Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 16 March 2021
Sec. Movement Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Psychological Factors in Physical Education and Sport View all 28 articles

Psychophysiological Benefits of Real-Time Heart Rate Feedback in Physical Education

\nTino Stckel
Tino Stöckel*Robert GrimmRobert Grimm
  • Sport and Exercise Psychology Unit, Department of Sport Science, University of Rostock, Rostock, Germany

School physical education (PE) has the potential to contribute to public-health promotion and well-being, but oftentimes students' lack of motivation toward PE or physical activity in general, especially during adolescence, diminishes, or eradicates the positive effects associated with PE. Therefore, practical approaches are required that help teachers to increase or awake students intrinsic motivation toward PE, for which self-determination theory may provide the conceptual framework. In that regard, the purpose of the present study was to examine whether the use of real-time, heart rate feedback (as a method to support students' need for autonomy and competence) during regular PE lessons has the potential to increase students' autonomous motivation and physical effort. To achieve this, we had forty healthy adolescents between 16 and 17 years of age run for 30 min either with (experimental group, EG) or without (control group, CG) real-time, individualized heart rate feedback during a regular PE class and compared physical and perceived exertion as well as joy of running between the two groups. Participants were randomly assigned to the groups. Our data revealed that participants in the EG enjoyed running more than participants in the CG (joy of running was 3.20 in the EG vs. 2.63 in the CG, p = 0.03) despite a higher physical (163 to 178 in EG vs. 141 to 156 beats per minute in the CG, p < 0.001) and perceived exertion (rating of perceived exertion of 13.22 in the EG vs. 10.59 in the CG, p = 0.02). That means, running with real-time, individualized heart rate feedback apparently increased participants' motivation to run and to enjoy running at higher levels of exertion. In that regard, real-time, individualized activity feedback should be implemented in regular PE classes systematically and repeatedly to create a controllable and attainable situation that allows students to actively adjust their own behavior to achieve appealing and realistic goals.

Introduction

Physical activity has long been known as a major, independent risk factor of individual and public health and well-being (World Health Organization, 2010; Hallal et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2012). As regular physical activity during childhood benefits all aspects of child development (Andersen et al., 2006; Reiner et al., 2013), it is recommended that children engage in 60 min of moderate (MPA) to vigorous aerobic physical activity (VPA; i.e., fast walking and running associated with elevated heart rates) every day (Strong et al., 2005; Janssen and LeBlanc, 2010; World Health Organization, 2010). Depending on the criteria and model used (Epstein et al., 2001; Armstrong and Welsman, 2006; Brown et al., 2006; McManus et al., 2008; American College of Sports Medicine, 2014; Eckard et al., 2019), MPA is typically defined with heart rates above 64–70% of maximum heart rate, or heart rates ranging between 140 and 160 beats per minute (bpm); and VPA with heart rates above 76–85% of maximum heart rate or with heart rates above 160 bpm. Field observations, however, indicated that not even half of the children and adolescents comply with the recommended levels of physical activity (Ekelund et al., 2011; Van Hecke et al., 2016). Actual daily engagement in moderate to vigorous physical activity in school-aged children is (with about 14–20 min on average) well below the recommended 60 min per day (Biddle and Goudas, 1996; Sallis et al., 2000; McManus et al., 2008). Making things worse, it has been reported that moderate and vigorous physical activity declines dramatically with increasing age during adolescence (Armstrong et al., 2000; Parish and Treasure, 2003; McManus et al., 2008; Knuth and Hallal, 2009; Van Hecke et al., 2016). Hallal et al. (2012) reported that worldwide four out of five adolescents do not meet the physical activity recommendations.

Physical education (PE) in school may have the potential to contribute to public-health promotion (Fox and Harris, 2003; Standage and Gillison, 2007; Singh et al., 2012; Burns et al., 2017) as it is mandatory for almost all children and adolescents in the world. However, the recommendations for daily physical activity are usually not met by PE in school alone as it only covers 2–3 days of a week of the recommended daily 60 min of moderate to vigorous physical activity (given the intensity is high enough; cf. Stratton, 1996; Fröberg et al., 2017). Therefore, it is required that children engage in extracurricular physical activity to make up for the lack. PE appears to be the perfect place to build up children's motivation to engage in extracurricular physical activities as a central aim of PE in school is to encourage students to take on a physically active and healthy lifestyle. It is generally agreed upon that this can be achieved by growing children's understanding of the benefits of an active and healthy lifestyle and by letting them experience that physical activity can bring joy and inherent satisfaction (Ferrer-Caja and Weiss, 2000; Vallerand, 2007; Ryan and Deci, 2017). In particular, enjoyment of PE has been shown to have a considerable positive impact on children's extracurricular physical activity (Dishman et al., 2005; Cairney et al., 2007; Cox et al., 2008).

The Self-determination theory (SDT; Deci and Ryan, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2007, 2017) provides a conceptual framework on how autonomous forms of motivation, which are typically accompanied by the feeling of enjoyment and inherent satisfaction, can be achieved. It describes different modes of regulation with external regulation being the least and intrinsic motivation being the most self-determined and autonomous forms of regulation (Ryan and Deci, 2000; Deci and Ryan, 2002). With regard to PE (or school in general), children and adolescents often act for external reasons (Ntoumanis, 2001; Ntoumanis et al., 2004), which are usually associated with the expectation of reward and/or punishment (e.g., external pressure by parents of getting good or avoiding bad marks) and the avoidance of guilt associated with not partaking or underperforming (e.g., internal pressure based on perceived expectations of classmates). However, acting for internal reasons (i.e., intrinsic, identified, and introjected regulation) is typically associated with a higher engagement and a stronger feeling of enjoyment, interest and inherent satisfaction (e.g., partaking in PE because being physical active or a specific behavior itself has personal value or brings the student joy, not just the outcome) as compared to acting for external reasons (i.e., lower autonomous motivation) (Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2017; Spray et al., 2006; Vallerand, 2007; Bice et al., 2016).

