Skip to main content
Log in

The situations of culture: humor and the limits of measurability

  • Published:
Theory and Society Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article develops a theory of humor and uses it to assess the attempt to measure meaning-structures in cultural sociology. To understand how humor operates, researchers need to attend to two layers of cultural competencies: general typifications and situation-specific know-how. These cultural competencies are then invoked in ways that define humor as a specific form of experiential frame—the bi-sociation of meaning, its condensation, and resonance with experienced tensions in the social world. I show the usefulness of this theorization through the empirical case of AIDS humor in Malawi, a small country in South-East Africa. Using conversational diaries, everyday interactions, and newspaper cartoons, I argue both that such humor is widespread and that it reveals important facets of life in a country ravaged by the pandemic—what it means for the shadow of AIDS to be ever-present. Through this case, I then turn back to the question of measurement, arguing that although measuring tools may be able to identify large-scale semantic shifts, they necessarily miss forms of interaction such as humor, that are based on allusion, condensation, and what is left unsaid.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Personal communication, 2006.

  2. However, as Lukes and Galnoor (1985) note about political jokes, some tensions fit all too well in a variety of settings

  3. Note that the transposability of cultural schemas is directly connected to bi-sociation. The ability to bi-sociate is predicated on the fact that we constantly move meanings between domains and situations in everyday meaning-making.

  4. As the funnies are published bi-weekly, this means AIDS humor appeared in approximately 7 % of the days.

  5. As before, there are other possible interpretations—switching to a humorous frame mitigates the cruelty of the remark, while allowing it to be said.

  6. This cartoon has the advantage, at least from a research frame of reference, that the joke is explained by the rats. While this makes it perhaps less funny and more “educational,” it does make the cartoon’s interpretation much more straightforward.

  7. Both these options, of course, entail methodological challenges. There may be a danger of over-confidence on the part of the researcher in the assumption that we can become insiders, as well as an assumption of a univocality of meaning (if only one possible interpretation is presented). On the other hand, as this article makes clear, when asked why something is funny, people often cannot explain it, neither to themselves nor to others.

References

  • Abrahams, R. (1962-1963). Ghastly commands: The cruel joke revisited. Midwest Folklore 11(4), 235-246.

  • Alexander, J. C., & Smith, P. (1993). The discourse of American civil society: a new proposal for cultural studies. Theory and Society, 22, 151–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bail, C. (2014). The cultural environment: measuring culture with big data. Theory and Society. doi:10.1007/s11186-014-9216-5.

  • Barley, S. R. (1983). Codes of the dead: the semiotics of funeral work. Urban Life, 12, 3–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind: Collected essays in anthropology, psychiatry, evolution, and epistemology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becker, H. (1982). Culture: a sociological view. Yale Review, 71(4), 513–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, P. L. (1997). Redeeming laughter: The comic dimension of human experience. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bergson, H. (1935 [1899]). Laughter: An essay on the meaning of the comic. London: Macmillan.

  • Billig, M. (2001). Humor and embarrassment: limits of ‘nice-guy’ theories of social life. Theory, Culture and Society, 18(5), 23–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1990). The logic of practice. Cambridge: Polity.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burke, K. (1945). A grammar of motives. New York: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, T. (1999). Jokes: Philosophical thoughts on joking matters. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, S. (1999). Ethics, politics, subjectivity. London: Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davies, C. (1990). Ethnic humour around the world. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davis, M. (1993). What’s so funny? The comic conception of culture and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure, F. (1986 [1916]). Course in general linguistics. New York: Open Court.

  • Douglas, M. (1968). The social control of cognition: some factors in joke perception. Man, 3(3), 361–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fine, G. A. (1984). Humorous interaction and the social construction of meaning: making sense in a jocular vein. Studies in Symbolic Interaction, 4, 83–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fine, G. A., & DeSoucey, M. (2005). Joking cultures: human themes as social regulation in group life. Humor, 18(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1916). Wit and its relation to the unconscious. London: Fisher & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fry, W. F. (1963). Sweet madness: A study of humor. Palo Alto: Pacific Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Giddens, A. (1979). Central problems in social theory: Action, structure and contradiction in social analysis. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis: An essay on the organization of experience. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Government of Malawi. (2004). Malawi demographic and health survey. Zomba: Government Printer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Griswold, W. (1987). The fabrication of meaning: literary interpretation in the United States, Great Britain, and the West Indies. American Journal of Sociology, 92, 1077–1117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joas, H. (1996). The creativity of action. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaler, A. (2003). ‘My girlfriends could fill a Yanu-Yanu bus’: Rural Malawian men’s claims about their own serostatus.”. Demographic Research, Special Collection, 1, 349–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, J. (1999). How emotions work. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koestler, A. (1964). The act of creation. London: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koller, M. R. (1988). Humor and society: Explorations in the sociology of humor. Houston: Cap and Gown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leifer, E. M. (1988). Interaction preludes to role setting: exploratory local action. American Sociological Review, 53(6), 865–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lizardo, O. (2014). Omnivorousness as the bridging of cultural holes: A measurement strategy. Theory and Society. doi:10.1007/s11186-014-9220-9.

