicates a positive and a dashed line a negative correlation

The career stage also plays a role in the skepticism towards the scientific method, with junior social researchers being more skeptical than seniors in the same field. On the other hand, there are no differences between junior and senior researchers among natural scientists. These results may indicate that with their professional development, researchers’ trust in the scientific method increases. On the other hand, it seems that junior researchers in the field of natural sciences do not consider the option of questioning the methods used in their field. This is further supported by the finding that there are no differences in beliefs about the theory of evolution between junior and senior researchers in natural sciences. However, junior researchers in the field of social sciences question the theory of evolution more than seniors. This could indicate that researchers in social sciences develop a greater trust in the scientific method in general during their careers.

We have also investigated the sociopolitical views of scientists. Conservatism has a weak positive correlation with beliefs in astrology. A medium effect was found between the participants’ political orientation and their beliefs about the theory of evolution. These results are in line with the finding that participants who score higher on the conservatism scale are more prone to questioning the theory of evolution (Miller et al. 2006). This is in accordance with our expectations: the results are compatible with the previous findings that different forms of scientific acceptance and rejection are generally grounded in conservatism (Rutjens et al. 2018).

The novelty of our findings stems from the fact that participants in our study are researchers from natural and social sciences. Our study challenges the conclusions of Bayir et al. (2014), who found no differences in natural and social scientists’ views on the nature of science.Footnote 8

As previously mentioned, this is an exploratory study, pioneering measures of novel constructs, that are essentially attitudes, some of which are socially desirable. Most notably, our participants showed high levels of epistemic tolerance and low levels of epistemic authoritarianism, as well as little skepticism towards the scientific method. They declared themselves as predominantly liberal and held the theory of evolution in high regard, while the same could not be said about astrology. Therefore, it is important to take these characteristics of the scientific community into account when drawing conclusions and implications from the results. The question remains whether our sample represents the scientific community as a whole and if greater effects would be found if there were more variations within our sample.

5 Conclusions

Epistemic tolerance and epistemic authoritarianism play an important role whenever scientists make epistemic decisions about competing theories in their discipline. Epistemically tolerant researchers do not dismiss the work of their peers with opposing views, while epistemically authoritarian scientists are less willing to revise views that they adopted. On the other hand, skepticism towards the scientific method is connected with the tendency to question and investigate the limits of science. Both epistemic tolerance and epistemic authoritarianism of scientists, as well as their level of skepticism towards the scientific method, were empirically measured for the first time. We proposed three measuring scales. The conducted analyses showed that all of the scales capture independent phenomena and that they are statistically reliable. Additionally, we compared the epistemic attitudes with the sociopolitical views of our survey participants in order to examine whether their epistemic attitudes are in any way “sullied” by their political orientation and level of conservatism.

Our results represent a positive finding regarding the scientific community: scientists, at least on a declarative level, see themselves as epistemically tolerant towards opposing theories and approaches, and not epistemically authoritarian. These values influence their epistemic decisions, such as equal consideration of rivaling theories in their field. On the other hand, the results revealed that the epistemic attitudes of scientists are, to a certain degree, dependent on their discipline and the level of their experience. As expected, given the irregular patterns governing their field of expertise, social scientists are to some extent more epistemically tolerant. Moreover, junior researchers in social sciences tend to relativize the scientific method more than their experienced colleagues. We attribute this finding to the fact that they are new to science and most likely did not yet become advocates for any specific approach. When it comes to endorsing pseudoscience, scientists dismissed believing in astrology almost unanimously, regardless of their political orientation, field of expertise, or career stage. Interestingly, junior researchers in social sciences also tend to believe less in evolutionary theory than the senior researchers, while such a difference was not found between junior and senior researchers in natural sciences. Finally, as for the impact of sociopolitical views on the corpus of epistemic attitudes, we pointed out that the tested epistemic values are not influenced by political orientation and are only weakly correlated with conservatism. Rather, the specific scientific field directs one’s level of epistemic tolerance. Hence, based on our findings, the influence that political attitudes have on the epistemic decisions of scientists is marginal.

Following Bruder et al. (2013) who showed that their Conspiracy Mentality Questionnaire could be used for exploring the differences in beliefs in conspiracy theories across European, North American and Middle Eastern population, it would be interesting, for further research, to conduct a cross-national study that would investigate the views of researchers coming from different sociopolitical backgrounds and academic cultures. It would also be interesting to further investigate the interplay between the proposed scales for measuring epistemic attitudes and other similar constructs such as intellectual humility (e.g., Alfano et al. 2017.) or open-mindedness (e.g., Fujita et al. 2007), as well as their non-epistemic counterparts, i.e., political intolerance and authoritarianism (e.g., Crawford & Pilanski 2014), and even character traits such as openness and agreeableness (e.g., Kajonius and Dåderman 2014). Finally, it would be relevant to adapt the proposed scales for measuring epistemic attitudes in the general population.