SDT proposes that autonomous motivation can be achieved by promoting the satisfaction of three innate psychological needs: competence, autonomy and relatedness (Deci and Ryan, 2002; Standage and Gillison, 2007; Ryan and Deci, 2017). Competence describes one's feeling to be able (i.e., to have what it takes) to effectively adjust own behavior to achieve desired outcomes. Several studies have come to the conclusion that perceived competence might be the most important predictor for intrinsic motivation toward and enjoyment of PE (Standage et al., 2005; Hashim et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2010; Gråstén et al., 2012; Ryan and Deci, 2017). Autonomy describes the need to self-endorse activities and the feeling to be able to handle situations on one's own. It has been shown that autonomy support provided by the teacher by actively supporting choice, initiation and understanding enhances motivation toward PE and extracurricular physical activity (Hagger et al., 2003; Standage et al., 2003, 2006; Reeve et al., 2004; Alderman et al., 2006). Relatedness describes the need to belong, to feel connected, accepted, and close to significant others. Relatedness appears not as important for enhancing and maintaining autonomous forms of motivation as autonomy and competence (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Standage and Gillison, 2007), but there is evidence to suggest that it supports maintaining motivated when acting for external reasons (Standage and Gillison, 2007). Satisfaction of the three psychological needs, especially competence and autonomy, in PE has been shown to be positively associated to autonomous forms of motivation toward PE (Standage et al., 2003, 2006; Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage and Gillison, 2007). Moreover, autonomous motivation toward PE has been positively associated to general self-esteem and psychological well-being (Hein and Hagger, 2007; Standage and Gillison, 2007) supporting the idea that (motivational processes within) PE has the potential to impact on self-perception and a healthy active lifestyle.

The Present Study

Especially during adolescence, many students are only physically active in PE for external reasons (i.e., because they have to or to avoid bad marks) or are not being motivated to be physically active at all (Ntoumanis, 2001; Ntoumanis et al., 2004) resulting in low levels of physical activity during PE classes (Knuth and Hallal, 2009). That completely undermines the central aim of PE to encourage students to take on a physically active and healthy lifestyle and the recommendation to engage in at least 60 min per day of moderate to vigorous physical activity. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to explore the effect of real-time activity feedback on adolescents' experience of intrinsic satisfaction of being physically active during PE (i.e., enjoying the activity due to the feeling of improvement and accomplishment; cf. Wankel and Kreisel, 1985; Vallerand, 2007; Bice et al., 2016). More specifically, we examined whether the use of immediate heart rate feedback during endurance training (i.e., running) in PE has the potential to increase students' joy of running and motivation to run as they may feel more in control of the situation and their own performance when being provided with immediate and individualized heart rate feedback.

During regular PE lessons, we asked forty adolescents between 16 and 17 years of age to run for 30 min at their maximum pace that would allow them to run for 30 min without any breaks. Participants were evenly and randomly assigned to either an experimental (EG) or a control group (CG). All participants wore heart rate monitors while running and were asked to report their perceived level of exertion and on whether they enjoyed running after the 30-min run. The groups only differed in the information provided about heart rate monitoring before running and the feedback given during running. Participants in the CG were just told that the heart rate monitor measures and records their hearts' bpm during the 30-min run. Participants in the EG were given additional information on what that means (i.e., information on how hard they push themselves while exercising) and how the heart rate feedback can be used during running (i.e., information on how to run in an optimal healthy heart rate zone to finish the 30-min run without breaks and to get better at running). Moreover, during running participants in the EG received real-time heart rate feedback (i.e., heart rate, percentage of their maximum heart rate and heart rate zone) displayed on a projector screen visible from all sides of the gym.

We hypothesized that participants in the EG enjoy running more than participants in the CG, and (willingly) push themselves harder while exercising. Based on previous work, the individualized activity feedback (i.e., based on each participant's level of exercise) in the EG should help participants to feel more competent (Ntoumanis, 2001; Alderman et al., 2006). Moreover, the possibility to actively adjust their running speed based on the heart rate feedback at all times (i.e., they had control over their running speed and exhaustion) should support the need for autonomy (i.e., reinforcing behavior by self-evaluation; self-endorsed adjustments to achieve goals; controllability of the situation) in the EG. Consequently, the satisfaction of the basic psychological needs for competence and autonomy by providing real-time, individualized heart rate feedback should result in an increase in autonomous motivation toward running (i.e., enjoy running) (Ntoumanis, 2001; Standage et al., 2005; Gråstén et al., 2012; Bice et al., 2016; Nation-Grainger, 2017).

Methods

Participants

Forty healthy male adolescents (age range = 16–17 years, mean age = 16.3 ± 0.5 years) from a 10th grade volunteered in this study, and were randomly allocated to an experimental group (EG) and a control group (CG) before any testing. On average, participants in EG and CG engaged in 4.6 (± 3.5) and 3.5 (± 2.6) h of recreational physical activity per week respectively, indicating that the sport engagement was similar between both groups (p = 0.30). As body-mass index (BMI) was higher in the CG (22.9 ± 4.5) than in the EG (19.5 ± 2.7) (p = 0.01), BMI was used as a co-variate in all analyses of exertion in the present study. All participants were free from any known musculoskeletal, neuromuscular, neurological, and/or mental disorders that may have had an impact on test performance, and had normal or corrected to normal vision. The study was approved by the local institutional review board and school authorities, and conformed to the declaration of Helsinki. Prior to participation, written informed consent was obtained from the parents of all participants.

Procedure

All participants completed two 30-min runs (separated by 1 week) in groups of 10 within their respective groups (i.e., EG and CG) during regular physical education lessons in a standard gym: The first running session was used for familiarization (i.e., wearing the heart rate monitor during running) and to determine the target heart rate zone for the experimental group. The second running session was used to test for the effect of heart rate feedback on (perceived) exertion and motivation by providing real time heart rate feedback in the experimental group, but not in the control group. All sessions were run by an instructor who was asked to limit interaction with participants to a minimum.