  • Lukes, S., & Galnoor, I. (1985). No laughing matter: A collection of political jokes. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, R. A. (2007). The psychology of humor: An integrative approach. Amsterdam: Elsevier Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mische, A., & White, H. C. (1998). Between conversation and situation: public switching dynamics across network domains. Social Research, 65, 295–324.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohr, J. W. & Bogdanov, P. (2013). Topic models: what they are and how they matter. Poetics, 41(6), 545–569.

  • Mohr, J. W., & Duquenne, V. (1997). The duality of culture and practice: poverty relief in New York City, 1888-1917. Theory and Society, 26(2–3), 305–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morreal, J. (1983). Taking laughter seriously. Albany: SUNY Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulkay, M. (1988). On humor. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortner, S. (1973). On Key symbols. American Anthropologist, 5, 1338–1346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poulin, M. J. (2007). Sex, money, and premarital partnerships in southern Malawi. Social Science and Medicine, 65(11), 2383–2393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radcliffe-Brown, A. R. (1940). On joking relationships. Africa, 13, 195–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radcliffe-Brown, A. R. (1949). A further note on joking relationships. Africa, 19, 133–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1967). The phenomenology of the social world. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1973). Collected papers I: The problem of social reality. Hingham: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sewell, W. H., Jr. (1992). A theory of structure: duality, agency, and transformation. American Journal of Sociology, 98(1), 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shifman, L., & Katz, E. (2005). ’Just call me Adonai’: a case study of ethnic humor and immigrant assimilation. American Sociological Review, 70(5), 843–859.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silver, D. (2011). The moodiness of action. Sociological Theory, 29(2), 199–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simmel, G. (1984 [1911]). Georg Simmel: On women, sexuality, and love. Trans. G. Oakes (pp. 133-152). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  • Somers, M. (1992). Narrativity, narrative identity, and social action: rethinking English working-class formation. Social Science History, 16(4), 591–630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavory, I. (2009). The structure of flirtation: on the construction of interactional ambiguity. Studies in Symbolic Interaction, 33, 59–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavory, I., & Eliasoph, N. (2013). Coordinating futures: toward a theory of anticipation. American Journal of Sociology, 118(4), 908–942.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavory, I., & Poulin, M. J. (2012). Sex work and the construction of intimacies: meanings and work pragmatics in rural Malawi. Theory and Society, 41(3), 211–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavory, I., & Swidler, A. (2009). Condom semiotics: meaning and condom use in rural Malawi. American Sociological Review, 74(2), 171–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, V. W. (1969). The ritual process: Structure and anti-structure. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner-Pacifici, R. (2010). Theorizing the restlessness of events. American Journal of Sociology, 115(5), 1351–1386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watkins, S. C., & Swidler, A. (2009). Hearsay ethnography: conversational journals as a method for studying culture in action. Poetics, 37(2), 162–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, H. C. (1992). Identity and control. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Bank. (2005). World development indicators database. (Available online at http://devdata.worldbank.org/external/CPProfile.asp?CCODE0MWI&PTYPE0CP

  • Xu, B. (2012). Grandpa Wen: scene and political performance. Sociological Theory, 30(2), 114–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zijderveld, A. C. (1983). Trend report on the sociology of humor and laughter. Current Sociology, 31, 1–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Nahoko Kameo, Inna Leykin, and the participants of the measuring culture mini-conference—Chris Bail, Amin Ghaziani, Neil Gross, Jenn Lena, Omar Lizardo, Terry McDonnell, Ashley Mears, Ann Mische, John Mohr, Steve Vaisey, and Fred Wherry—for useful comments on earlier drafts. Ann Swidler, Tara McKay, and Susan Watkins talked me through the project in its early stages. I also thank Hrag Balian, Peter Bearman, and Harel Shapira for our attempt to think about humor together.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Iddo Tavory.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tavory, I. The situations of culture: humor and the limits of measurability. Theor Soc 43, 275–289 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11186-014-9222-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11186-014-9222-7

Keywords

Navigation