Before any running, participants were asked (1) to report on whether they were looking forward to run or not (i.e., motivation), and (2) to put on a chest strap heart rate monitor (Polar H10). The experimenter always checked the correct position of the heart rate monitor and connected the respective heart rate monitor with the Polar Club App, which allowed real-time data recording and processing. Then, participants were asked to run for 30 min around a marked area (10 m × 20 m rectangle) in the maximum speed that would still allow them to run for 30 min without any breaks. After completing the 30-min run, participants were asked to report their perceived exertion (Borg's scale of perceived exertion), and on whether they enjoyed running (i.e., joy and motivation). EG and CG only differed in the information provided about heart rate monitoring before running and the feedback given during running. While participants in the CG were just told that the heart rate monitor measures and records their heart beats per minute (bpm) during the 30-min run, participants in the EG were given additional information on what that means (i.e., information on how hard they push themselves while exercising) and how the heart rate feedback can be used during running (i.e., information on how to run in an optimal healthy heart rate zone to finish the 30-min run without breaks and to get better at running). Moreover, during running participants in the EG received real-time heart rate feedback (i.e., heart rate in bpm, percentage of their maximum heart rate, and heart rate zone) displayed on a projector screen (120 inches) visible from all sides of the gym. Participants in the EG were instructed to always keep their heart rate between 80 and 90% (162/163 to 183/184 bpm; i.e., vigorous physical activity, American College of Sports Medicine, 2014) of the maximum heart rate (simplified as 220 bpm minus age; Fox et al., 1971), which was indicated by a yellow background color and the respective percentage on the projector screen (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Heart-rate feedback as provided on the projector screen during running. Displayed are the number of the participant in the upper left corner, the real-time heart-rate in beats per minute in the lower left corner under a heart symbol, the respective percentage of their maximum heart-rate emphasized on the right side and the respective heart rate zone indicated by the background color. In the present example all information were provided for 12 participants at a time with participants being identified by their participant number.

Measures

Heart Rate Monitoring

Participants' heart rate data was collected using 10 Polar® H10 heart rate sensors attached to an adjustable chest strap with embedded electrodes to measure the heart's electrical signals. Previous studies provided evidence that chest strap monitors like the Polar H10 are (almost) as accurate as the current gold standard electrocardiography to obtain heart rates in sport settings (cf. Gilgen-Ammann et al., 2019). All data were transferred in real time via Bluetooth (with a maximum signal radius of about 35 m) to a 9.7″ Apple IPad, on which all data was stored and further processed using the Polar® Club App. To be able to match the simultaneously captured heart rate data of all participants within the EG and CG group to single subjects, we created individual accounts for all participants in the Polar® Club App and connected each participant's heart rate monitor with his or her account before any running started. Mean heart rate (in bpm) averaged over 30 min and time (in percent) in the five Polar® heart rate zones above 50% (i.e., 50–60% indicates very light, 60–70% light, 70–80% moderate, 80–90% intense, and 90–100% maximum intensity/exertion) were used as measures of physical exertion (i.e., how hard they push themselves during running). Following the guidelines of the American College of Sports Medicine (American College of Sports Medicine, 2014), moderate physical activity is associated with heart rates above 64% of maximum heart rate and vigorous physical activity with heart rates above 76% of maximum heart rate.

Perceived Exertion

Perceived exertion was assessed using Borg's rating of perceived exertion (RPE; Borg, 1985, 1998), a valid measure of exercise intensity (Chen et al., 2002) ranging from “no exertion” (score of 6) over “light exertion” (score of 11) and “hard exertion” (score of 15) to “maximum exertion” (score of 20). Immediately after running, all participants were handed the Borg's RPE scale showing scores, colors, smileys, and descriptions for each level of exertion (see Supplementary Material), and were asked to report their perceived level of exertion by making a cross at the respective number/description. Following the guidelines of the American College of Sports Medicine (American College of Sports Medicine, 2014), moderate physical activity is associated with RPE's of 12 to 13 and vigorous physical activity with RPE's of 14–17.

Motivation

Participants' motivation was assessed using short questionnaires before and after running. Before running, participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point Likert scale whether they are looking forward to the upcoming 30-min run (i.e., agree) or not (i.e., disagree). After running, participants were asked to indicate on the same 5-point Likert scale whether they enjoyed running or not. All possible responses were accompanied by sad (disagree), neutral (neither agree nor disagree), and happy (agree) smileys (see Supplementary Material) to make it easier for the participants to choose from the five possible responses (e.g., based on their own mood with regard to the upcoming or just completed run). The anticipation before running and the joy of running were used as a measure of motivation (Wankel and Kreisel, 1985; cf. Bice et al., 2016). A change in score from before to after the run will be interpreted as a change in motivation due to the running session itself.

After all running was completed, participants were also asked to report on the 5-point Likert scale explained above whether they enjoy running in general and whether they enjoyed running with the heart rate monitor. Responses to the first question were used to control data for participants' general running motivation and responses to the second question were used as direct feedback on whether participants liked the implementation of this technical gadget.

Data Analysis

Preliminary analyses were conducted on all relevant measures to check for normality, sphericity (Mauchly test) and outliers, with no serious violations noted. In order to study the effect of enriched heart rate feedback on participants' exertion we ran separate analyses of variances (ANOVA) for physical (bpm averaged over 30 min) and perceived exertion (Borg's RPE) with group (EG vs. CG) as between-subject factor and controlled for participants' BMI. To compare experimental and control groups' time (in percent) spend in the five Polar® heart rate zones, we ran a group (EG vs. CG) × heart rate zone (1 through 5) ANOVA. Additionally we ran an ANOVA comparing the joy of running (controlled for the anticipation to run) between groups (EG vs. CG) to study the effect of enriched heart rate feedback on participants' motivation. Data are reported as mean (M) and 95% confidence interval of the mean (95% CI), as well as mean difference (MD) along with 95% CI. Partial eta-squared (ηp2) and adjusted Cohens d (due to the rather small sample size) based on Ezekiel's correction formula (Ezekiel, 1930; Ivarsson et al., 2013; Schweizer and Furley, 2016) along with 95% CI of the effect size are reported as measures of effect size. The level of significance was set at p ≤ 0.05. All analyses were performed using SPSS statistical package 25.0.

Results

Physical Exertion

Raw means and standard deviations for all measures of physical and perceived exertion as well as anticipation and joy of running are displayed in Table 1. Data analysis revealed a significant difference between groups, F(1,35) = 15.89, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.31, with a higher physical exertion averaged over the 30 min of running in the EG (M = 170.34 bpm, 95% CI = [163.11, 177.56]) as compared to the CG (M = 148.41 bpm, 95% CI = [140.74, 156.08]) (adjusted Cohen's d = 1.27, 95% CI = [0.54, 2.14]). The difference between EG and CG in physical exertion is shown in Figure 2A.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and descriptive statistics on raw data (means and standard deviation [in parentheses]) of physical and perceived exertion, anticipation to run, and joy of running of participants in the experimental and control groups.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Physical (A) and perceived exertion (B) during the 30 min run in comparison between the experimental (EG, gray bar) and the control group (CG, white bar). Actual physical exertion during running (i.e., averaged over the 30 min run) was measured using heart rate monitors, with higher beats per minute (bpm) indicating higher physical exertion. For perceived exertion students were asked to self-report how hard they had to push during running using Borg's rating of perceived exertion (RPE). Higher RPEs indicate higher exertion. Error bars indicate 95% CI.

Analysis of time (in percent) spend in the five Polar® heart rate zones revealed a group × heart rate zone interaction, F(4,140) = 25.51, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.42. Post-hoc analysis revealed that participants of the EG spend more time in heart zone 4 (MD = 69.29%, 95% CI = [52.72, 85.85], p = 0.001), and less time in heart rate zone 2 (MD = −36.08%, 95% CI = [−52.66, −19.50], p < 0.001) and heart rate zone 3 (MD = −21.88%, 95% CI = [−40.41, −3.35], p = 0.02) than participants of the CG. The differences between EG and CG for the time spent in the five Polar® heart rate zones are presented in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Time (in percent) students of the experimental (EG, gray bars) and control group (CG, white bars) were running at very light (50–60% of maximum heart rate), light (60–70%), moderate (70–80%), vigorous (80–90%), and maximum intensities (90–100%) during the 30 min run. Error bars indicate 95% CI.

Perceived Exertion

Data analysis revealed a significant difference between groups, F(1,35) = 5.77, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.14, with a higher RPE in the EG (M = 13.22, 95% CI = [11.78, 14.66]) as compared to the CG (M = 10.59, 95% CI = [9.06, 12.12]) (adjusted Cohen's d = 0.74, 95% CI = [0.06, 1.50]). Based on these means and 95% CIs of the mean, on average participants of the EG engaged in moderate to vigorous physical activity, while participants of the CG engaged in light to moderate physical activity (American College of Sports Medicine, 2014). The difference between groups in perceived exertion is shown in Figure 2B. Correlation analysis revealed that perceived exertion was positively related to physical exertion (r = 0.53, p = 0.001).

Motivation

Analysis of participants' motivation revealed a significant difference between groups, F(1,36) = 5.26, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.13. Data analysis revealed that participants in the EG (M = 3.20, 95% CI = [2.85, 3.56]) enjoyed running more than participants in the CG (M = 2.63, 95% CI = [2.27, 2.99]) (adjusted Cohen's d = 0.69, 95% CI = [0.03, 1.43]). Anticipation to run (i.e., motivation before any running) did not differ between groups (MD = −0.01, 95% CI = [−0.69, 0.67]; p = 0.98). The difference in motivation between the two groups is shown in Figure 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Self-report of how much students enjoyed the 30 min run in the experimental (EG, gray bar) and control groups (CG, white bar) with higher scores indicating greater enjoyment. Error bars indicate 95% CI. The dashed line indicates the average of participants' anticipation to run before any running took place (M = 2.21).

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to examine whether the use of real-time, individualized heart rate feedback during PE has the potential to increase students' joy of running, which would be indicative of an increased autonomous motivation toward running. To achieve this, we had forty adolescents run for 30 min either with (EG) or without real-time, individualized heart rate feedback during PE and compared physical and perceived exertion as well as joy of running between the two groups. Our data revealed that participants in the EG enjoyed running more than participants in the CG despite a higher physical and perceived exertion.

In detail, we found that the level of physical exertion during the 30 min run was higher in the EG than in the CG. More specifically, students that were able to use the individualized, real-time heart rate feedback (EG) engaged in vigorous physical activity (163–178 bpm) at an average of about 83% of their maximum heart rate, while students without access to real-time heart rate feedback (CG) engaged in moderate physical activity (141–156 bpm) at an average of about 73% of their maximum heart rate. Participants in the CG spend more time in heart rate zones associated to light (37.4% of the running time) or moderate intensities (35.1% of the running time) as opposed to participants in the EG who spend most of their 30 min run in heart rate zone 4 (79.5% of the running time) which is associated to vigorous physical activity. Thus, it appears that the use of individualized, real-time heart rate feedback helped students to push themselves harder and run at higher intensities over the course of the 30 min run (i.e., vigorous instead of light to moderate physical activity).

Of particular importance, our data suggests that students tend to (willingly) underperform (i.e., to run at light to moderate intensities) if not being provided with real-time, individualized feedback of their running performance. Ratings of perceived exertion indicated that the lower intensities in the CG are rather an intentional choice of the students (e.g., a lack of motivation) than a perceived limitation of own resources (i.e., physical capacities) as these students seem to be well aware of the low intensities. Participants in the CG rated their perceived level of exertion after the run as very light to light, while participants in the EG rated it as somewhat hard to hard. Ratings of perceived exertion thus indicate that adolescents in our study were not willing to push themselves hard while running (i.e., lack of motivation), unless they received real-time, individualized heart rate feedback which apparently gave them a reason to push themselves harder (i.e., being motivated due to feedback of goal attainment). This is in line with research indicating that (digital) individualized feedback of performance, learning and goal attainment encourages students to put more effort into improving themselves and to keep them engaged with an activity (Alderman et al., 2006; Bice et al., 2016; Nation-Grainger, 2017). For example, Alderman et al. (2006) argued that (individualized) feedback of performance and learning (as opposed to outcome feedback) encourages students to test different solutions during skill acquisition (i.e., trial and error learning) and to put more effort into improving the skill. Bice et al. (2016) found that real-time, individualized activity tracking significantly increased participants' motivation toward physical activity. They argued that feedback and individual goal setting keeps participants engaged and motivated (i.e., provides a reason for them to perform).

Most important, participants in the EG enjoyed running more than those in the CG despite higher levels of physical and perceived exertion. Anticipation to run did not differ between both groups; on average neither group did really look forward to run. While that didn't change in the CG over the course of running, the EG reported a higher joy of running after the run as compared to their initial anticipation to run before the 30 min run. That means, running with real-time, individualized heart rate feedback apparently increased participants' motivation to run and to enjoy running at higher levels of exertion, which is in line with findings on the relation between physical activity and positive affect (Cameron et al., 2018; Ludwig and Rauch, 2018). In detail, students' may have experienced positive affects as they were in control of the situation (real-time adjustments possible) and felt competent (individualized feedback), which led to an increase in their effort to achieve their goals.

That said, the most intriguing finding of the present study was that students provided with real-time, individualized heart rate feedback enjoyed running more than those without such feedback, despite the fact that they pushed themselves harder during the 30-min run (as evidenced by significantly higher levels of physical and perceived exertion in the EG). Thus, real-time, individualized heart rate feedback apparently enhanced students' motivation toward running (i.e., toward a more internal form of regulation). Based on SDT (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Ryan and Deci, 2000, 2007, 2017), autonomous forms of motivation can be achieved by satisfaction of the three psychological needs competence, autonomy and relatedness. With regard to motivation toward PE and physical activity, especially the needs of competence and autonomy have been shown to be positively associated to autonomous forms of motivation (Standage et al., 2003, 2006; Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage and Gillison, 2007; Hashim et al., 2008; Taylor et al., 2010; Gråstén et al., 2012; Ryan and Deci, 2017). In a study by Ntoumanis (2001), students reported that individualized feedback made PE more interesting, they enjoyed it more and they wanted to improve more. Kalaja et al. (2009) found that adjusting task demands during PE to individual levels of exercise highly contributes to the feeling of perceived competence. Similarly, Alderman et al. (2006) argued that (individualized) feedback of performance and learning encourages students to put more effort into improving the skill as they feel more in control of their own performance. In that regard, it is very likely that the individualized activity feedback (i.e., based on each participant's level of exercise) in the EG helped participants to feel more competent (Ntoumanis, 2001; Alderman et al., 2006; Bice et al., 2016) by being able to effectively adjust own behavior (i.e., running speed) to achieve an individualized (i.e., realistic) goal. Moreover, the possibility to actively adjust running speed based on self-monitoring of heart rates at all times (i.e., having control over their own performance in terms of effort, exhaustion, relaxation and goal attainment) probably supported the need for autonomy in the EG. Although relatedness appears not as important as autonomy and competence for enhancing and maintaining autonomous forms of motivation (Deci and Ryan, 2000; Standage and Gillison, 2007), the immediate visibility (and possibility to compare) of one's performance to others could have added an element of competition to the urge to achieve the predefined goal (for the group), i.e., supporting externally driven forms of regulation. That means, the “group pressure” (i.e., a kind of relatedness) might have helped those participants that could not build up any internal desire to run (i.e., students with low autonomous motivation toward PE).

There are some limitations to the current study, which may inform future directions in this line of research. First, we only tested adolescents of 16–17 years of age, an age group already known for a low motivation toward PE (Ntoumanis, 2001; Ntoumanis et al., 2004) and physical activity in general (Knuth and Hallal, 2009; Hallal et al., 2012; Van Hecke et al., 2016). While from a practical perspective this is surely the age group that needs motivation enhancing methods in PE the most, it is also the group for which largest motivation and performance changes can be expected as there is probably more room for improvements than in any other age group. In that regard, it might be worthwhile to study other age groups and see how the positive effects of immediate activity feedback change with increasing age and different forms of motivation toward PE and physical activity in general. Second, in order to be able to easily implement heart rate monitoring and feedback in regular PE classes a simplified approach to determine the individual maximum heart rate (220 bpm minus age; Fox et al., 1971) was chosen in the present study. While this makes sense from a practical point of view (i.e., feasibility in the school-setting), it does not sufficiently consider individual differences in maximum heart rate that have been found to greatly depend on individual physiology and environmental factors (Zhu et al., 2010; cf. Sarzynski et al., 2013). However, with the goal in mind to implement heart rate monitoring and feedback in the school-setting, it is probably the best compromise to start with the Fox formula (Fox et al., 1971), as it appears good enough to have a positive impact on students motivation toward PE, and adjust students' maximum heart rate measures based on experience when required (e.g., when observing marked differences between estimates and indicators of effort and exhaustion). However, given the nature of the measures (also regarding self-report data on anticipation and joy of running), the present results should be understood in context and interpreted with caution. Third, it's almost impossible to tease apart the social component of the intervention from the heart rate feedback. By nature of the intervention, there was more interaction with the instructor in the EG than in the CG even though the instructor was asked to limit the interaction to the necessary information. This may indeed have facilitated the hypothesized response (i.e., increase in autonomy and physical effort) in the EG. However, even if immediate heart rate feedback only helped increase students' motivation toward PE due to the increased social interaction, its implementation in regular PE settings would still be beneficial and desired. Finally, participants in the present study were only confronted with heart rate monitoring and feedback on a single occasion. That means that based on the present data it is impossible to draw meaningful conclusions on whether the use of individualized, real-time heart rate feedback during PE classes affects students' motivation toward running, PE or physical activity in general, or in a long term. It could well be that the positive effects wear off the longer the method is used or that the positive effects for running during PE do not generalize to other forms of physical activity during PE or in general. McManus et al. (2008) for example found that heart rate feedback led to modest increases in daily time children spent above 140 bpm and percentage of time spent being vigorously active, however, these changes in behavior were not lasting when feedback was removed. In that regard, future studies should look into the long term and transfer effects of autonomy and competence supportive methods during PE, and real-time, individualized heart rate feedback in particular. Moreover, more studies are needed that inform practitioners (i.e., PE teachers, coaches, or instructors) about how such methods are applied in specific situations to guarantee maximum gain. For example, while it appears conclusive that introducing real-time, individualized activity feedback for the first time has immediate positive effects on motivation, future studies should tackle the question on how, when or how often such methods should be implemented to preserve or enlarge these positive effects.

Limitations notwithstanding, findings of the present study demonstrate that the use of immediate, individualized activity feedback during endurance training (i.e., running) in PE has the potential to increase students' joy of running (i.e., motivation to run) and effort during PE. This is of great importance since enjoyment of PE has been found to have a positive impact on children's extracurricular physical activity (Dishman et al., 2005; Cairney et al., 2007; Cox et al., 2008). That means, understanding the benefits and experiencing the satisfaction of physical activity will ultimately help children to develop an autonomous (i.e., intrinsic) motivation toward physical activity in general.

In that regard, real-time, individualized heart rate feedback or biofeedback in general (i.e., direct or indirect feedback of a physiological process given without interrupting the exercise) should be implemented in regular PE classes systematically and repeatedly (whenever required to increase autonomous forms of motivation toward physical activity during PE) to create a controllable (autonomy) and attainable (competence) situation that allows all students to actively adjust (self-endorsed) their own behavior to achieve appealing and realistic goals. At the very least, the visibility of own (under)performance to self and others should help students to push harder while exercising. As especially heart rate sensors are becoming smaller, better, more reliable, and cheaper almost on a daily basis, and they are easy to use even in larger groups in the school setting, it is probably one of the technical gadgets that has the potential to provide for sustainable improvements of PE classes and become a regular measuring and feedback tool in the majority of PE classes worldwide in the future.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

Ethical review and approval was not required for the study on human participants in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

TS and RG developed the study protocol. RG was responsible for data collection. Both authors equally contributed to the analysis and interpretation of the results, and to drafting the article. Both authors approved the final version.

Funding

We acknowledge financial support by the German Research Foundation and the University of Rostock within the funding program Open Access Publishing.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Polar Deutschland for providing the Polar H10 chest strap heart rate monitors and access to the Polar Club App used in this study free of charge.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651065/full#supplementary-material

Abbreviations

MPA, moderate physical activity; VPA, vigorous physical activity; bpm, beats per minute; PE, physical education; SDT, self-determination theory; EG, experimental group; CG, control group; BMI, body-mass index; RPE, rating of perceived exertion.

References

Alderman, B. L., Beighle, A., and Pangrazi, R. P. (2006). Enhancing motivation in physical education. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Dance 77, 41–51. doi: 10.1080/07303084.2006.10597828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American College of Sports Medicine (2014). ACSM's Guidelines for Exercise Testing and Prescription. Baltimore, MD: Wolters Kluwer/Lippincott Williams and Wilkins Health.

Google Scholar

Andersen, L. B., Harro, M., Sardinha, L. B., Froberg, K., Ekelund, U., Brage, S., et al. (2006). Physical activity and clustered cardiovascular risk in children: a cross-sectional study (The European Youth Heart Study). Lancet 368, 299–304. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69075-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, N., Welsman, J., and Kirby, B. (2000). Longitudinal changes in 11-13-year-olds' physical activity. Acta Paediatr. 89, 775–780. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2000.tb00384.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Armstrong, N., and Welsman, J. R. (2006). The physical activity patterns of European youth with reference to methods of assessment. Sports Med. 36, 1067–1086. doi: 10.2165/00007256-200636120-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bice, M. R., Ball, J. W., and McClaran, S. (2016). Technology and physical activity motivation. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 14, 295–304. doi: 10.1080/1612197X.2015.1025811

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biddle, S., and Goudas, M. (1996). Analysis of children's physical activity and its association with adult encouragement and social cognitive variables. J. School Health 66, 75–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.1996.tb07914.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Borg, G. (1985). An Introduction to Borg's RPE-Scale. New York, NY: Movement Publications.

Borg, G. (1998). Borg's Perceived Exertion and Pain Scales. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Google Scholar

Brown, W. H., Pfeiffer, K. A., McIver, K. L., Dowda, M., Pate, R. R., and Almeida, M. J. C. A. (2006). Assessing preschool children's physical activity: the observational system for recording physical activity in children-preschool version. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 77, 167–176. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2006.10599351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burns, R. D., Fu, Y., and Podlog, L. W. (2017). School-based physical activity interventions and physical activity enjoyment: a meta-analysis. Prev. Med. 103, 84–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cairney, J., Hay, J., Mandigo, J., Wade, T., Faught, B. E., and Flouris, A. (2007). Developmental coordination disorder and reported enjoyment of physical education in children. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 13, 81–98. doi: 10.1177/1356336X07072678

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cameron, D. S., Bertenshaw, E. J., and Sheeran, P. (2018). Positive affect and physical activity: testing effects on goal setting, activation, prioritisation, and attainment. Psychol. Health 33, 258–274. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1314477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, M. J., Fan, X., and Moe, S. T. (2002). Criterion-related validity of the Borg ratings of perceived exertion scale in healthy individuals: a meta-analysis. J. Sports Sci. 20, 873–899. doi: 10.1080/026404102320761787

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, A. E., Smith, A. L., and Williams, L. (2008). Change in physical education motivation and physical activity behavior during middle school. J. Adolesc. Health 43, 506–513. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.04.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychol. Inquiry 11, 227–268. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2002). Handbook of Self-Determination Research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.

Google Scholar

Dishman, R. K., Motl, R. W., Saunders, R., Felton, G., Ward, D. S., Dowda, M., et al. (2005). Enjoyment mediates effects of a school-based physical-activity intervention. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 37, 478–487. doi: 10.1249/01.MSS.0000155391.62733.A7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckard, M. L., Kuwabara, H. C., and Van Camp, C. M. (2019). Using heart rate as a physical activity metric. J. Appl. Behav. Anal. 52, 718–732. doi: 10.1002/jaba.581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekelund, U., Tomkinson, G. R., and Armstrong, N. (2011). What proportion of youth are physically active? Measurement issues, levels and recent time trends. Br. J. Sports Med. 45, 859–865. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2011-090190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, L. H., Paluch, R. A., Kalakanis, L. E., Goldfield, G. S., Cerny, F. J., and Roemmich, J. N. (2001). How much activity do youth get? A quantitative review of heart-rate measured activity. Pediatrics 108:e44. doi: 10.1542/peds.108.3.e44

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ezekiel, M. (1930). The sampling variability of linear and curvilinear regressions: a first approximation to the reliability of the results secured by the graphic “Successive Approximation” Method. Ann. Math. Stat. 1, 275–315. doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177733062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrer-Caja, E., and Weiss, M. R. (2000). Predictors of intrinsic motivation among adolescent students in physical education. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 71, 267–279. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2000.10608907

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, K., and Harris, J. (2003). “Promoting physical activity through schools,” in Perspectives on Health and Exercise, eds. J. McKenna and C. Riddoch (Basingstoke: Palgrave-Macmillan), 181–202.

Fox, S., Naughton, J., and Haskell, W. (1971). Physical activity and the prevention of coronary heart disease. Ann. Clin. Res. 3, 404–432.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fröberg, A., Raustorp, A., Pagels, P., Larsson, C., and Boldemann, C. (2017). Levels of physical activity during physical education lessons in Sweden. Acta Paediatr. Int. J. Paediatr. 106, 135–141. doi: 10.1111/apa.13551

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gilgen-Ammann, R., Schweizer, T., and Wyss, T. (2019). RR interval signal quality of a heart rate monitor and an ECG Holter at rest and during exercise. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 119, 1525–1532. doi: 10.1007/s00421-019-04142-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gråstén, A., Jaakkola, T., Liukkonen, J., Watt, A., and Yli-Piipari, S. (2012). Prediction of enjoyment in school physical education. J. Sports Sci. Med. 11, 260–269.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Hagger, M. S., Culverhouse, T., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., and Biddle, S. J. H. (2003). The processes by which perceived autonomy support in physical education promotes leisure-time physical activity intentions and behavior: a trans-contextual model. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 784–795. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hallal, P. C., Andersen, L. B., Bull, F. C., Guthold, R., Haskell, W., Ekelund, U., et al. (2012). Global physical activity levels: Surveillance progress, pitfalls, and prospects. Lancet 380, 247–257. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60646-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hashim, H., Grove, R. J., and Whipp, P. (2008). Validating the youth sport enjoyment construct in high school physical education. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 79, 183–194. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2008.10599482

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hein, V., and Hagger, M. S. (2007). Global self-esteem, goal achievement orientations, and self-determined behavioural regulations in a physical education setting. J. Sports Sci. 25, 149–159. doi: 10.1080/02640410600598315

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ivarsson, A., Andersen, M. B., Johnson, U., and Lindwall, M. (2013). To adjust or not adjust: nonparametric effect sizes, confidence intervals, and real-world meaning. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 14, 97–102. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Janssen, I., and LeBlanc, A. G. (2010). Systematic review of the health benefits of physical activity and fitness in school-aged children and youth. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 7, 40. doi: 10.1186/1479-5868-7-40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kalaja, S., Jaakkola, T., Watt, A., Liukkonen, J., and Ommundsen, Y. (2009). The associations between seventh grade Finnish students' motivational climate, perceived competence, self-determined motivation, and fundamental movement skills. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 15, 315–335. doi: 10.1177/1356336X09364714

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knuth, A. G., and Hallal, P. C. (2009). Temporal trends in physical activity: a systematic review. J. Phys. Act. Health 6, 548–559. doi: 10.1123/jpah.6.5.548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, I. M., Shiroma, E. J., Lobelo, F., Puska, P., Blair, S. N., Katzmarzyk, P. T., et al. (2012). Effect of physical inactivity on major non-communicable diseases worldwide: an analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. Lancet 380, 219–229. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61031-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ludwig, K., and Rauch, W. A. (2018). Associations between physical activity, positive affect, and self-regulation during preschoolers' everyday lives. Ment. Health Phys. Act. 15, 63–70. doi: 10.1016/j.mhpa.2018.07.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McManus, A. M., Masters, R. S. W., Laukkanen, R. M. T., Yu, C. C. W., Sit, C. H. P., and Ling, F. C. M. (2008). Using heart-rate feedback to increase physical activity in children. Prev. Med. 47, 402–408. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2008.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nation-Grainger, S. (2017). 'It's just PE' till 'It felt like a computer game': using technology to improve motivation in physical education. Res. Pap. Educ. 32, 463–480. doi: 10.1080/02671522.2017.1319590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntoumanis, N. (2001). A self-determination approach to the understanding of motivation in physical education. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 71, 225–242. doi: 10.1348/000709901158497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A prospective study of participation in optional school physical education using a self-determination theory framework. J. Educ. Psychol. 97, 444–453. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.97.3.444

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ntoumanis, N., Pensgaard, A. M., Martin, C., and Pipe, K. (2004). An idiographic analysis of amotivation in compulsory school physical education. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 26, 197–214. doi: 10.1123/jsep.26.2.197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parish, L. E., and Treasure, D. C. (2003). Physical activity and situational motivation in physical education: influence of the motivational climate and perceived ability. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 74, 173–182. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2003.10609079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reeve, J., Deci, E. L., and Ryan, R. M. (2004). “Self-determination theory: a dialectical framework for understanding socio-cultural influences on student motivation,” in Big Theories Revisited, eds. S. Van Etten and M. Pressley (Greenwich, CT: Information Age Press), 31–60.

Reiner, M., Niermann, C., Jekauc, D., and Woll, A. (2013). Long-term health benefits of physical activity - A systematic review of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health 13:813. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. Am. Psychol. 55, 68–78. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.68

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2007). “Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in exercise and sport,” in Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport, eds. M. S. Hagger and N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 1–19.

Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2017). Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness. London: Guilford Press.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Sallis, J. F., Prochaska, J. J., and Taylor, W. C. (2000). A review of correlates of physical activity of children and adolescents. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 32, 963–975. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200005000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sarzynski, M. A., Rankinen, T., Earnest, C. P., Leon, A. S., Rao, D. C., Skinner, J. S., et al. (2013). Measured maximal heart rates compared to commonly used age-based prediction equations in the heritage family study. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 25, 695–701. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweizer, G., and Furley, P. (2016). Reproducible research in sport and exercise psychology: the role of sample sizes. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 23, 114–122. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.11.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singh, A., Uijtdewilligen, L., Twisk, J. W. R., Van Mechelen, W., and Chinapaw, M. J. M. (2012). Physical activity and performance at school: a systematic review of the literature including a methodological quality assessment. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 166, 49–55. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spray, C. M., Wang, C. K. J., Biddle, S. J. H., and Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2006). Understanding motivation in sport: An experimental test of achievement goal and self determination theories. Eur. J. Sport Sci. 6, 43–51. doi: 10.1080/17461390500422879

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standage, M., Duda, J. L., and Ntoumanis, N. (2003). A model of contextual motivation in physical education: using constructs from self-determination and achievement goal theories to predict physical activity intentions. J. Educ. Psychol. 95, 97–110. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.95.1.97

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standage, M., Duda, J. L., and Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A test of self-determination theory in school physical education. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 75, 411–433. doi: 10.1348/000709904X22359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standage, M., Duda, J. L., and Ntoumanis, N. (2006). Students' motivational processes and their relationship to teacher ratings in school physical education: a self-determination theory approach. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport 77, 100–110. doi: 10.1080/02701367.2006.10599336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Standage, M., and Gillison, F. (2007). Students' motivational responses toward school physical education and their relationship to general self-esteem and health-related quality of life. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 8, 704–721. doi: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stratton, G. (1996). Children's heart rates during physical education lessons: a review. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 8, 215–233. doi: 10.1123/pes.8.3.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strong, W. B., Malina, R. M., Blimkie, C. J. R., Daniels, S. R., Dishman, R. K., Gutin, B., et al. (2005). Evidence based physical activity for school-age youth. J. Pediatr. 146, 732–737. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.01.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., Standage, M., and Spray, C. M. (2010). Motivational predictors of physical education students' effort, exercise intentions, and leisure-time physical activity: a multilevel linear growth analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 32, 99–120. doi: 10.1123/jsep.32.1.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallerand, R. J. (2007). “A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation for sport and physical activity,” in Intrinsic Motivation and Self-Determination in Exercise and Sport, eds. M. S. Hagger and N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics), 255–279, 356–363.

Google Scholar

Van Hecke, L., Loyen, A., Verloigne, M., van der Ploeg, H. P., Lakerveld, J., Brug, J., et al. (2016). Variation in population levels of physical activity in European children and adolescents according to cross-European studies: a systematic literature review within DEDIPAC. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 13, 70. doi: 10.1186/s12966-016-0396-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wankel, L. M., and Kreisel, P. S. J. (1985). Factors underlying enjoyment of youth sports: sport and age group comparisons. J. Sport Psychol. 7, 51–64. doi: 10.1123/jsp.7.1.51

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

World Health Organization (2010). Global Recommendations on Physical Activity for Health. Geneva: WHO press.

Google Scholar

Zhu, N., Suarez-Lopez, J. R., Sidney, S., Sternfeld, B., Schreiner, P. J., Carnethon, M. R., et al. (2010). Longitudinal examination of age-predicted symptom-limited exercise maximum HR. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 42, 1519–1527. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cf8242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: self-determination theory, heart rate monitoring, motivation, biofeedback, school

Citation: Stöckel T and Grimm R (2021) Psychophysiological Benefits of Real-Time Heart Rate Feedback in Physical Education. Front. Psychol. 12:651065. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.651065

Received: 08 January 2021; Accepted: 16 February 2021;
Published: 16 March 2021.

Edited by:

Carla Maria Chicau Costa Borrego, Polytechnic Institute of Santarém, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Marta Martins, Polytechnic Institute of Santarém, Portugal
Mwarumba Mwavita, Oklahoma State University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Stöckel and Grimm. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tino Stöckel, tino.stoeckel@uni-rostock.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.